Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T22:21:21.478Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What BANE can offer GANE: Individual differences in function of hotspot mechanisms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2017

Rebecca M. Todd
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 1Z4becket.todd@psych.ubc.camanaehlers@psych.ubc.cahttp://mclab.psych.ubc.ca/
Mana R. Ehlers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada, V6T 1Z4becket.todd@psych.ubc.camanaehlers@psych.ubc.cahttp://mclab.psych.ubc.ca/
Adam K. Anderson
Affiliation:
College of Human Ecology Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853aka47@cornell.eduhttp://www.human.cornell.edu/bio.cfm?netid=aka47

Abstract

In this commentary we focus on individual differences in proposed mechanisms underlying arousal-based enhancement of prioritized stimuli. We discuss the potential of genotyping studies for examining effects of noradrenergic processes on stimulus prioritization in humans and stress the importance of potential individual differences in the activity of specific receptor subtypes in hotspot processes proposed by the GANE model.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

We believe that the GANE (glutamate amplifies noradrenergic effects) model makes a novel and vital contribution to understanding emotional modulation of attention and memory – specifically via its detailed description of the influence of glutamate on noradrenergic processes mediating the impact of emotional arousal on the fate of neutral items. However, Mather et al. do not address potential individual differences in such mechanisms, which may be linked to mood disorders and addiction.

In our BANE (biased attention via norepinephrine) model, we described effects of the locus coeruleus–norepinephrine system in enhancing attention and memory for stimuli that are salient because of associations with arousal (Markovic et al. Reference Markovic, Anderson and Todd2014). However, a key focus of the BANE model was individual differences in prioritization of affectively salient stimuli (Markovic et al. Reference Markovic, Anderson and Todd2014; Todd et al. Reference Todd, Cunningham, Anderson and Thompson2012). This emphasis on individual differences has been based in part on observations of human carriers of a common deletion variant in the ADRA2b gene, which codes for noradrenergic α2B-autoreceptors (Small et al. Reference Small, Brown, Forbes and Liggett2001). The GANE model makes a valuable contribution in extending beyond the BANE model to incorporate the role of glutamatergic activity in enhancing effects of arousal on processing stimuli that are already high priority. However, the authors are somewhat dismissive of studies examining polymorphisms in genes coding for noradrenergic receptors and, specifically, of the notion that findings concerning the role of ADRA2b can be discussed in relation to the GANE model's hotspot mechanisms. They do so based partly on evidence that α2B-receptors are unlikely to play an important role in GANE hotspots because the inhibitory role of α2B-receptors is not as well established as for α2A-receptors and because α2B-receptors are poorly expressed in key regions mediating affective salience. We argue that the study of genetic influences on affective prioritization of salient stimuli can provide data relevant to some of the GANE model's claims, and that evidence against an inhibitory role of α2B-receptors in key brain regions is not entirely straightforward.

First, we argue that genotyping studies have value, in general, for understanding mechanisms of stimulus prioritization because, along with pharmacologic manipulations (e.g., De Martino et al. Reference De Martino, Strange and Dolan2008; Strange et al. Reference Strange, Hurlemann and Dolan2003), they are among the few vehicles for examining effects of inhibitory versus excitatory noradrenergic processes in humans. Because the specificity of ligands for receptor subtypes is limited (Jasper et al. Reference Jasper, Lesnick, Chang, Yamanishi, Chang, Hsu, Daunt, Bonhaus and Eglen1998), genotyping studies can help specify the role of each subtype in patterns of brain activation and behavior. Of course, we acknowledge that it is important to use other methods, such as positron emission tomography and examination of mRNA activity, to help confirm the role of specific ADRA2a and ADRA2b polymorphisms in α2 activity.

Second, it is important to consider potential individual differences in the activity of specific receptor subtypes in proposed hotspot processes and what the behavioral consequences might be. Genetic differences influencing such receptor function are one source of such differences, and can provide a valuable window into how GANE mechanisms can vary normally and go awry. For example, common variants in genes coding for both ADRA2b and ADRA2a receptors have been associated with neural and behavioral indices of enhanced attention and memory for affectively salient stimuli that characterize affective disorders as well as cognitive biases associated with addictive behaviors (de Quervain et al. Reference de Quervain, Kolassa, Ertl, Onyut, Neuner, Elbert and Papassotiropoulos2007; Havranek et al. Reference Havranek, Hulka, Tasiudi, Eisenegger, Vonmoos, Preller, Mossner, Baumgartner, Seifritz, Grunblattt and Quednow2015; Todd et al. Reference Todd, Müller, Lee, Robertson, Eaton, Freeman, Palombo, Levine and Anderson2013). Using genotyping to infer the role of each receptor subtype on such endophenotypes can help elucidate how patterns of inhibitory/excitatory activity proposed by GANE may contribute to variation in healthy populations and in psychopathology.

Studies of the ADRA2b deletion variant can serve precisely that function. Convergent evidence is highly consistent with the view that ADRA2b deletion carriers have reduced inhibitory autoreceptor function. In vivo, consequences of carrying the ADRA2b deletion variant (found in ~50% of the populations we have studied) are similar to those of the α2-antagonist yohimbine (de Quervain et al. Reference de Quervain, Kolassa, Ertl, Onyut, Neuner, Elbert and Papassotiropoulos2007). This claim is supported by the reliability and robustness of effects of enhanced emotional biases in attention and memory, increased amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal activation for arousing stimuli, and differences in amygdala gray matter volume associated with carrying the deletion variant (de Quervain et al. Reference de Quervain, Kolassa, Ertl, Onyut, Neuner, Elbert and Papassotiropoulos2007; Ehlers et al. Reference Ehlers, Palombo, Mueller, Levine, Anderson and Todd2015; Rasch et al. Reference Rasch, Spalek, Buholzer, Luechinger, Boesiger, Papassotiropoulos and Quervain2009; Todd et al. Reference Todd, Müller, Lee, Robertson, Eaton, Freeman, Palombo, Levine and Anderson2013; Reference Todd, Muller, Palombo, Robertson, Eaton, Freeman, Levine and Anderson2014; Reference Todd, Ehlers, Muller, Robertson, Palombo, Freeman, Levine and Anderson2015). According to the GANE model, affectively salient stimuli are one category of prioritized stimulus whose encoding is enhanced by arousal. Here, the enhanced affective prioritization we have observed in deletion carriers could lead to intensified positive feedback loops at hotspots, although possibly only when stimuli are prioritized because of their pre-existing associations with arousal. Further, because outside of the lab there are likely to be a range of motivationally relevant goals, behavior of deletion carriers may be driven by affective or visual salience over more “top-down” goals relative to non-carriers.

Finally, with regard to the authors' claims that it is α2B-autoreceptors that carry the full burden of inhibitory function in the brain, we suggest that the picture is somewhat more complicated. There is evidence that, in addition to its pre-synaptic inhibitory function, α2A is the most commonly observed postsynaptic receptor in the prefrontal cortex (Arnsten et al. Reference Arnsten, Steere and Hunt1996; U'Prichard et al. Reference U'Prichard, Bechtel, Rouot and Snyder1979). Indeed, some evidence suggests that increased post-synaptic α2A activity in the PFC may be associated with enhanced rather than reduced noradrenergic transmission (Ramos et al. Reference Ramos, Stark, Verduzco, van Dyck and Arnsten2006). Moreover, brain regions mediating heightened emotional sensitivity in deletion carriers show relatively high levels of ADRA2b expression (Hawrylycz et al. Reference Hawrylycz, Lein, Guillozet-Bongaarts, Shen, Ng, Miller, van de Lagemaat, Smith, Ebbert, Riley, Abajian, Beckmann, Bernard, Bertagnolli, Boe, Cartagena, Chakravarty, Chapin, Chong, Dalley, Daly, Dang, Datta, Dee, Dolbeare, Faber, Feng, Fowler, Goldy, Gregor, Haradon, Haynor, Hohmann, Horvath, Howard, Jeromin, Jochim, Kinnunen, Lau, Lazarz, Lee, Lemon, Li, Li, Morris, Overly, Parker, Parry, Reding, Royall, Schulkin, Sequeira, Slaughterbeck, Smith, Sodt, Sunkin, Swanson, Vawter, Williams, Wohnoutka, Zielke, Geschwind, Hof, Smith, Koch, Grant and Jones2012: Allen Human Brain Atlas: http://human.brain-map.org). Animal research points further toward the importance of α2B-receptors in emotional processing (Moriceau & Sullivan Reference Moriceau and Sullivan2004). This challenges the notion of a straightforward role for α2A-receptors as the only mediators of inhibitory activity suggested by the GANE model.

In summary, although we acknowledge that effects of the deletion variant may be mediated by factors other than proposed GANE hotspot mechanisms, the growing body of research on polymorphisms influencing both a α2A- and α2B-receptors poses both questions and challenges for the GANE model.

References

Arnsten, A. F., Steere, J. C. & Hunt, R. D. (1996) The contribution of alpha 2-noradrenergic mechanisms of prefrontal cortical cognitive function: Potential significance for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 53:448–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Martino, B., Strange, B. A. & Dolan, R. J. (2008) Noradrenergic neuromodulation of human attention for emotional and neutral stimuli. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 197:127–36.Google Scholar
de Quervain, D. J., Kolassa, I.-T., Ertl, V., Onyut, P. L., Neuner, F., Elbert, T. & Papassotiropoulos, A. (2007) A deletion variant of the α2b-adrenoceptor is related to emotional memory in Europeans and Africans. Nature Neuroscience 10(9):1137–39.Google Scholar
Ehlers, M. E., Palombo, D. J., Mueller, D., Levine, B., Anderson, A. K. & Todd, R. M. (2015) Grey matter differences are predicted by variation in the ADRA2b gene. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Chicago, IL, USA.Google Scholar
Havranek, M. M., Hulka, L. M., Tasiudi, E., Eisenegger, C., Vonmoos, M., Preller, K. H., Mossner, R., Baumgartner, M. R., Seifritz, E., Grunblattt, E. & Quednow, B. B. (2015) α2A-adrenergic receptor polymorphisms and mRNA expression levels are associated with delay discounting in cocaine users. Addiction Biology. doi: 10.1111/adb.12324.Google Scholar
Hawrylycz, M. J., Lein, E. S., Guillozet-Bongaarts, A. L., Shen, E. H., Ng, L., Miller, J. A., van de Lagemaat, L. N., Smith, K. A., Ebbert, A., Riley, Z. L., Abajian, C., Beckmann, C. F., Bernard, A., Bertagnolli, D., Boe, A. F., Cartagena, P. M., Chakravarty, M. M., Chapin, M., Chong, J., Dalley, R. A., Daly, B. D., Dang, C., Datta, S., Dee, N., Dolbeare, T. A., Faber, V., Feng, D., Fowler, D. R., Goldy, J., Gregor, B. W., Haradon, Z., Haynor, D. R., Hohmann, J. G., Horvath, S., Howard, R. E., Jeromin, A., Jochim, J. M., Kinnunen, M., Lau, C., Lazarz, E. T., Lee, C., Lemon, T. A., Li, L., Li, Y., Morris, J. A., Overly, C. C., Parker, P. D., Parry, S. E., Reding, M., Royall, J. J., Schulkin, J., Sequeira, P. A., Slaughterbeck, C. R., Smith, S. C., Sodt, A. J., Sunkin, S. M., Swanson, B. E., Vawter, M. P., Williams, D., Wohnoutka, P., Zielke, H. R., Geschwind, D. H., Hof, P. R., Smith, S. M., Koch, C., Grant, S. G. & Jones, A. R. (2012) An anatomically comprehensive atlas of the adult human brain transcriptome. Nature 489:391–99.Google Scholar
Jasper, J. R., Lesnick, J. D., Chang, L. K., Yamanishi, S. S., Chang, T. K., Hsu, S. A., Daunt, D. A., Bonhaus, D. W. & Eglen, R. M. (1998) Ligand efficacy and potency at recombinant alpha2 adrenergic receptors: Agonist-mediated [35S]GTPgammaS binding. Biochemistry and Pharmacology 55:1035–43.Google Scholar
Markovic, J., Anderson, A. K. & Todd, R. M. (2014) Tuning to the significant: Neural and genetic processes underlying affective enhancement of visual perception and memory. Behavioural Brain Research 259:229–41.Google Scholar
Moriceau, S. & Sullivan, R. M. (2004) Unique neural circuitry for neonatal olfactory learning. The Journal of Neuroscience 24(5):1182–89. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4578-03.2004.Google Scholar
Ramos, B. P., Stark, D., Verduzco, L., van Dyck, C. H. & Arnsten, A. F. (2006) α2A-adrenoceptor stimulation improves prefrontal cortical regulation of behavior through inhibition of cAMP signaling in aging animals. Learning and Memory 13(6):770–76.Google Scholar
Rasch, B., Spalek, K., Buholzer, S., Luechinger, R., Boesiger, P., Papassotiropoulos, A. & Quervain, D. J. F. d. (2009) A genetic variation of the noradrenergic system is related to differential amygdala activation during encoding of emotional memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(45):19191–96. doi: 10.2307/25593165.Google Scholar
Small, K. M., Brown, K. M., Forbes, S. L. & Liggett, S. B. (2001) Polymorphic deletion of three intracellular acidic residues of the alpha 2B-adrenergic receptor decreases G protein-coupled receptor kinase-mediated phosphorylation and desensitization. The Journal of Biological Chemistry 276:4917–22.Google Scholar
Strange, B. A., Hurlemann, R. & Dolan, R. J. (2003) An emotion-induced retrograde amnesia in humans is amygdala- and beta-adrenergic-dependent. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100(23):13626–31. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14595032.Google Scholar
Todd, R. M., Cunningham, W. A., Anderson, A. K. & Thompson, E. (2012) Affect-biased attention as emotion regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16:365–72.Google Scholar
Todd, R. M., Ehlers, M. R., Muller, D. J., Robertson, A., Palombo, D. J., Freeman, N., Levine, B. & Anderson, A. K. (2015) Neurogenetic variations in norepinephrine availability enhance perceptual vividness. The Journal of Neuroscience 35:6506–16.Google Scholar
Todd, R. M., Müller, D. J., Lee, D. H., Robertson, A., Eaton, T., Freeman, N., Palombo, D. J., Levine, B. & Anderson, A. K. (2013) Genes for emotion-enhanced remembering are linked to enhanced perceiving. Psychological Science 24(11):2244–53. doi: 10.1177/0956797613492423.Google Scholar
Todd, R. M., Muller, D. J., Palombo, D. J., Robertson, A., Eaton, T., Freeman, N., Levine, B. & Anderson, A. K. (2014) Deletion variant in the ADRA2B gene increases coupling between emotional responses at encoding and later retrieval of emotional memories. Neurobiological Learning and Memory 112:222–29.Google Scholar
U'Prichard, D. C., Bechtel, W. D., Rouot, B. M. & Snyder, S. H. (1979) Multiple apparent alpha-noradrenergic receptor binding sites in rat brain: Effect of 6-hydroxydopamine. Molecular Pharmacology 16:4760.Google ScholarPubMed