Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T22:30:30.877Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Importance of amygdala noradrenergic activity and large-scale neural networks in regulating emotional arousal effects on perception and memory1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2017

Benno Roozendaal
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlandsbenno.roozendaal@radboudumc.nl Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6525 EN Nijmegen, The Netherlandslinda.devoogd@donders.ru.nlerno.hermans@donders.ru.nlhttp://www.ru.nl/donders/research/theme-3-plasticity/
Laura Luyten
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlandsbenno.roozendaal@radboudumc.nl Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6525 EN Nijmegen, The Netherlandslinda.devoogd@donders.ru.nlerno.hermans@donders.ru.nlhttp://www.ru.nl/donders/research/theme-3-plasticity/ Center for the Psychology of Learning and Experimental Psychopathology, KU Leuven, 3000 Leuven, Belgiumlaura.luyten@ppw.kuleuven.be
Lycia D. de Voogd
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlandsbenno.roozendaal@radboudumc.nl Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6525 EN Nijmegen, The Netherlandslinda.devoogd@donders.ru.nlerno.hermans@donders.ru.nlhttp://www.ru.nl/donders/research/theme-3-plasticity/
Erno J. Hermans
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlandsbenno.roozendaal@radboudumc.nl Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6525 EN Nijmegen, The Netherlandslinda.devoogd@donders.ru.nlerno.hermans@donders.ru.nlhttp://www.ru.nl/donders/research/theme-3-plasticity/

Abstract

Mather and colleagues postulate that norepinephrine promotes selective processing of emotionally salient information through local “hotspots” where norepinephrine release interacts with glutamatergic activity. However, findings in rodents and humans indicate that norepinephrine is ineffective in modulating mnemonic processes in the absence of a functional amygdala. We therefore argue that emphasis should shift toward modulatory effects of amygdala-driven changes at the network level.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Emotional arousal enhances memory of currently relevant – that is, salient – information, whereas it can impair memory of irrelevant information (Bennion et al. Reference Bennion, Ford, Murray and Kensinger2013; Mather & Sunderland Reference Mather and Sutherland2011). Mather et al. formulate the interesting hypothesis that when norepinephrine (NE) release coincides with high glutamatergic activity within an activated brain region or neuronal ensemble, NE release is increased further, resulting in locally enhanced neuronal activity and better memory. In contrast, when NE release does not coincide with high glutamate levels, NE suppresses neuronal activity, resulting in memory impairment. Although their model incorporates interactions at the systems level, it places strong emphasis on local processes, creating NE “hotspots.” Here, we argue that such primarily local effects underestimate the importance of modulatory influences of the amygdala on encoding and consolidation of information throughout the network and that, without a functioning amygdala, such NE hotspots might be unable to affect local mnemonic processes.

According to the widely accepted “amygdala modulation hypothesis,” basolateral amygdala (BLA) activity enhances memory of emotionally arousing experiences by influencing neural plasticity mechanisms in target regions elsewhere (McGaugh Reference McGaugh2002). In rodents, pharmacologically enhancing or reducing noradrenergic activity within the BLA, that is, mimicking different arousal conditions, is sufficient to alter training-associated neural plasticity in distal brain regions (Beldjoud et al. Reference Beldjoud, Barsegyan and Roozendaal2015; McIntyre et al. Reference McIntyre, Miyashita, Setlow, Marjon, Steward, Guzowski and McGaugh2005) and to determine whether neural representations in these other areas are being strengthened (Roozendaal & McGaugh Reference Roozendaal and McGaugh2011). Recent evidence suggests that such BLA interactions with other brain regions not only modulate the strength of memory, but also are significantly involved in regulating memory precision (Ghosh & Chattarji Reference Ghosh and Chattarji2015), and that NE activity in particular may be the driving force behind improved accuracy (Barsegyan et al. Reference Barsegyan, McGaugh and Roozendaal2014). Human neuroimaging research corroborates these findings by showing that amygdala activity during encoding of emotionally arousing stimuli predicts enhancement of hippocampus-dependent memory (Canli et al. Reference Canli, Zhao, Brewer, Gabrieli and Cahill2000; Hamann et al. Reference Hamann, Ely, Grafton and Kilts1999). ß-Adrenoceptor blockade during encoding abolishes the emotional memory enhancement effect (Cahill et al. Reference Cahill, Prins, Weber and McGaugh1994) and suppresses memory-related amygdala activity (Strange & Dolan Reference Strange and Dolan2004). Amygdala–hippocampal connectivity, furthermore, is stronger for emotionally arousing than for neutral stimuli (Dolcos et al. Reference Dolcos, LaBar and Cabeza2004), and the dominant directionality of this connectivity is indeed from amygdala toward hippocampus (Fastenrath et al. Reference Fastenrath, Coynel, Spalek, Spalek, Milnik, Gschwind, Roozendaal, Papassotiropoulos and de Quervain2014).

Critically, amygdala–NE interactions selectively enhance memory for emotionally arousing as compared with neutral stimuli (e.g., Cahill et al. Reference Cahill, Prins, Weber and McGaugh1994). Mather et al. posit that the amygdala modulation hypothesis explains this selectivity in terms of a trade-off in which resources are shifted toward the emotional stimuli. However, recent findings indicate that there may be more to it than a simple trade-off. For example, Lovitz and Thompson (Reference Lovitz and Thompson2015) report that intra-BLA infusion of the β-adrenoceptor agonist clenbuterol induces a long-term increase in excitability of hippocampal neurons when administered after emotionally arousing inhibitory avoidance training, but that clenbuterol decreases hippocampal excitability in non-trained control animals. These findings strongly support the idea that the impairing effects of amygdala–NE interactions on memory of non-salient/non-arousing information involve an active process that is dependent on the amygdala.

Converging human evidence for this notion comes from patients with damage to the amygdala. For example, patients with Urbach–Wiethe disease (UWD), who exhibit selective calcifications in the BLA (Terburg et al. Reference Terburg, Morgan, Montoya, Hooge, Thornton, Hariri, Stein and van Honk2012), fail to show emotional enhancement of episodic memory (Cahill et al. Reference Cahill, Babinsky, Markowitsch and McGaugh1995). Furthermore, studies in patients with other forms of amygdala pathology revealed a deficit in upregulating processing of emotional stimuli in higher-order visual cortices (Vuilleumier et al. Reference Vuilleumier, Richardson, Armony, Driver and Dolan2004), as well as an impairment in increasing encoding-related hippocampal activity for emotional items (Richardson et al. Reference Richardson, Strange and Dolan2004). Critically, UWD patients also exhibit enhanced memory for neutral information encountered in close temporal proximity to emotionally arousing stimuli (i.e., diminishing the impairment for such information observed in healthy controls [Strange et al. Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003]). One could argue that such findings remain consistent with an interpretation in terms of local hotspots of NE activity if amygdala damage would lead to a general impairment of NE signaling. However, UWD patients, although they fail to acquire conditioned responses, appear to exhibit normal arousal responses, as evidenced by normal skin conductance and startle responses to unconditioned stimuli (Bechara et al. Reference Bechara, Tranel, Damasio, Adolphs, Rockland and Damasio1995; Klumpers et al. Reference Klumpers, Morgan, Terburg, Stein and van Honk2015). Thus, findings from amygdala-lesioned patients agree with work in animals in suggesting that because of BLA damage, NE is ineffective in modulating local memory processes elsewhere in the brain.

Other studies have indicated that stress-related hormones such as glucocorticoids also contribute to selective enhancement of emotional memories. For example, in humans, elevating stress hormone levels after learning generally leads to consolidation benefits for emotionally arousing as compared with neutral information (Abercrombie et al. Reference Abercrombie, Speck and Monticelli2006; Kuhlmann & Wolf Reference Kuhlmann and Wolf2006). Work in rodents has shown that NE activity within the amygdala also crucially determines the modulatory effects of stress hormones on neural plasticity and memory in distal brain regions (Roozendaal et al. Reference Roozendaal, Nguyen, Power and McGaugh1999). The synthetic glucocorticoid dexamethasone, given immediately after inhibitory avoidance training, enhances long-term memory of this training in rats with an intact BLA, but dexamethasone impairs inhibitory avoidance memory if noradrenergic activity in the BLA is blocked with a β-adrenoceptor antagonist (Quirarte et al. Reference Quirarte, Roozendaal and McGaugh1997). Thus, these findings again support a critical role for BLA noradrenergic activity in determining enhancements or impairments of information storage in other brain regions.

In conclusion, local hotspots of NE activity at sites where mnemonic operations take place alone cannot explain the selectivity afforded by amygdala-driven modulatory processes. This observation, of course, raises the question of what mechanism underlies these distant modulatory effects. Important clues have come from functional connectivity studies in humans, showing that modulated regions are part of distinct large-scale neural systems, such as the “salience” and “default mode” networks (Hermans et al. Reference Hermans, van Marle, Ossewaarde, Henckens, Qin, van Kesteren, Schoots, Cousijn, Rijpkema, Oostenveld and Fernandez2011; Reference Hermans, Battaglia, Atsak, de Voogd, Fernández and Roozendaal2014). Novel technologies for electrophysiological recordings and optogenetics in rodents are beginning to make it possible to study such networks in unprecedented spatiotemporal detail. We predict that these developments will ultimately lead to the conclusion that selective processing of arousing material results primarily from amygdala-driven changes in network properties of large-scale neural systems, rather than NE-induced local hotspots of activity.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We acknowledge the financial support from Radboud University Nijmegen Topfund to Benno Roozendaal and the KU Leuven Center for Excellence Grant PF/10/005, and from Research Foundation—Flanders (FWO) Research Grant 1504614N to Laura Luyten.

Footnotes

1.

BR and LL contributed equally to the preparation of this commentary.

References

Abercrombie, H. C., Speck, N. S. & Monticelli, R. M. (2006) Endogenous cortisol elevations are related to memory facilitation only in individuals who are emotionally aroused. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31(2):187–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsegyan, A., McGaugh, J. L. & Roozendaal, B. (2014) Noradrenergic activation of the basolateral amygdala modulates the consolidation of object-in-context recognition memory. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8:Article 160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C. & Damasio, A. R. (1995) Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science 269(5227):1115–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beldjoud, H., Barsegyan, A. & Roozendaal, B. (2015) Noradrenergic activation of the basolateral amygdala enhances object recognition memory and induces chromatin remodeling in the insular cortex. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9:108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennion, K. A., Ford, J. H., Murray, B. D. & Kensinger, E. A. (2013) Oversimplification in the study of emotional memory. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 19(9):953–61.Google Scholar
Cahill, L., Babinsky, R., Markowitsch, H. J. & McGaugh, J. L. (1995) The amygdala and emotional memory. Nature 377(6547):295–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M. & McGaugh, J. L. (1994) Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature 371(6499):702704.Google Scholar
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Brewer, J., Gabrieli, J. D. & Cahill, L. (2000) Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience. The Journal of Neuroscience 20(19):RC99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S. & Cabeza, R. (2004) Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events. Neuron 42(5):855–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fastenrath, M., Coynel, D., Spalek, K., Spalek, K., Milnik, A., Gschwind, L., Roozendaal, B., Papassotiropoulos, A. & de Quervain, J. F. (2014) Dynamic modulation of amygdala–hippocampal connectivity by emotional arousal. The Journal of Neuroscience 34(42):13935–47.Google Scholar
Ghosh, S. & Chattarji, S. (2015) Neuronal encoding of the switch from specific to generalized fear. Nature Neuroscience 18(1):112–20.Google Scholar
Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Grafton, S. T. & Kilts, C. D. (1999) Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nature Neuroscience 2(3):289–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermans, E. J., Battaglia, F. P., Atsak, P., de Voogd, L. D., Fernández, G. & Roozendaal, B. (2014) How the amygdala affects emotional memory by altering brain network properties. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 112:216.Google Scholar
Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J. F., Ossewaarde, L., Henckens, M., Qin, S. Z., van Kesteren, M. T. R., Schoots, V. C., Cousijn, H., Rijpkema, M., Oostenveld, R. & Fernandez, G. (2011) Stress-related noradrenergic activity prompts large-scale neural network reconfiguration. Science 334(6059):1151–53. doi: 10.1126/science.1209603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klumpers, F., Morgan, B., Terburg, D., Stein, D. J. & van Honk, J. (2014) Impaired acquisition of classically conditioned fear-potentiated startle reflexes in humans with focal bilateral basolateral amygdala damage. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 10(9):1161–68.Google Scholar
Kuhlmann, S. & Wolf, O. T. (2006) Arousal and cortisol interact in modulating memory consolidation in healthy young men. Behavioral Neuroscience 120(1):217–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovitz, E. S. & Thompson, L. T. (2015) Memory-enhancing intra-basolateral amygdala clenbuterol infusion reduces post-burst afterhyperpolarizations in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons following inhibitory avoidance learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 119(1):3441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M. & Sutherland, M. R. (2011) Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science 6(2):114–33. doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234.Google Scholar
McGaugh, J. L. (2002) Memory consolidation and the amygdala: A systems perspective. Trends in Neuroscience 25(9):456–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntyre, C. K., Miyashita, T., Setlow, B., Marjon, K. D., Steward, O., Guzowski, J. F. & McGaugh, J. L. (2005) Memory-influencing intra-basolateral amygdala drug infusions modulate expression of Arc protein in the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102(30):10718–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quirarte, G. L., Roozendaal, B. & McGaugh, J. L. (1997) Glucocorticoid enhancement of memory storage involves noradrenergic activation in the basolateral amygdala. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 94(25):14048–53.Google Scholar
Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A. & Dolan, R. J. (2004) Encoding of emotional memories depends on amygdala and hippocampus and their interactions. Nature Neuroscience 7(3):278–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roozendaal, B. & McGaugh, J. L. (2011) Memory modulation. Behavioral Neuroscience 125(6):797824. doi: 10.1037/a0026187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roozendaal, B., Nguyen, B. T., Power, A. E. & McGaugh, J. L. (1999) Basolateral amygdala noradrenergic influence on the memory-enhancing effect of glucocorticoid receptor activation in the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 96(20):11642–47.Google Scholar
Strange, B. A. & Dolan, R. J. (2004) Beta-adrenergic modulation of emotional memory-evoked human amygdala and hippocampal responses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101(31):11454–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strange, B. A., Hurlemann, R. & Dolan, R. J. (2003) An emotion-induced retrograde amnesia in humans is amygdala- and beta-adrenergic-dependent. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100(23):13626–31. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14595032.Google Scholar
Terburg, D., Morgan, B. E., Montoya, E. R., Hooge, I. T., Thornton, H. B., Hariri, A. R. Panksepp, J., Stein, D. J. & van Honk, J. (2012) Hypervigilance for fear after basolateral amygdala damage in humans. Translational Psychiatry 2:e115.Google Scholar
Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J. & Dolan, R. J. (2004) Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience 7(11):1271–78.Google Scholar