Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T23:03:07.208Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The allure of the unknown in a tamed, mapped, and homogenized world

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 November 2022

Liane Gabora
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology (Okanagan Campus), University of British Columbia, Kelowna, BCV1V 1V7, Canada liane.gabora@ubc.ca; https://people.ok.ubc.ca/lgabora/ mislabel.gomez@ubc.ca
Isabel Gomez
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology (Okanagan Campus), University of British Columbia, Kelowna, BCV1V 1V7, Canada liane.gabora@ubc.ca; https://people.ok.ubc.ca/lgabora/ mislabel.gomez@ubc.ca

Abstract

As the physical world becomes tamed and mapped out, opportunities to experience the unknown become rarer; imaginary worlds provide a much-needed sense of potentiality. Potentiality is central to the Self-Other Re-organization theory of cultural evolution, which postulates that creativity fuels cumulative cultural change. We point to evidence that fear affects, not the magnitude of exploration, but how cautiously it proceeds.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

We find the argument that our species' enjoyment of other worlds is related to the adaptive value of exploratory behavior to be highly plausible. We suggest that the authors' proposal can be extended by incorporating what has been referred to as the adjacent possible (Kauffman, Reference Kauffman2019; Koppl, Kauffman, Felin, & Longo, Reference Koppl, Kauffman, Felin and Longo2015): the realm of what is just beyond reach. Exploratory behavior enables us to dip into the realm of what is “not quite actual” but rather, the halo of potentiality that is hinted at by what is actual. The appeal of imaginary worlds in the twenty-first century may be related to a radical change in our perception of the adjacent possible, given the rate at which our world has been tamed, mapped out, and homogenized. Compared to our ancestors' world, in which you could hop in a boat and have no idea what sort of exotic land you might encounter, our present world does not hold the same kind of mystery and intrigue. Imaginary worlds provide us with what the physical world offered our ancestors: a sense of potentiality, and a key to the unknown.

Research on concepts, and in particular, quantum models of concept interactions (e.g., Aerts, Aerts, & Gabora, Reference Aerts, Aerts, Gabora, Bruza, Lawless, van Rijsbergen and Sofge2009; Aerts, Broekaert, Gabora, & Sozzo, Reference Aerts, Broekaert, Gabora and Sozzo2016), suggests that this sense of potentiality ultimately stems from how rigidly one's concepts (such as SKY) are tied to specific properties (e.g., “blue”) and contexts (e.g., “Earth”). In quantum models of concepts, the state of full potentiality for a concept is referred to as its ground state, and in a particular context (e.g., the context “Mars”), the concept SKY “collapses” to an actualized state (e.g., the sky of Mars), with context-specific properties (e.g., the sky is red). Exposure to imaginary worlds may decrease mental rigidity by pushing concepts closer to their ground state of full potentiality, thereby enhancing the capacity for creative connections. This hypothesis is consistent with evidence that cognitive flexibility plays an important role in creativity (Dreu, Nijstad, & Baas, Reference Dreu, Nijstad and Baas2011). It is also consistent with studies showing that the creative process is conducive to holding the contents of working memory in a state of potentiality such that ambiguities and inconsistencies are tolerated, and different interpretations or affordances are considered, before gradually settling on a particular solution or outcome (Carbert, Gabora, Schwartz, & Ranjan, Reference Carbert, Gabora, Schwartz, Ranjan, Kozbelt, Locher and Tinio2014; Gabora, Reference Gabora1998; Gabora & Saab, Reference Gabora, Saab, Carlson, Hőlscher and Shipley2011; Scotney, Schwartz, Carbert, Adam Saab, & Gabora, Reference Scotney, Schwartz, Carbert, Adam Saab and Gabora2020).

The authors claim that the existence of imaginary worlds is not explainable in terms of existing theories of cultural evolution. We suggest, however, that they are explainable in terms of the Self-Other Re-organization (SOR) theory of cultural evolution (Gabora, Reference Gabora2019; Smith, Gabora, & Gardner-O'Kearny, Reference Smith, Gabora and Gardner-O'Kearny2018; for mathematical models of SOR, see Gabora & Steel, Reference Gabora and Steel2017, Reference Gabora and Steel2020a, Reference Gabora and Steel2020b, Reference Gabora and Steel2021). SOC postulates that culture evolves, not through a Darwinian process of competitive exclusion and survival of the fittest, but through a largely cooperative process of internal self-maintenance and restructuring within individuals, interleaved with social interaction between individuals. Central to SOR is the notion that cultural outputs are the external evidence of internal change brought about by creativity, which transforms potentialities into actualities by tapping into the adjacent possible. SOR would attribute the existence of imaginary worlds to their capacity to enhance cognitive flexibility and provoke restructuring of one's internal model of the world, or worldview, leading to enhanced capacity to adapt to new circumstances, and thereby, accelerated cultural evolution. Support for this hypothesis comes from evidence that diversifying experiences enhance cognitive flexibility (Ritter et al., Reference Ritter, Damian, Simonton, van Baaren, Strick, Derks and Dijksterhuis2012); imaginary worlds are indeed the ultimate diversifying experience.

The authors' suggestion that imaginary worlds are more popular with individuals who are relatively well-off and enjoy a sense of safety (target article, sect. 4.4.2, para. 2) appears to be inconsistent with the notion of adversity as a form of creative capital, and evidence that agency evolves out of threatening or marginalized conditions (Forgeard, Reference Forgeard2013; Riley & Gabora, Reference Riley, Gabora, Miyake, Peebles and Cooper2012; Weston & Imas, Reference Weston, Imas, Martin and Wilson2018). Indeed, this body of research suggests a hypothesis quite contrary to that of the target paper: that those who are less fortunate may have an even greater appreciation of the sense of escape afforded by imaginary worlds. “Gallows humor,” for example, may be evidence of the creativity-inspiring impact of adversity (Watson, Reference Watson2011). Well-off individuals may simply be more aware of the existence of books and media with imaginary worlds, or more able to afford them, or possess more well-developed reading skills, or more free time.

We suggest that the notion that exploratory behavior in animals is inversely related to fear of predation is only partly true. In a study of exploratory behavior in killifish, although killifish from a high predation lake took longer to explore a new space, they would eventually explore it as thoroughly as killifish from a lake without predation (Gabora & Colgan, Reference Gabora, Colgan, Wilson and Mayer1990). Thus, when space explored was plotted as a function of time, the curve for the killifish populations from the high predation lake was wider and flatter, but the total area under the curves was the same for both populations. This suggests that fear does not affect the magnitude of the drive to explore; it merely tempers it, such that exploration proceeds more cautiously, and takes longer.

We note that imaginary worlds do not appeal to everyone, nor does everyone feel compelled to create them. This is consistent with evidence from an agent-based computational model of cultural evolution which showed that the mean fitness and diversity of the artificial society's outputs was highest when there was a balance between novelty-generating innovators and novelty-preserving imitators (Gabora & Tseng, Reference Gabora and Tseng2017). Innovators may be attracted to imaginary worlds because of their impact on mental flexibility (discussed above), whereas those who favor cultural continuity through the perpetuation of existing methods might tend to view imaginary worlds as merely frivolous.

Funding

This work was supported by a grant (62R06523) from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and funding to LG and her lab from Susan and Jacques Leblanc for research on creativity.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Aerts, D., Aerts, S., & Gabora, L. (2009). Experimental evidence for quantum structure in cognition. In Bruza, P., Lawless, W., van Rijsbergen, K. & Sofge, D. (Eds.) Lecture notes in computer science: Quantum interaction (pp. 5979). Springer.Google Scholar
Aerts, D., Broekaert, J., Gabora, L., & Sozzo, S. (2016). Generalizing prototype theory: A formal quantum framework. Frontiers in Psychology (Section: Cognition), 7(418). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00418Google ScholarPubMed
Carbert, N., Gabora, L., Schwartz, J., & Ranjan, A. (2014). Cognitive states of potentiality in art-making. In Kozbelt, A., Locher, P. & Tinio, P. (Eds.), Proceedings of the IAEA congress on empirical aesthetics (pp. 121126). International Association of Empirical Aesthetics Press.Google Scholar
Dreu, C. K. D., Nijstad, B. A., & Baas, M. (2011). Behavioral activation links to creativity because of increased cognitive flexibility. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(1), 7280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgeard, M. (2013). Perceiving benefits after adversity: The relationship between self-reported posttraumatic growth and creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, Arts 7, 245264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabora, L. (1998). Weaving, bending, patching, mending the fabric of reality: A cognitive science perspective on worldview inconsistency. Foundations of Science, 3(2), 395428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabora, L. (2019). Creativity: Linchpin in the quest for a viable theory of cultural evolution. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 7783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabora, L., & Colgan, P. (1990). A model of the mechanisms underlying exploratory behavior. In Wilson, S. & Mayer, J. A. (Eds.), Proceedings of first international conference on the simulation of adaptive behavior (pp. 475484). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gabora, L., & Saab, A. (2011). Creative interference and states of potentiality in analogy problem solving. In Carlson, L., Hőlscher, C. & Shipley, T. F. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 35063511). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Gabora, L., & Steel, M. (2020a). A model of the transition to behavioral and cognitive modernity using reflexively autocatalytic networks. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 17(171), 1720200545. http://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2020.0545CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabora, L., & Steel, M. (2020b). Modeling a cognitive transition at the origin of cultural evolution using autocatalytic networks. Cognitive Science, 44(9), e12878. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12878CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabora, L., & Steel, M. (2021). An evolutionary process without variation and selection. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 18(180). 20210334. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2021.0334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabora, L., & Steel, M. (2017). Autocatalytic networks in cognition and the origin of culture. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 431, 8795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gabora, L., & Tseng, S. (2017). The social benefits of balancing creativity and imitation: Evidence from an agent-based model. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(4), 457473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kauffman, S. A. (2019). A world beyond physics: The emergence and evolution of life. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Koppl, R., Kauffman, S., Felin, T., & Longo, G. (2015). Economics for a creative world. Journal of Institutional Economics, 11(1), 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riley, S., & Gabora, L. (2012). Evidence that threatening situations enhance creativity. In Miyake, N., Peebles, D. & Cooper, R. P. (Eds.) Proceedings of the 34th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 22342239). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Ritter, S. M., Damian, R. I., Simonton, D. K., van Baaren, R. B., Strick, M., Derks, J., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). Diversifying experiences enhance cognitive flexibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(4), 961964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scotney, V., Schwartz, J., Carbert, C., Adam Saab, A., & Gabora, L. (2020). The form of a ‘half-baked’ creative idea: Empirical explorations into the structure of ill-defined mental representations. Acta Psychologica, 202, 102983. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102981Google Scholar
Smith, C., Gabora, L., & Gardner-O'Kearny, W. (2018). The extended evolutionary synthesis facilitates evolutionary models of culture change. Cliodynamics: The Journal of Quantitative History and Cultural Evolution, 9(2), 84107.Google Scholar
Watson, K. (2011). Gallows humor in medicine. Hastings Center Report, 41(5), 3745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weston, A., & Imas, J. M. (2018) Creativity: Transformation of adversity. In Martin, L. & Wilson, N. (Eds.), The Palgrave handbook of creativity at work (pp. 287–307). Palgrave Macmillan, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-77350-6_14.Google Scholar