Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Zénon, Alexandre
Solopchuk, Oleg
and
Pezzulo, Giovanni
2019.
An information-theoretic perspective on the costs of cognition.
Neuropsychologia,
Vol. 123,
Issue. ,
p.
5.
Richter, Fabian
García, Adolfo M.
Rodriguez Arriagada, Nicolas
Yoris, Adrian
Birba, Agustina
Huepe, David
Zimmer, Heinz
Ibáñez, Agustín
and
Sedeño, Lucas
2021.
Behavioral and neurophysiological signatures of interoceptive enhancements following vagus nerve stimulation.
Human Brain Mapping,
Vol. 42,
Issue. 5,
p.
1227.
Lee, Kent M.
Ferreira-Santos, Fernando
and
Satpute, Ajay B.
2021.
Predictive processing models and affective neuroscience.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,
Vol. 131,
Issue. ,
p.
211.
Theriault, Jordan E.
Young, Liane
and
Barrett, Lisa Feldman
2021.
Situating and extending the sense of should.
Physics of Life Reviews,
Vol. 37,
Issue. ,
p.
10.
Ramos-Grille, Irene
Weyant, Jennifer
Wormwood, Jolie B.
Robles, Marta
Vallès, Vicenç
Camprodon, Joan A.
and
Chanes, Lorena
2022.
Predictive processing in depression: Increased prediction error following negative valence contexts and influence of recent mood-congruent yet irrelevant experiences.
Journal of Affective Disorders,
Vol. 311,
Issue. ,
p.
8.
Campos, Carlos
Rocha, Nuno Barbosa
and
Barbosa, Fernando
2023.
Dissociating cognitive and affective empathy across psychopathy dimensions: The role of interoception and alexithymia.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 14,
Issue. ,
Theriault, Jordan E.
Shaffer, Clare
Dienel, Gerald A.
Sander, Christin Y.
Hooker, Jacob M.
Dickerson, Bradford C.
Barrett, Lisa Feldman
and
Quigley, Karen S.
2023.
A functional account of stimulation-based aerobic glycolysis and its role in interpreting BOLD signal intensity increases in neuroimaging experiments.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,
Vol. 153,
Issue. ,
p.
105373.
Ventura-Bort, Carlos
Katsumi, Yuta
Wirkner, Janine
Wendt, Julia
Schwabe, Lars
Hamm, Alfons O.
Dolcos, Florin
and
Weymar, Mathias
2024.
Disentangling emotional source memory: a mega-analysis on the effects of emotion on item-context binding in episodic long-term memory.
Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 15,
Issue. ,
Classical models of information flow in the cerebral cortex consider that primary sensory regions detect the physical properties of the stimuli which are then combined into increasingly complex representations along the hierarchy of perceptual processing. As such, on the one hand perceptual processing is considered to be largely bottom-up, and top-down effects are expected to modulate the processing stream only. On the other hand, the predictive coding framework suggests that the cortical representation of objects is produced largely by top-down feedback to sensory cortices (i.e., predictions about what is being perceived originate in higher-level regions) (Clark Reference Clark2013; Friston Reference Friston2005; Reference Friston2010). In this view, sensory information is not fed forward along the cortex, but, rather, what is communicated along the cortical hierarchy is only the difference between the predicted and actual inputs: the prediction errors. When such a mismatch occurs, the prediction errors are then used at the higher levels of the hierarchy to update the predictive model so as to eliminate prediction errors in the next round of comparisons (Clark Reference Clark2013; Huang & Rao Reference Huang and Rao2011; Rao & Ballard Reference Rao and Ballard1999). Predictions and prediction errors are thought to be instantiated by different neural units, and the balance between the two depends on precision cells that modulate their relative weights. Increased precision of the prediction errors means that the error signal will be strengthened by the precision units and lead to a stronger updating of the predictive model, whereas decreased precision suppresses the prediction errors and, thus, maintains the current model (Barrett & Simmons Reference Barrett and Simmons2015). With this brief introduction in mind, I now turn to how the GANE (glutamate amplifies noradrenergic effects) model may be integrated within a predictive coding approach – a possibility that is acknowledged by Mather et al.
The activity of norepinephrine (NE) neurons has been in the focus of researchers interested in the neural coding of prediction errors (Dayan & Yu Reference Dayan and Yu2006). NE cells respond phasically to unexpected stimuli across sensory modalities and cease to respond after a few repetitions of the stimulus, a pattern of activity that is consistent with what would be expected from units coding prediction errors (Schultz & Dickinson Reference Schultz and Dickinson2000). As detailed by the GANE model, however, the overall effect of NE seems to be more akin to a modulation of the precision weights of prediction errors. Thus, in predictive coding terminology, NE amplifies the stronger feedforward glutamatergic error signals while suppressing weaker prediction errors, leading to a stronger updating of only the most unexpected inputs. Indeed, this is a sensible explanation: strong prediction errors signal highly unexpected sensory input and, thus, elicit orienting responses and concomitant central NE release to boost signal-to-noise ratio and favor the updating of the most relevant predictions.
The salience or priority of stimuli that seem to trigger NE effects, however, is not fully dependent on sensory mismatch. It is true that phasic NE responses occur to intense unexpected sensory inputs (Petersen & Posner Reference Petersen and Posner2012), but also to stimuli that are not physically extreme, namely, stimuli that carry emotional or task-related significance (Schultz & Dickinson Reference Schultz and Dickinson2000). Indeed, the affective/motivational aspect of arousal is something that has not been the focus of the more classic formulations of predictive coding approaches. However, recent models of affective predictive coding extend the predictive coding framework, originally developed to account for perception of external objects, to include interoception, that is, the cortical representation of internal states that constitute the basis of emotional experience (Barrett & Simmons Reference Barrett and Simmons2015; Seth Reference Seth2013). Also, affective predictive coding models do not consider interoceptive inferences as independent from exteroceptive processing, but rather consider that affective predictions and affective prediction errors are basic components of “regular” perception (Barrett & Bar Reference Barrett and Bar2009). This means that the perception of an object involves predictions not only about its physical features (e.g., shape, color), but also about its affective properties (e.g., very pleasant, neutral, scary), and that the prediction errors that are elicited may concern sensory and affective mismatches.
One hypothesis consistent with this view is that engagement of NE neurons in the locus coeruleus may depend on a threshold of the net sum of prediction errors for a given input. This would mean that arousal effects may occur following sensory, affective, or task-related mismatch (depending on whether the stimulus is, respectively, inconsistent with perceptual, interoceptive, or goal-related predictions) or a combination of these. If this combination of prediction errors reaches a given threshold, then a phasic NE response is elicited to facilitate the selective updating of predictions in the prioritized manner that Mather and colleagues elegantly describe. Indeed, it has been reported that emotionally deviant stimuli evoke larger cortical prediction errors than neutral deviants (Vogel et al. Reference Vogel, Shen and Neuhaus2015a), but the precise role of NE in this effect remains an issue for future investigation.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
Fernando Ferreira-Santos is supported by a grant from the BIAL Foundation (242/14).