Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b6zl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T20:45:53.645Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Beyond prejudice to prejudices

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2012

Mark Schaller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. schaller@psych.ubc.cahttp://neuron4.psych.ubc.ca/~schallerlab
Steven L. Neuberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104. steven.neuberg@asu.eduhttp://psychology.clas.asu.edu/neuberg

Abstract

Different groups, because they are perceived to pose different threats, elicit different prejudices. Collective action by disadvantaged groups can amplify the perception of specific threats, with predictable and potentially counterproductive consequences. It is important to carefully consider the threat-based psychology of prejudice(s) before implementing any strategy intended to promote positive social change.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Dixon et al. observe that it is problematic to equate prejudice with antipathy – that doing so limits scientific understanding of prejudice and discrimination and retards efforts to undo inequitable treatment of disadvantaged groups. We agree. They also suggest that discrimination and group inequality may be more effectively reduced by interventions that focus not on prejudice reduction, but on collective action instead. We are less convinced of that.

Prejudice comes in many different forms. Although prejudice is often defined simply as a negative attitude directed toward a group, this definition fails to convey the fact that different groups elicit different kinds of prejudicial attitudes. One study within the United States revealed that prejudices against Mexican Americans and gay men were equally negative, but negative in very different ways. Prejudice against Mexican Americans was characterized substantially by fear, whereas prejudice against gay men was characterized primarily by disgust (Cottrell & Neuberg Reference Cottrell and Neuberg2005). These differences matter. They are associated with different stereotypes, different inferences, and different forms of discrimination that have implications for the fortunes of different disadvantaged groups (e.g., Cottrell et al. Reference Cottrell, Richards and Nichols2010).

Different groups elicit different prejudices because they are perceived to pose different threats. For example, whereas Mexican Americans are perceived to threaten physical safety, gay men are perceived to threaten disease transmission (Cottrell & Neuberg Reference Cottrell and Neuberg2005). Findings such as these follow from an evolutionary perspective on prejudices (Schaller & Neuberg Reference Schaller and Neuberg2012), in which prejudices are conceptualized as highly automatized stimulus-response linkages that, in ancestral environments, facilitated specific behavioral responses to people who appeared to pose specific forms of threat to reproductive fitness. In contemporary circumstances, these prejudices are elicited by the perception of superficial cues that heuristically (and sometimes incorrectly) imply threat. Disgust-based prejudices are elicited by features connoting a person's potential to transmit infectious diseases (or to violate behavioral norms that serve as buffers against disease transmission). For example, because morphological anomalies have been symptomatic of many diseases throughout history, disgust-based prejudices may be elicited not only by people who actually are infectious, but also by people whose facial or bodily appearance is anomalous in any way (Park et al. Reference Park, Schaller and Crandall2007). In contrast, fear-based prejudices are elicited by features connoting a person's potential to commit intentional harm. Because of high rates of intergroup violence in ancestral ecologies, these features may include any trait (e.g., different language, different clothing, different values) suggesting membership in a coalitional outgroup – even if the actual intentions of these outgroup members are benign.

Threat-based prejudices are amplified when people perceive themselves to be vulnerable to the specific form of threat and are inhibited when people feel less vulnerable. Disgust-based prejudices against many disadvantaged groups are reduced when people feel less vulnerable to disease transmission, such as when they have been recently inoculated against seasonal influenza (Huang et al. Reference Huang, Sedlovskaya, Ackerman and Bargh2011). Analogously, fear-based prejudices are reduced when people feel less vulnerable to intergroup hostility. Within the context of actual ethnopolitical conflict, Sri Lankans who were induced to perceive their ethnic in-group to be relatively numerous compared to the out-group (a perception that connotes “safety in numbers”) consequently perceived the out-group to be less hostile and were also more favorable toward peaceful conflict resolution (Schaller & Abeysinghe Reference Schaller and Abeysinghe2006).

These and other findings suggest that interventions focusing on prejudice reduction can be successful. But to be successful, they cannot be informed merely by idealism and hope or by crude conceptualizations that equate prejudice with undifferentiated antipathy. They must be informed by realistic consideration of the distinct nature of specific prejudices and by an awareness of the distinct psychological antecedents that produce these prejudices. Failure to do so may produce intervention strategies that are ineffective or even counterproductive.

The same is true for collective action strategies. Collective action may indeed foster group identification among disadvantaged individuals and facilitate their efforts to challenge the status quo; but these immediate outcomes do not translate neatly to positive social change. When people perceive the actions of others as representing the collective objectives of coalitional outgroups, they tend to appraise such actions as threatening. This distrustful “us versus them” mindset gives rise to fearful prejudices and demonizing stereotypes, which in turn promote coordinated resistance to those objectives and to hostile rather than conciliatory responses (Insko & Schopler Reference Insko, Schopler, Sedikides, Schopler and Insko1998; McDonald et al. Reference McDonald, Navarrete and Van Vugt2012; Schaller & Abeysinghe Reference Schaller and Abeysinghe2006). Can this inflamed battle of wills hasten an end to group inequalities? Perhaps sometimes it can. Many times it will not, however, and may lead to even more desperate and blood-stained inequalities instead.

We suspect that collective action strategies will be most successful if they are perceived not as united actions of a coalitional outgroup, but as coordinated actions of individuals instead. And if disadvantaged individuals are to be perceived as a group, their fortunes are likely to improve more swiftly if they publicly (if not privately) adopt attitudes that minimize the perception of “us versus them” coalitional threat and, thus, reduce the likelihood for hostile resistance. The U.S. civil rights movement was successful not simply because it was a form of collective action. It was successful in part because prominent civil rights leaders such as Martin Luther King Jr. cleverly employed strategies that made the movement less overtly threatening (protesters were actively nonviolent; the protest rhetoric echoed with white majority Christian narratives and values; etc.). Had the movement been defined solely by the more confrontational tactics of Malcolm X or the Black Panthers, the response is likely to have been more fearful and more violently repressive—and the movement's landmark accomplishments are likely to have gone unrealized for yet another generation or more.

It is important to consider the threat-based psychology of prejudice before implementing any strategy intended to change the societal status quo (whether that strategy focuses directly on prejudice reduction, collective action, or something else entirely). To do so, it is necessary to move beyond any simplistic definition of prejudice (singular) and to attend carefully to the many different causes, and consequences, of prejudices (plural).

References

Cottrell, C. A. & Neuberg, S. L. (2005) Different emotional reactions to different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to “prejudice.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88:770–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cottrell, C. A., Richards, D. A. R. & Nichols, A. L. (2010) Predicting policy attitudes from general prejudice versus specific intergroup emotions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46:247–54.Google Scholar
Huang, J. Y., Sedlovskaya, A., Ackerman, J. M. & Bargh, J. A. (2011) Immunizing against prejudice: Effects of disease protection on outgroup attitudes. Psychological Science 22:1550–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Insko, C. A. & Schopler, J. (1998) Differential distrust of groups and individuals. In: Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior, ed. Sedikides, C., Schopler, J. & Insko, C. A., pp. 75107. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McDonald, M. M., Navarrete, C. D. & Van Vugt, M. (2012) Evolution and the psychology of intergroup conflict: The male warrior hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 367:670–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J. H., Schaller, M. & Crandall, C. S. (2007) Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people. Evolution and Human Behavior 28:410–14.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. & Abeysinghe, A. M. N. D. (2006) Geographical frame of reference and dangerous intergroup attitudes: A double-minority study in Sri Lanka. Political Psychology 27:615–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, M. & Neuberg, S. L. (2012) Danger, disease, and the nature of prejudice(s). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 46:154.Google Scholar