Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T16:29:08.948Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Culture and causal inference: The impact of cultural differences on the generalisability of findings from Mendelian randomisation studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 September 2022

Amy Campbell
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UKamy.campbell@bristol.ac.uk School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TU, UKmarcus.munafo@bristol.ac.uk
Marcus R. Munafò
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UKamy.campbell@bristol.ac.uk School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TU, UKmarcus.munafo@bristol.ac.uk
Hannah M. Sallis
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UKamy.campbell@bristol.ac.uk School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TU, UKmarcus.munafo@bristol.ac.uk Centre for Academic Mental Health, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UKhannah.sallis@bristol.ac.uk
Rebecca M. Pearson
Affiliation:
Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK Faculty of Health, Psychology & Social Care, Manchester Metropolitan University, Manchester M15 6GX, UKr.pearson@mmu.ac.uk
Daniel Smith
Affiliation:
MRC Integrative Epidemiology Unit, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UKamy.campbell@bristol.ac.uk Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK.dan.smith@bristol.ac.uk

Abstract

Cultural effects can influence the results of causal genetic analyses, such as Mendelian randomisation, but the potential influences of culture on genotype–phenotype associations are not currently well understood. Different genetic variants could be associated with different phenotypes in different populations, or culture could confound or influence the direction of the association between genotypes and phenotypes in different populations.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Uchiyama and colleagues present a comprehensive overview of how cultural evolution can influence heritability estimates. We expand on this and discuss how cultural differences can influence causal analyses, such as Mendelian randomisation (MR). MR uses genetic variants associated with an exposure as proxies for that exposure when testing exposure–outcome associations (Smith & Ebrahim, Reference Smith and Ebrahim2003). Human genotypes are fixed at conception and, according to Mendel's laws, should be randomly and independently assorted within families. Therefore, in principle and under certain assumptions, MR allows researchers to draw causal conclusions by overcoming some limitations associated with observational epidemiology – in particular, confounding, including reverse causation (Smith & Ebrahim, Reference Smith and Ebrahim2002). However, if the potential effects of culture on MR results are not adequately considered, MR assumptions and the generalisability of findings could be undermined.

MR studies of the relationship between educational attainment and body mass index (BMI) (as a marker of obesity) across high- and low-income countries illustrate this. MR studies using samples from high-income countries have found evidence for a causal effect of lower educational attainment and higher BMI (Sanderson, Davey Smith, Windmeijer, & Bowden, Reference Sanderson, Davey Smith, Windmeijer and Bowden2019). However, there is observational evidence for the opposite association in low-income countries (Cohen, Rai, Rehkopf, & Abrams, Reference Cohen, Rai, Rehkopf and Abrams2013). No MR studies have been conducted in this setting, but it is warranted given the possibility that different causal pathways may operate. Cross-cultural variability could mean that different genetic variants are associated with different phenotypes in different populations, and/or that the causal pathways between genotypes and phenotypes operate through different mechanisms.

Are different genetic variants associated with different phenotypes in different populations?

MR assumes that the genetic instrument used is robustly associated with the exposure. Polygenic risk scores (PRSs) derived from genome-wide association study (GWAS) findings are generally used as genetic instruments. Approximately 80% of existing GWASs have used samples of European ancestry (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Kanai, Kamatani, Okada, Neale and Daly2021) typically drawn from WEIRD (western, educated, industrialised, rich, and democratic) populations (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010). WEIRD populations differ from many other populations, so conclusions may not be generalisable. The predictive power of PRS is reduced in non-European populations (Scutari, Mackay, & Balding, Reference Scutari, Mackay and Balding2016), which could reflect differences in allele frequency and population substructure, or differences in how phenotypes manifest in different populations (e.g., Abdellaoui & Verweij, Reference Abdellaoui and Verweij2021). This limits the generalisability of MR studies using European samples and the potential for using PRS derived from European populations as genetic instruments in studies sampling from other populations.

Is the pathway between the genotype and phenotype partially confounded?

MR also assumes that the association between the genetic instrument and outcome is independent of confounders. However, cultural effects can influence genetic features of populations and introduce confounding through population stratification. For example, educational attainment is influenced by both cultural (Bowles, Gintis, & Groves, Reference Bowles, Gintis and Groves2009) and genetic (Morris, Davies, Hemani, & Davey Smith, Reference Morris, Davies, Hemani and Davey Smith2020) factors and assortative mating may occur based on educational attainment (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Davies, Hemani and Davey Smith2020). This means that individuals with similar educational attainment phenotypes are more likely to produce offspring together. Because of the genetic influence on educational attainment, if assortative mating occurs based on this phenotype a pair of individuals who produce offspring is likely to be more genetically similar than a random pair of individuals. Assortative mating can influence the genetic features of a population, such as allele frequency (Yengo et al., Reference Yengo, Robinson, Keller, Kemper, Yang, Trzaskowski and Visscher2018) and population stratification (Sebro & Risch, Reference Sebro and Risch2012), which can confound the genotype–phenotype association. This is not limited to assortative mating – phenomena such as migration can also influence population genetics through culture (Rogers & Jorde, Reference Rogers and Jorde1987). As culture influences behaviour, and behaviour influences populations' genetic features, it becomes increasingly difficult to make an estimate of a phenotype–genotype association that is not biased by cultural effects. One method often used to reduce this bias is to adjust analyses for the first 10–20 principal components of genetic architecture. However, even after accounting for 100 principal components, bias because of confounding may still be present (Abdellaoui et al., Reference Abdellaoui, Hugh-Jones, Yengo, Kemper, Nivard and Veul2019), although this bias is likely to be small (Morris et al., Reference Morris, Davies, Hemani and Davey Smith2020).

Is the causal pathway between the genotype and phenotype direct?

The final assumption of MR is that the genetic instrument influences the outcome solely via the exposure. If the genotype–phenotype association is mediated by another variable the interpretation of the causal pathway may be complex. For example, the effect of educational attainment and BMI may be opposite in high- versus low-income countries because the effect operates via access to resources, and the impact of access to resources may differ in each setting. Income and educational attainment increase in line with one another and as income increases, so does access to resources (Psacharopoulos & Patrinos, Reference Psacharopoulos and Patrinos2018). In high-income countries, a healthier diet composed of lean meat and fresh fruit and vegetables is generally more costly than an unhealthier diet consisting of processed meats, refined grains, and added sugars and fats. Conversely, in many low-income countries, foods with high levels of added sugars and fats are more costly, and the most affordable foods are the ones with the lowest nutrient density, such as corn (Headey & Alderman, Reference Headey and Alderman2019). In high-income countries, people with lower educational attainment and income are generally priced out of the “healthy” food market, which may result in higher BMI. In low-income countries, lower educational attainment may lead to decreased likelihood of buying foods high in nutrient density, leading to lower BMI. If causal pathways between genotype and phenotype are indirect and differ between populations (as with educational attainment, access to resources and diet in higher- vs. lower-income countries), interpretation of MR results may differ across these contexts and may not be generalisable.

Conclusion

MR represents an exciting opportunity to use genetic data to understand causal relationships between phenotypes. However, when interpreting results from MR studies researchers should carefully consider how cultural contexts can influence these results and their generalisability. We should also endeavour to extend the current evidence beyond samples of European ancestry to understand the full range of human genetic and cultural diversity, and how they interact.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Abdellaoui, A., & Verweij, K. J. (2021). Dissecting polygenic signals from genome-wide association studies on human behaviour. Nature Human Behaviour, 5, 686694. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01110-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abdellaoui, A., Hugh-Jones, D., Yengo, L., Kemper, K. E., Nivard, M. G., Veul, L., & (2019). Genetic correlates of social stratification in Great Britain. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 13321342. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0757-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowles, S., Gintis, H., & Groves, M. O. (2009). Unequal chances: Family background and economic success. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, A. K., Rai, M., Rehkopf, D. H., & Abrams, B. (2013). Educational attainment and obesity: A systematic review. Obesity Review, 14(12), 9891005. doi: 10.1111/obr.12062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Headey, D. D., & Alderman, H. A. (2019). The relative caloric prices of healthy and unhealthy foods differ systematically across income levels and continents. The Journal of Nutrition, 149(11), 20202033. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxz158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A. R., Kanai, M., Kamatani, Y., Okada, Y., Neale, B. M., & Daly, M. J. (2021). Clinical use of current polygenic risk scores may exacerbate health disparities. Nature Genetics, 54, 584591. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0379-xGoogle Scholar
Morris, T. T., Davies, N. M., Hemani, G., & Davey Smith, G. (2020). Population phenomena inflate genetic associations of complex social traits. Science Advances, 6. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aay0328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Psacharopoulos, G., & Patrinos, H. A. (2018). Returns to investment in education: A decennial review of the global literature. Education Economics, 28(5), 445458. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09645292.2018.1484426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, A. R., & Jorde, L. B. (1987). The effect of non-random migration on genetic differences between populations. Annals of Human Genetics, 51(2), 169-176. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-1809.1987.tb01059.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanderson, E., Davey Smith, G., Windmeijer, F., & Bowden, J. (2019). An examination of multivariable Mendelian randomization in the single-sample and two-sample summary data settings. International Journal of Epidemiology, 49(3), 713727. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyaa101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scutari, M., Mackay, I., & Balding, D. (2016). Using genetic distance to infer the accuracy of genomic prediction. PLoS Genetics, 12(9). doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sebro, R., & Risch, N. J. (2012). A brief note on the resemblance between relatives in the presence of population stratification. Heredity, 108, 563568. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.2011.124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, G. D., & Ebrahim, S. (2002). Data dredging, bias, or confounding. British Medical Journal, 326(7378), 14371438. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7378.1437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G. D., & Ebrahim, S. (2003). “Mendelian randomization”: Can genetic epidemiology contribute to understanding environmental determinants of disease? International Journal of Epidemiology, 32(1), 122. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyg070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yengo, L., Robinson, M. R., Keller, M. C., Kemper, K. E., Yang, Y., Trzaskowski, M., … Visscher, P. M. (2018). Imprint of assortative mating on the human genome. Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 948954. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0476-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed