Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T16:25:05.080Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dentate gyrus and hilar region revisited

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2017

Conor Houghton*
Affiliation:
Department of Computer Science, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1UB, United Kingdomconor.houghton@bristol.ac.ukhttp://www.cs.bris.ac.uk/home/cscjh/

Abstract

It is suggested that the dentate gyrus and hilar region in the hippocampus perform memory selection and that the selectivity of the gating of memory by this circuit is modulated by the norepinephrine–glutamate loop described by Mather et al.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Mather et al. propose that arousal modulates attention through a norepinephrine–glutamate feedback loop in local circuits. Here, I suggest a specific circuit where this mechanism may be in operation: the granule cell–mossy cell loop in the hippocampus.

It is commonly proposed that the CA3 region of hippocampus forms an auto-associative memory store for short- and medium-term memories (Gardner-Medwin Reference Gardner-Medwin1976; Hopfield Reference Hopfield1982; Levy & Steward Reference Levy and Steward1979; Marr Reference Marr1971; McNaughton & Morris Reference McNaughton and Morris1987; Rolls Reference Rolls, Durbin, Miall and Mitchison1989; Treves & Rolls Reference Treves and Rolls1992). Here, pictures, memories, in the form of patterns of activity in the entorhinal cortex, feed forward along the perforant pathway to CA3, activating a sparse subset of the CA3 pyramidal cells. Plasticity in the synapses of the recurrent network in CA3 and in the perforant pathway synapses onto CA3 neurons fixes the memory so that it can be recalled: If a part of the same pattern of activity occurs in entorhinal cortex, the corresponding part pattern is activated in CA3 and it is then completed by auto-associative dynamics.

Pattern collision, where two similar memories are confused during pattern completion, is a problem in auto-associative networks, particularly if they are required to rapidly store memories with only a small number of presentations. It is likely that the hippocampus has a mechanism to avoid or reduce pattern collision: the hippocampus stores rapidly acquired memories, and it is important that similar but distinct memories can be distinguished during recall.

It has been proposed that the role of the dentate gyrus is to separate patterns and thereby reduce collisions (Gilbert et al. Reference Gilbert, Kesner and Lee2001; Leutgeb et al. Reference Leutgeb, Leutgeb, Moser and Moser2007; McHugh et al. Reference McHugh, Jones, Quinn, Balthasar, Coppari, Elmquist, Lowell, Fanselow, Wilson and Tonegawa2007; O'Reilly & McClelland Reference O'Reilly and McClelland1994; Treves & Rolls Reference Treves and Rolls1992). In addition to CA3 neurons, the perforant pathway connects to the granule layer in dentate gyrus. The granule layer of dentate gyrus is, in turn, connected to CA3 along the mossy fibers. This means that the entorhinal cortex is connected to CA3 directly, along the perforant pathway, and indirectly, via dentate gyrus. In the specific version of dentate gyrus pattern separation proposed by O'Reilly and McClelland (Reference O'Reilly and McClelland1994), there is local k-winner-take all dynamics between cells in dentate gyrus, and the consequence of this is that only a random subset of the cells receiving input from entorhinal cortex become active. This activity is fed forward along the mossy fibers to CA3 and, in turn, excites a random subset of those cells in CA3 that receive input from entorhinal cortex. This randomization separates the patterns that are then learned in the CA3 auto-associative network.

There is experimental evidence (McHugh et al. Reference McHugh, Jones, Quinn, Balthasar, Coppari, Elmquist, Lowell, Fanselow, Wilson and Tonegawa2007) that the dentate gyrus is important for pattern separation and that the adult neurogenesis of dentate gyrus granule cells, which may support the randomization, is linked to pattern separation (Altman Reference Altman1963; Bayer et al. Reference Bayer, Yackel and Puri1982; Clelland et al. Reference Clelland, Choi, Romberg, Clemenson, Fragniere, Tyers, Jessberger, Saksida, Barker, Gage and Bussey2009; Sahay et al. Reference Sahay, Scobie, Hill, O'Carroll, Kheirbek, Burghardt, Fenton, Dranovsky and Hen2011). However, it seems unlikely that pattern separation is the only role of the dentate gyrus; for a start, pattern separation on its own seems a modest role for such a substantial brain region. Beyond this, pattern separation does not explain either the hilar region or the role of norepinephrine in the dentate gyrus.

The hilar region lies between dentate gyrus and CA3. As the mossy fibers run through the hilar region they form en passant connections with the mossy cells (Amaral Reference Amaral1978; Scharfman & Myers 2013). These are large excitatory cells whose proximal dendrites are covered in mossy-looking spines. The mossy cells, in turn, have a substantial backprojection that extends along the longitudinal axis of the dentate gyrus (Amaral & Witter Reference Amaral and Witter1989; Amaral et al. Reference Amaral, Scharfman and Lavenex2007) and connects to both granule cells and inhibitory interneurons (Scharfman Reference Scharfman1994; Reference Scharfman1995).

This two-layer structure seems more elaborate than a simple randomizing k-winner-takes-all network would require; random subselection from a pattern could be achieved by local excitatory–inhibitory dynamics within the dentate gyrus itself. However, the two-layer structure would make sense if the role of the dentate gyrus encompassed memory selection as well as pattern separation. As pointed out by Koch et al. (Koch & Ullman Reference Koch and Ullman1984; Reference Koch, Ullman and Vaina1987; Olshausen et al. Reference Olshausen, Anderson and Van Essen1993), a single layer winner-takes-all network in which competition occurs across the whole network requires considerable interneuronal connectivity. This issue is resolved by having more than one layer; in the first layer, competition is restricted to subregions, and a champion emerges from each subregion to compete in the next layer where the competition between subregions occurs. In short, I suggest here that, in addition to separating patterns, the winner-take-all dynamics in the dentate gyrus also compares the salience of different aspects of its input and that this selection gates and refines the storage of memories in CA3. The role of the hilar region is to facilitate this comparison.

The locus coeruleus projects to the dentate gyrus, which contains β-adrenergic receptors (Berridge & Waterhouse Reference Berridge and Waterhouse2003; Harley Reference Harley2007). Norepinephrine release in response to novelty during exploration enhances excitability in the dentate gyrus (Kitchigina et al. Reference Kitchigina, Vankov, Harley and Sara1997); in fact, the activity of both interneurons (Nitz & McNaughton Reference Nitz and McNaughton2004) and excitatory neurons (Dahl & Winson Reference Dahl and Winson1985; Neuman & Harley Reference Neuman and Harley1983) in dentate gyrus show norepinephrine-promoted increase in response to novelty. Furthermore, it has been reported that in hippocampus, glutamate causes enhanced norepinephrine release (Pittaluga & Raiteri Reference Pittaluga and Raiteri1990; Raiteri et al. Reference Raiteri, Garrone and Pittaluga1992), an effect that is most marked in the dentate gyrus (Andrés et al. Reference Andrés, Bustos and Gysling1993). Conversely, norepinephrine in dentate gyrus, but not in other hippocampal regions, potentiates the release of glutamate (Lynch & Bliss Reference Lynch and Bliss1986). The role of norepinephrine in dentate gyrus seems somewhat mysterious if the role of the dentate gyrus is restricted to pattern separation. However, if, as proposed here, the dentate gyrus also performs memory selection, then the norepinephrine–glutamate mechanism for modulating memory selectivity described by Mather et al. becomes the missing clue that could explain the role of norepinephrine in dentate gyrus.

References

Altman, J. (1963) Autoradiographic investigation of cell proliferation in the brains of rats and cats. The Anatomical Record 145:573–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amaral, D. G. (1978) A Golgi study of cell types in the hilar region of the hippocampus in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology 182:851914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amaral, D. G., Scharfman, H. E. & Lavenex, P. (2007) The dentate gyrus: Fundamental neuroanatomical organization (dentate gyrus for dummies). Progress in Brain Research 163:322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amaral, D. G. & Witter, M. P. (1989) The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: A review of anatomical data. Neuroscience 31:571–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrés, M. E., Bustos, G. & Gysling, K. (1993) Regulation of [3H]norepinephrine release by N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in minislices from the dentate gyrus and the CA1–CA3 area of the rat hippocampus. Biochemical Pharmacology 46:1983–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayer, S. A., Yackel, J. W. & Puri, P. S. (1982) Neurons in the rat dentate gyrus granular layer substantially increase during juvenile and adult life. Science 216:890–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, C. W. & Waterhouse, B. D. (2003) The locus coeruleus–noradrenergic system: Modulation of behavioral state and state-dependent cognitive processes. Brain Research Reviews 42(1):3384. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(03)00143-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clelland, C. D., Choi, M., Romberg, C., Clemenson, G. D. Jr., Fragniere, A., Tyers, P., Jessberger, S., Saksida, L. M., Barker, R. A., Gage, F. H. & Bussey, T. J. (2009) A functional role for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in spatial pattern separation. Science 325: 210–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, D. & Winson, J. (1985) Action of norepinephrine in the dentate gyrus. I. Stimulation of locus coeruleus. Experimental Brain Research 59:491–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner-Medwin, A. (1976) The recall of events through the learning of associations between their parts. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 194:375402.Google ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P. E., Kesner, R. P. & Lee, I. (2001) Dissociating hippocampal subregions: A double dissociation between dentate gyrus and CA1. Hippocampus 11:626–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harley, C. W. (2007) Norepinephrine and the dentate gyrus. Progress in Brain Research 163:299318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopfield, J. J. (1982) Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 79:2554–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitchigina, V., Vankov, A., Harley, C. & Sara, S. J. (1997) Novelty-elicited, noradrenaline-dependent enhancement of excitability in the dentate gyrus. European Journal of Neuroscience 9:4147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koch, C. & Ullman, S. (1984) Selecting one among the many: A simple network implementing shifts in selective visual attention. MIT Artificial Intelligence Laboratory Technical Report Memo 770.Google Scholar
Koch, C. & Ullman, S. (1987) Shifts in selective visual attention: Towards the underlying neural circuitry. In: Matters of intelligence, ed. Vaina, L. M., pp. 115–41. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leutgeb, J. K., Leutgeb, S., Moser, M.-B. & Moser, E. I. (2007) Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science 315:961–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, W. B. & Steward, O. (1979) Synapses as associative memory elements in the hippocampal formation. Brain Research 175:233–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, M. & Bliss, T. (1986) Noradrenaline modulates the release of [14C]glutamate from dentate but not from CA1/CA3 slices of rat hippocampus. Neuropharmacology 25:493–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marr, D. (1971) Simple memory: A theory for archicortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 262:2381.Google ScholarPubMed
McHugh, T. J., Jones, M. W., Quinn, J. J., Balthasar, N., Coppari, R., Elmquist, J. K., Lowell, B. B., Fanselow, M. S., Wilson, M. A. & Tonegawa, S. (2007) Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pattern separation in the hippocampal network. Science 317:9499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNaughton, B. L. & Morris, R. G. M. (1987) Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information storage within a distributed memory system. Trends in Neurosciences 10:408–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neuman, R. & Harley, C. (1983) Long-lasting potentiation of the dentate gyrus population spike by norepinephrine. Brain Research 273:162–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nitz, D. & McNaughton, B. (2004) Differential modulation of CA1 and dentate gyrus interneurons during exploration of novel environments. Journal of Neurophysiology 91:863–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olshausen, B. A., Anderson, C. H. & Van Essen, D. C. (1993) A neurobiological model of visual attention and invariant pattern recognition based on dynamic routing of information. The Journal of Neuroscience 13:4700–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Reilly, R. C. & McClelland, J. L. (1994) Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus 4:661–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pittaluga, A. & Raiteri, M. (1990) Release-enhancing glycine-dependent presynaptic NMDA receptors exist on noradrenergic terminals of hippocampus. European Journal of Pharmacology 191(2):231–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raiteri, M., Garrone, B. & Pittaluga, A. (1992) N-Methyl-D-aspartic acid (NMDA) and non-NMDA receptors regulating hippocampal norepinephrine release. II. Evidence for functional cooperation and for coexistence on the same axon terminal. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 260:238–42.Google ScholarPubMed
Rolls, E. T. (1989) The representation and storage of information in neural networks in the primate cerebral cortex and hippocampus. In: The computing neuron, ed. Durbin, R., Miall, C. & Mitchison, G., pp. 125–59. Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Sahay, A., Scobie, K. N., Hill, A. S., O'Carroll, C. M., Kheirbek, M. A., Burghardt, N. S., Fenton, A. A., Dranovsky, A. & Hen, R. (2011) Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature 472 (7344):466–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scharfman, H. E. (1994) Evidence from simultaneous intracellular recordings in rat hippocampal slices that area CA3 pyramidal cells innervate dentate hilar mossy cells. Journal of Neurophysiology 72:2167–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scharfman, H. E. (1995) Electrophysiological evidence that dentate hilar mossy cells are excitatory and innervate both granule cells and interneurons. Journal of Neurophysiology 74:179–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scharfman, H. E. & Myers, C. E. (2012) Hilar mossy cells of the dentate gyrus: A historical perspective. Frontiers in Neural Circuits 6:106.Google ScholarPubMed
Treves, A. & Rolls, E. T. (1992) Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus 2:189–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar