Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T13:51:06.226Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Women need to stay alive and protect reproductive choice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 July 2022

Melissa M. McDonald
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Rochester, MI 48309-4482, USA mmmcdonald@oakland.edu rmjames@oakland.eduhttps://www.mcdonaldpeplab.com/
Rachel M. James
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Rochester, MI 48309-4482, USA mmmcdonald@oakland.edu rmjames@oakland.eduhttps://www.mcdonaldpeplab.com/

Abstract

Defense of reproductive choice is an important motivation in women's self-protection psychology for which the “staying alive theory” cannot fully account. Evidence indicates that some elements of women's self-protection psychology function to protect reproductive choice rather than survival, or may be equally well explained by either motivation. Integrating perspectives will result in greater explanatory breadth and precision in theory testing.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Benenson et al. propose an extension of Campbell's (Reference Campbell1999) staying alive theory (i.e., SAT), and marshal an impressive array of empirical data to support their claims. We agree with the authors' theoretical premise, but suggest that in order to fully capture females' self-protection psychology, the SAT must also address the importance of protecting reproductive choice. A key means by which females increase reproductive fitness is through strong mate preferences (Trivers, Reference Trivers and Campbell1972). Yet, female choice is often thwarted by males through sexual coercion and aggression (e.g., Brown, Reference Brown1952; Chagnon, Reference Chagnon1988; Minturn, Grosse, & Haider, Reference Minturn, Grosse and Haider1969; Palmer, Reference Palmer1989; Rozée, Reference Rozée1993; Sanday, Reference Sanday1981). Converging lines of evidence suggest that this intersexual competition has generated selection pressure for a psychological system in women designed to protect reproductive choice (reviewed in McDonald, James, & Roberto, Reference McDonald, James and Roberto2021). This is an important extension of the ideas proposed by Benenson et al. for each of the reasons outlined below.

Not all self-protection functions to promote survival

Women express greater fear of personal crimes (e.g., murder, assault, rape, burglary) than men (e.g., Ferraro, Reference Ferraro1996; Franklin & Franklin, Reference Franklin and Franklin2009), despite the fact that women are victimized less frequently for most such crimes, outside of rape (Craven, Reference Craven1997; Rennison & Rand, Reference Rennison and Rand2003). Although these data align with the SAT, women's greater fear of personal crimes appears to be driven more by the desire to protect reproductive choice than to stay alive. Indeed, sex differences in fear of personal crimes are eliminated or reversed when fear of rape is statistically controlled, but not fear of murder (Ferraro, Reference Ferraro1996; but see Lane & Meeker, Reference Lane and Meeker2003). Described as the “shadow of sexual assault,” women's greater fear of personal crimes is largely driven by a fear that those crimes will co-occur with rape (Ferraro, Reference Ferraro1995, Reference Ferraro1996; Fisher & Sloan, Reference Fisher and Sloan2003; May, Reference May2001; Warr, Reference Warr1984, Reference Warr1985). Moreover, women's fear of rape often matches or exceeds their fear of murder (Ferraro, Reference Ferraro1995, Reference Ferraro1996; Warr, Reference Warr1984). In short, the SAT is not sufficient to fully explain the sex difference observed in fear of personal crimes.

Women's fear of rape and their history of sexual assault are also associated with self-protection behaviors aimed at reducing the likelihood of becoming the victim of violent crime. For example, women consume true crime media with greater frequency than men, particularly when it has high information value for self-protection (e.g., when it features female victims, describes the perpetrator's motives, and provides tips for escape; Vicary & Fraley, Reference Vicary and Fraley2010). This desire for self-protective information appears to be rooted in women's fear of rape. Indeed, women who express a stronger fear of rape consume true crime media with greater frequency, and do so with the explicit motivation to learn strategies for defensive vigilance (McDonald et al., Reference McDonald, James and Roberto2021). Although consumption of true crime media may cause elevated rape fear, it is plausible that the causal pathway is bidirectional. To that point, women's sexual assault history (unlikely to be the causal outcome of true crime consumption) is positively associated with women's frequency of true crime media consumption and the motivation to consume it for the purpose of defensive vigilance. In total, there is good evidence to suggest that women who are particularly fearful of rape engage in behaviors to protect their reproductive choice.

A number of studies have also documented that women's self-protection behavior varies as a function of conception risk across the menstrual cycle, a key factor that exacerbates the reproductive cost of sexual assault. For example, studies have shown that women at high risk of conception, relative to women at low risk, are less likely to engage in sexually risky activities (Bröder & Hohmann, Reference Bröder and Hohmann2003; Chavanne & Gallup, Reference Chavanne and Gallup1998), infer greater threat from male strangers (Fessler, Holbrook, & Fleischman, Reference Fessler, Holbrook and Fleischman2014; Garver-Apgar, Gangestad, & Simpson, Reference Garver-Apgar, Gangestad and Simpson2007), exert greater handgrip strength in response to sexually threatening contexts (Petralia & Gallup, Reference Petralia and Gallup2002), and express greater bias against outgroup males (McDonald, Asher, Kerr, & Navarrete, Reference McDonald, Asher, Kerr and Navarrete2011, Reference McDonald, Donnellan, Cesario and Navarrete2015; Navarrete, Fessler, Fleischman, & Geyer, Reference Navarrete, Fessler, Fleischman and Geyer2009). While a general motivation for self-protection should be constant across the menstrual cycle, the motivation to protect reproductive choice is most pertinent on days when conception risk is elevated, indicating that these findings are better explained by the motivation to protect reproductive choice than the SAT.

The functions of self-protection behaviors may not be mutually exclusive

Some of the data presented by Benenson et al. cannot be easily explained by a motivation to protect reproductive choice (e.g., women's heightened immune response and lower pain threshold), but others are somewhat ambiguous. For example, the higher rate of nighttime awakenings among girls versus boys, and the onset of this sex difference during puberty, suggests that such awakenings could also be explained by the protection of reproductive choice. Given social and legal sanctions against rape, it is a behavior often hidden at night (Waterhouse, Reynolds, & Egan, Reference Waterhouse, Reynolds and Egan2016). Indeed, “nightcrawling” is a common form of rape in small-scale societies in which men trespass into women's homes to sexually coerce, rape, and abduct women (Rozée, Reference Rozée1993; Sanday, Reference Sanday1981).

Many of the findings highlighting women's tendency to engage in behaviors that reduce the risk of interpersonal conflict, such as smiling, politeness, recognizing negative emotions, avoiding confrontation, and experiencing more threat-responsive negative emotions, may also be seen as attempts to reduce the risk of sexual aggression – particularly given the overlap in women's fear of crime and rape. Similarly, the tendency for women to score more highly on measures of neuroticism may simultaneously function to protect reproductive choice, given research showing positive associations between neuroticism and women's fear of rape (McDonald, Coleman, & Brindley, Reference McDonald, Coleman and Brindley2019).

Conclusion

The expansion of the SAT is both a useful and powerful means of explaining women's self-protective psychology. However, a pure emphasis on survival without the integration of the importance of protecting reproductive choice, leaves some aspects of women's self-protection unexplained. Moreover, integrating reproductive choice as an additional motivation for self-protection would encourage research to engage in more precise theory testing to determine whether the pattern of data is better aligned with general self-protection motives or domain-specific self-protection attuned to threats against reproductive choice.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Bröder, A., & Hohmann, N. (2003). Variations in risk taking behavior over the menstrual cycle: An improved replication. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 391398. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00055-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. S. (1952). A comparative study of deviations from sexual mores. American Sociological Review, 17(2), 135146. https://doi.org/10.2307/2087650CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, A. (1999). Staying alive: Evolution, culture, and women's intrasexual aggression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(2), 203214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chagnon, N. A. (1988). Life histories, blood revenge, and warfare in a tribal population. Science, 239(4843), 985992. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.239.4843.985CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chavanne, T. J., & Gallup, G. G. Jr. (1998). Variation in risk taking behavior among female college students as a function of the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 2732. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00016-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craven, D. (1997). Sex differences in violent victimization, 1994. U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics, Special Report. Retrieved from https://bjs.ojp.gov/content/pub/pdf/SDVV.PDFGoogle Scholar
Ferraro, K. F. (1995). Fear of crime: Interpreting victimization risk. SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Ferraro, K. F. (1996). Women's fear of victimization: Shadow of sexual assault? Social Forces, 75, 667690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T., Holbrook, C., & Fleischman, D. S. (2014). Assets at risk: Menstrual cycle variation in the envisioned formidability of a potential sexual assailant reveals a component of threat assessment. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 1(3), 270290. doi:10.1007/s40750-014-0006-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, B. S., & Sloan, J. J. III. (2003). Unraveling the fear of victimization among college women: Is the shadow of sexual assault hypothesis supported? Justice Quarterly, 20, 633659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, C. A., & Franklin, T. W. (2009). Predicting fear of crime: Considering differences across gender. Feminist Criminology, 4(1), 83106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garver-Apgar, C. E., Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2007). Women's perceptions of men's sexual coerciveness change across the menstrual cycle. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39, 536540.Google Scholar
Lane, J., & Meeker, J. W. (2003). Women's and men's fear of gang crimes: Sexual and nonsexual assault as perceptually contemporaneous offenses. Justice Quarterly, 20, 337371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
May, D. C. (2001). The effect of fear of sexual victimization on adolescent fear of crime. Sociological Spectrum, 21, 141174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, M., Coleman, B., & Brindley, S. (2019). Calibrating fear of rape: Threat likelihood and victimization costs. Personality and Individual Differences, 139, 326330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, M. M., Asher, B. D., Kerr, N. L., & Navarrete, C. D. (2011). Fertility and intergroup bias in racial and minimal-group con- texts: Evidence for shared architecture. Psychological Science, 22, 860865. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611410985CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, M. M., Donnellan, M. B., Cesario, J., & Navarrete, C. D. (2015). Mate choice preferences in an intergroup context: Evidence for a sexual coercion threat-management system among women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 438445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, M. M., James, R. M., & Roberto, D. P. (2021). True crime consumption as defensive vigilance: Psychological mechanisms of a rape avoidance system. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 50(5), 20852108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minturn, L., Grosse, M., & Haider, S. (1969). Cultural patterning of sexual beliefs and behavior. Ethnology, 8(3), 301308. https://doi.org/10.2307/3772759CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Navarrete, C. D., Fessler, D. M., Fleischman, D. S., & Geyer, J. (2009). Race bias tracks conception risk across the menstrual cycle. Psychological Science, 20, 661665. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02352.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, C. (1989). Is rape a cultural universal? A re-examination of the ethnographic data. Ethnology, 28(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.2307/3773639CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petralia, S. M., & Gallup, G. G. Jr. (2002). Effects of a sexual assault scenario on handgrip strength across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 310. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00085-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rennison, C. M., & Rand, M. R. (2003). Criminal victimization, 2002. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics. Retrieved from https://www.prisonpolicy.org/scans/bjs/cv02.pdfGoogle Scholar
Rozée, P. D. (1993). Forbidden or forgiven? Rape in cross-cultural perspective. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17(4), 499514. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1993.tb00658.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanday, P. R. (1981). The socio-cultural context of rape: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Social Issues, 37(4), 527. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1981.tb01068.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, B. (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man (pp. 136179). Aldine.Google Scholar
Vicary, A. M., & Fraley, R. C. (2010). Captured by true crime: Why are women drawn to tales of rape, murder, and serial killers? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1(1), 8186. doi: 10.1177/1948550609355486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warr, M. (1984). Fear of victimization: Why are women and the elderly more afraid? Social Science Quarterly, 65, 681702.Google Scholar
Warr, M. (1985). Fear of rape among urban women. Social Problems, 32, 238250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waterhouse, G. F., Reynolds, A., & Egan, V. (2016). Myths and legends: The reality of rape offences reported to a UK police force. The European Journal of Psychology Applied to Legal Context, 8(1), 110. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpal.2015.04.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar