Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T15:04:57.229Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The many geographical layers of culture

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 September 2022

Friedrich M. Götz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, V6T1Z4, Canadafriedrich.goetz@ubc.ca; https://psych.ubc.ca/profile/friedrich-gotz/
Tobias Ebert
Affiliation:
Mannheim Centre for European Social Research, University of Mannheim, 68159 Mannheim, Germanytobias.ebert@uni-mannheim.de; https://www.mzes.uni-mannheim.de/d7/en/profiles/tobias-ebert
Peter J. Rentfrow
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK. pjr39@cam.ac.uk; https://www.psd.psychol.cam.ac.uk

Abstract

Uchiyama et al. present a dual inheritance framework for conceptualizing how behavioural genetics and cultural evolution interact and affect heritability. We posit that to achieve a holistic and nuanced representation of the cultural environment and evolution against which genetic effects should be evaluated, it is imperative to consider the multiple geographic cultural layers impacting individuals and genetic heritability.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

We applaud Uchiyama and colleagues for their thoughtful and important contribution which marks a major step towards a culture-attentive, dynamic understanding of the genetic heritability of psychological and behavioural traits. In our commentary, we argue that an even richer and more comprehensive account of relevant sources of cultural influence can be accomplished by more explicitly considering geographical cultural layers within societies.

Like the authors and others before them (Cohen, Reference Cohen2009; Muthukrishna et al., Reference Muthukrishna, Bell, Henrich, Curtin, Gedranovich, McInerney and Thue2020), we are convinced that few societies are culturally homogeneous and firmly espouse the notion of culture as a construct that exists in many shapes and forms (e.g., religion and social class). Here, we seek to direct attention towards the multiple geographical layers of culture. The national level might be the most obvious and accessible geographical cultural layer. However, to capture the cultural space in which everyday life experiences occur, it could be valuable to consider more granular levels, such as regions (Ebert et al., Reference Ebert, Gebauer, Brenner, Bleidorn, Gosling, Potter and Rentfrow2021; Rentfrow & Jokela, Reference Rentfrow and Jokela2016), cities (Park & Peterson, Reference Park and Peterson2010), or neighbourhoods (Jokela, Bleidorn, Lamb, Gosling, & Rentfrow, Reference Jokela, Bleidorn, Lamb, Gosling and Rentfrow2015).

Thanks to the advent of big data, researchers now have the means to make these previously hidden cultural layers visible (Obschonka, Reference Obschonka2017; Rentfrow, Reference Rentfrow2020). Indeed, under the banner of geographical psychology, ample research has demonstrated pronounced intranational variation along cultural constructs, such as tightness–looseness (Chua, Huang, & Jin, Reference Chua, Huang and Jin2019; Harrington & Gelfand, Reference Harrington and Gelfand2014), collectivism (Talhelm et al., Reference Talhelm, Zhang, Oishi, Shimin, Duan, Lan and Kitayama2014; Vandello & Cohen, Reference Vandello and Cohen1999), and personality (Götz, Ebert, & Rentfrow, Reference Götz, Ebert and Rentfrow2018; Rentfrow, Gosling, & Potter, Reference Rentfrow, Gosling and Potter2008; Rentfrow, Jokela, & Lamb, Reference Rentfrow, Jokela and Lamb2015). Variation in such psychological constructs could affect variation in the frequency and type of cultural innovation that occurs (Harrington & Gelfand, Reference Harrington and Gelfand2014; Lee, Reference Lee2017; Obschonka, Schmitt-Rodermund, Silbereisen, Gosling, & Potter, Reference Obschonka, Schmitt-Rodermund, Silbereisen, Gosling and Potter2013). For example, regions high in cultural looseness and openness have been shown to have higher rates of inventions (Chua et al., Reference Chua, Huang and Jin2019), entrepreneurship (Obschonka et al., Reference Obschonka, Schmitt-Rodermund, Silbereisen, Gosling and Potter2013, Reference Obschonka, Stuetzer, Gosling, Rentfrow, Lamb, Potter and Audretsch2015), creative capital (Jackson, Gelfand, De, & Fox, Reference Jackson, Gelfand, De and Fox2019), and patent production (Fritsch, Obschonka, & Wyrwich, Reference Fritsch, Obschonka and Wyrwich2019; Harrington & Gelfand, Reference Harrington and Gelfand2014).

Importantly, subnational geographical cultural units are not only smaller than countries, but also more culturally nimble. That is, although cultural changes in the country-level typically unfold over decades and often centuries (Grossmann & Varnum, Reference Grossmann and Varnum2015; Inglehart & Baker, Reference Inglehart and Baker2000), regions or cities may experience considerable cultural shifts within shorter periods of time. For example, regional variation in the legalization of same sex marriage in the United States led to swift and substantial state-wide differences in implicit and explicit antigay bias (Ofosu, Chambers, Chen, & Hehman, Reference Ofosu, Chambers, Chen and Hehman2019). Likewise, Götz et al. (Reference Götz, Ebert, Gosling, Obschonka, Potter and Rentfrow2021) showed that changing amenities in cities (measured by housing prices) lead to swift and substantial changes in city-level openness.

To sum up, we readily acknowledge the importance of countries as salient and consequential containers of culture, and of households as the most nuclear cultural entity discussed by Uchiyama and colleagues. Nonetheless, we posit that to achieve a holistic and nuanced representation of the cultural environment and evolution against which genetic effects should be evaluated, it is imperative to consider the multiple geographic cultural layers impacting individuals and genetic heritability.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Chua, R. Y. J., Huang, K. G., & Jin, M. (2019). Mapping cultural tightness and its links to innovation, urbanization, and happiness across 31 provinces in China. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(14), 67206725. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1815723116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, A. B. (2009). Many forms of culture. American Psychologist, 64(3), 194204. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebert, T., Gebauer, J. E., Brenner, T., Bleidorn, W., Gosling, S. D., Potter, J., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2021). Are regional differences in psychological characteristics and their correlates robust? Applying spatial-analysis techniques to examine regional variation in personality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 174569162199832. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691621998326.Google ScholarPubMed
Fritsch, M., Obschonka, M., & Wyrwich, M. (2019). Historical roots of entrepreneurship-facilitating culture and innovation activity: An analysis for German regions. Regional Studies, 53(9), 12961307. https://doi.org/10.1080/00343404.2019.1580357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Götz, F. M., Ebert, T., Gosling, S. D., Obschonka, M., Potter, J., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2021). Local housing market dynamics predict rapid shifts in cultural openness: A 9-year study across 199 cities. American Psychologist, 76(6), 947961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Götz, F. M., Ebert, T., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2018). Regional cultures and the psychological geography of Switzerland: Person–environment–fit in personality predicts subjective wellbeing. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 116. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossmann, I., & Varnum, M. E. W. (2015). Social structure, infectious diseases, disasters, secularism, and cultural change in America. Psychological Science, 26(3), 311324. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614563765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrington, J. R., & Gelfand, M. J. (2014). Tightness-looseness across the 50 United States. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(22), 79907995. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1317937111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inglehart, R., & Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. American Sociological Review, 65(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.2307/2657288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, J. C., Gelfand, M., De, S., & Fox, A. (2019). The loosening of American culture over 200 years is associated with a creativity–order trade-off. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(3), 244250. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0516-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jokela, M., Bleidorn, W., Lamb, M. E., Gosling, S. D., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2015). Geographically varying associations between personality and life satisfaction in the London metropolitan area. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(3), 725730. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1415800112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, N. (2017). Psychology and the geography of innovation. Economic Geography, 93(2), 106130. https://doi.org/10.1080/00130095.2016.1249845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthukrishna, M., Bell, A. V., Henrich, J., Curtin, C. M., Gedranovich, A., McInerney, J., & Thue, B. (2020). Beyond Western, educated, industrial, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) psychology: Measuring and mapping scales of cultural and psychological distance. Psychological Science, 31(6), 678701. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620916782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obschonka, M. (2017). The quest for the entrepreneurial culture: Psychological big data in entrepreneurship research. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 18, 6974. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obschonka, M., Schmitt-Rodermund, E., Silbereisen, R. K., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2013). The regional distribution and correlates of an entrepreneurship-prone personality profile in the United States, Germany, and the United Kingdom: A socioecological perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(1), 104122. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obschonka, M., Stuetzer, M., Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., Lamb, M. E., Potter, J., & Audretsch, D. B. (2015). Entrepreneurial regions: Do macro-psychological cultural characteristics of regions help solve the “Knowledge paradox” of economics? PLoS ONE, 10(6), e0129332. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ofosu, E. K., Chambers, M. K., Chen, J. M., & Hehman, E. (2019). Same-sex marriage legalization associated with reduced implicit and explicit antigay bias. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(18), 88468851. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1806000116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, N., & Peterson, C. (2010). Does it matter where we live? The urban psychology of character strengths. American Psychologist, 65(6), 535547. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rentfrow, P. J. (2020). Geographical psychology. Current Opinion in Psychology, 32, 165170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.09.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rentfrow, P. J., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). A theory of the emergence, persistence, and expression of geographic variation in psychological characteristics. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 339369. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00084.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rentfrow, P. J., & Jokela, M. (2016). Geographical psychology: The spatial organization of psychological phenomena. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(6), 393398. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416658446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rentfrow, P. J., Jokela, M., & Lamb, M. E. (2015). Regional personality differences in Great Britain. PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0122245. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0122245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., Oishi, S., Shimin, C., Duan, D., Lan, X., & Kitayama, S. (2014). Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science, 344(6184), 603608. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1246850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandello, J. A., & Cohen, D. (1999). Patterns of individualism and collectivism across the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 279292. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.2.279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar