Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T15:26:31.107Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

But how does it develop? Adopting a sociocultural lens to the development of intergroup bias among children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2019

Niamh McLoughlin
Affiliation:
Wheelock College of Education and Human Development, Boston University, MA 02215. niamhmc@bu.edukcorriv@bu.eduhttp://www.bu.edu/wheelock/profile/niamh-mcloughlin/https://www.bu.edu/wheelock/profile/kathleen-corriveau/
Kathleen H. Corriveau
Affiliation:
Wheelock College of Education and Human Development, Boston University, MA 02215. niamhmc@bu.edukcorriv@bu.eduhttp://www.bu.edu/wheelock/profile/niamh-mcloughlin/https://www.bu.edu/wheelock/profile/kathleen-corriveau/

Abstract

We argue that adopting a sociocultural lens to the origins of intergroup bias is important for understanding the nature of attacking and defending behavior at a group level. We specifically propose that the potential divergence in the development of in-group affiliation and out-group derogation supports De Dreu and Gross's framework but does indicate that more emphasis on early sociocultural input is required.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Based on theory and empirical findings drawn from the fields of neuroscience, behavioral economics, social, and evolutionary psychology, De Dreu and Gross develop a framework to disentangle the neurobiological, psychological, and cultural characteristics that contribute to the nature of intergroup conflict. More specifically, they propose that potentially distinct psychological and cultural factors could be involved in what they conceptualize as “attacking” versus “defending” behavior at a group level. We agree with the argument that social affiliation, such as identification with and loyalty to in-group members, emerges more spontaneously within defender groups, whereas the formation and behavior of attacking groups may rely more heavily on cultural tools and arrangements (e.g., propaganda, cultural rituals, religious belief). However, research in developmental psychology that adopts a sociocultural lens to examine the origins of intergroup bias is critical to understanding these phenomena.

Children are motivated to affiliate with others from early in development (Carpenter et al. Reference Carpenter, Nagell and Tomasello1998; Over Reference Over2016) and particularly with individuals who belong to their own social group. A substantial number of studies have demonstrated that 4- and 5-year-old children are more likely to selectively befriend (Kinzler et al. Reference Kinzler, Dupoux and Spelke2007), stay loyal to (Misch et al. Reference Misch, Over and Carpenter2016), share more resources with (Renno & Shutts Reference Renno and Shutts2015), and trust the testimony of (Harris & Corriveau Reference Harris and Corriveau2011) in-group members. These social affiliative tendencies are observed in societies in which intergroup tension is relatively low and even when group membership is based on very minimal cues (i.e., shirt color; Dunham et al. Reference Dunham, Baron and Carey2011). De Dreu & Gross posit that the spontaneously coordinated and affiliative behavior witnessed within defense groups could be due, in part, to the earlier emergence of defense behavior in ancient societies. The early appearance of affiliative behavior in development also supports the evolutionary value of belonging to a group (Brewer Reference Brewer1999; Brewer & Caporael Reference Brewer, Caporael, Schaller, Simpson and Kenrick2006). Although the drive to affiliate with socially similar others is considered to be a fairly ubiquitous phenomenon (Meltzoff Reference Meltzoff2007), some researchers have emphasized the role of early sociocultural experiences in the development of in-group positivity. For example, minority race children can exhibit less favoritism for in-group members compared with majority (white) children (Margie et al. Reference Margie, Killen, Sinno and McGlothlin2005; Shutts et al. Reference Shutts, Kinzler, Katz, Tredoux and Spelke2011), and variability in socialization with and exposure to different racial groups can shape young children's social preferences (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Corriveau, Lai, Poon and Gaither2018; Gaither et al. Reference Gaither, Chen, Corriveau, Harris, Ambady and Sommers2014).

Other research in developmental psychology has suggested that negative out-group attitudes appear slightly later in development, typically after the age of 6 (Buttelmann & Böhm Reference Buttelmann and Böhm2014; McLoughlin & Over 2018). A recent body of work has illustrated that older children (ages between 6 and 11 years) can even hold dehumanizing out-group views, including the belief that out-group members possess fewer mental state capabilities (Dore et al. Reference Dore, Hoffman, Lillard and Trawalter2014; McLoughlin & Over Reference McLoughlin and Over2017) and uniquely human characteristics (Chas et al. Reference Chas, Betancor, Delgado and Rodríguez-Pérez2018; Costello & Hodson Reference Costello and Hodson2014). Yet, there is evidence to suggest that the developmental onset and trajectory of harmful out-group perceptions can vary widely, depending on different sociocultural factors, such as the cultural saliency of historical conflict between ethnic groups (Bennett et al. Reference Bennett, Barrett, Karakozov, Kipiani, Lyons, Pavlenko and Riazanova2004; Birnbaum et al. Reference Birnbaum, Deeb, Segall, Ben-Eliyahu and Diesendruck2010; Smyth et al. Reference Smyth, Feeney, Eidson and Coley2017), as well as the shared ideologies of the surrounding community (Rhodes & Mandalaywala Reference Rhodes and Mandalaywala2017; Segall et al. Reference Segall, Birnbaum, Deeb and Diesendruck2015). Taken together, these findings reinforce the importance of cultural learning and communication (Harris et al. Reference Harris, Koenig, Corriveau and Jaswal2018; Over & McCall Reference Over and McCall2018; Skinner & Meltzoff Reference Skinner and Meltzoff2019) and further speak to the relevance of social tools in motivating attacking behavior.

In sum, the potential divergence in the development of affiliation with the in-group and derogation of the out-group – at least in the cultural contexts studied thus far – supports the authors’ proposed framework but does indicate that more emphasis on early sociocultural input is required. One caveat to consider, however, is that it is sometimes not feasible to experimentally manipulate or measure parallel behaviors in research with children (e.g., extreme attacking intentions). Nonetheless, this field can provide valuable insight into the adaptive psychological mechanisms and the social learning processes contributing to intergroup conflict.

References

Bennett, M., Barrett, M., Karakozov, R., Kipiani, G., Lyons, E., Pavlenko, V. & Riazanova, T. (2004) Young children's evaluations of the ingroup and of outgroups: A multi-national study. Social Development 13(1):124–41. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-9507.2004.00260.x.Google Scholar
Birnbaum, D., Deeb, I., Segall, G., Ben-Eliyahu, A. & Diesendruck, G. (2010) The development of social essentialism: The case of Israeli children's inferences about Jews and Arabs. Child Development 81(3):757–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01432.x.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (1999) The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love and outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues 55(3):429–44. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00126.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B. & Caporael, L. R. (2006) An evolutionary perspective on social identity: Revisiting groups. In: Evolution and social psychology, ed. Schaller, M., Simpson, J. & Kenrick, D., pp. 143–61. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D. & Böhm, R. (2014) The ontogeny of the motivation that underlies in-group bias. Psychological Science 25:921–27. doi: 10.1177/0956797613516802.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. & Tomasello, M. (1998) Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 63(4):i–vi, 1143.Google Scholar
Chas, A., Betancor, V., Delgado, N. & Rodríguez-Pérez, A. (2018) Children consider their own groups to be more human than other social groups: Evidence from indirect and direct measures. Social Psychology 49(3):125–34.Google Scholar
Chen, E. E., Corriveau, K. H., Lai, V. K. W., Poon, S. L. & Gaither, S. E. (2018) Learning and Socializing Preferences in Hong Kong Chinese Children. Child Development 89(6):2109–17. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13083.Google Scholar
Costello, K. & Hodson, G. (2014) Explaining dehumanization among children: The interspecies model of prejudice. British Journal of Social Psychology 53(1):175–97. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12016.Google Scholar
Dore, R. A., Hoffman, K. M., Lillard, A. S. & Trawalter, S. (2014) Children's racial bias in perceptions of others' pain. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 32(2):218–31. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12038.Google Scholar
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S. & Carey, S. (2011) Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development 82(3):793811. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01577.x.Google Scholar
Gaither, S. E., Chen, E. E., Corriveau, K. H., Harris, P. L., Ambady, N. & Sommers, S. R. (2014) Monoracial and biracial children: effects of racial identity saliency on social learning and social preferences. Child Development 85(6):2299–316. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12266.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. & Corriveau, K. H. (2011) Young children's selective trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366(1567):1179–87. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0321.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L., Koenig, M. A., Corriveau, K. H. & Jaswal, V. K. (2018) Cognitive Foundations of Learning from Testimony. Annual Review of Psychology 69(1):251–73. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011710.Google Scholar
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E. & Spelke, E. S. (2007) The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104(30):12577–80. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705345104Google Scholar
Margie, N. G., Killen, M., Sinno, S. & McGlothlin, H. (2005) Minority children's intergroup attitudes about peer relationships. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 23(2):251–70. doi: 10.1348/026151005X26075.Google Scholar
McLoughlin, N. & Over, H. (2017) Young children are more likely to spontaneously attribute mental states to members of their own group. Psychological Science 28(10):1503–509. doi: 10.1177/0956797617710724.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (2007) 'Like me': A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science 10(1):126–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00574.x.Google Scholar
Misch, A., Over, H. & Carpenter, M. (2016) I won't tell: Young children show loyalty to their group by keeping group secrets. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 142:96106. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.09.016.Google Scholar
Over, H. (2016) The origins of belonging: Social motivation in infants and young children. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 371:20150072. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0072.Google Scholar
Over, H. & McCall, C. (2018) Becoming us and them: Social learning and intergroup bias. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 12(4):e12384. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12384.Google Scholar
Renno, M. P. & Shutts, K. (2015) Children's social category-based giving and its correlates: expectations and preferences. Developmental Psychology 51(4):533–43. doi: 10.1037/a0038819.Google Scholar
Rhodes, M. & Mandalaywala, T. M. (2017) The development and developmental consequences of social essentialism. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 8(4):1437. doi:10.1002/wcs.1437.Google Scholar
Segall, G., Birnbaum, D., Deeb, I. & Diesendruck, G. (2015) The intergenerational transmission of ethnic essentialism: How parents talk counts the most. Developmental Science 18(4):543–55. doi: 10.1111/desc.12235.Google Scholar
Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., Katz, R. C., Tredoux, C. & Spelke, E. S. (2011) Race preferences in children: Insights from South Africa. Developmental Science 14(6):1283–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01072.x.Google Scholar
Skinner, A. L. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2019) Childhood Experiences and Intergroup Biases among Children. Social Issues and Policy Review 13(1):211–40. doi: 10.1111/sipr.12054.Google Scholar
Smyth, K., Feeney, A., Eidson, R. C. & Coley, J. D. (2017) Development of essentialist thinking about religion categories in Northern Ireland (and the United States). Developmental Psychology 53(3):475–96. doi: 10.1037/dev0000253.Google Scholar