Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T19:48:47.691Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Disentangling the sense of ownership from the sense of fairness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 February 2013

Luca Tummolini
Affiliation:
Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, Italian National Research Council, 00185 Rome, Italy. luca.tummolini@istc.cnr.ithttp://www.istc.cnr.it/people/luca-tummoliniannamaria.borghi@unibo.ithttp://laral.istc.cnr.it/borghi/
Claudia Scorolli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, 40127 Bologna, Italy. claudia.scorolli2@unibo.ithttp://www.emco.unibo.it/groupCS.htm
Anna M. Borghi
Affiliation:
Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, Italian National Research Council, 00185 Rome, Italy. luca.tummolini@istc.cnr.ithttp://www.istc.cnr.it/people/luca-tummoliniannamaria.borghi@unibo.ithttp://laral.istc.cnr.it/borghi/ Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, 40127 Bologna, Italy. claudia.scorolli2@unibo.ithttp://www.emco.unibo.it/groupCS.htm

Abstract

Both evolutionary and developmental research indicate that humans are adapted to respecting property rights, independently (and possibly orthogonally) of considerations of fairness. We offer evidence from psychological experiments suggesting that enforcing one's rights and respecting others' possessions are basic cognitive mechanisms automatically activated and grounded in humans' sensory-motor system. This may entail an independent motivation that is more profound than considerations of fairness and impartiality.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013

Baumard et al. hypothesize that cooperative moves in the form of transfer of money to other participants are often not forms of altruistic giving but rather attempts to refrain from stealing the money over which the others have legitimate claims. Though we share with Baumard et al. the claim that people take into account property rights when distributing monetary resources, it is not clear whether this is in fact evidence that subjects aim at a fair distribution motivated by a partner-selection based morality.

Actually, respecting property rights may be an adaptation independent from, and possibly orthogonal to, mutualistic morality. Indeed, evolutionary and developmental work suggests that humans (as many other animals) are equipped with a basic sense of ownership that exploits a number of cues to establish property rights over things. Sensitivity to these cues is an evolved adaptation for mutual advantage, which, however, does not need social selection to be explained (Maynard Smith & Parker Reference Maynard Smith and Parker1976). As far as low-value items are concerned, ownership rights established by cues of first possession and over the product of one's own labor can be explained in this way. In contrast, rights over high-value resources that can be secured only through collaboration require the cultural evolution of some form of sharing norms to be sustained (Gintis Reference Gintis2007). Developmental evidence supports this view, too. Several studies show that infants have a sense of ownership since their birth (Rochat Reference Rochat2011) and become sensitive at property rights of others already at 3 years (Friedman & Neary Reference Friedman and Neary2008; Kanngiesser et al. Reference Kanngiesser, Gjersoe and Hood2010; Rossano et al. Reference Rossano, Rakoczy and Tomasello2011). However, it is mainly due to the role of active teaching (especially by their parents) that they learn to share with others from there on (Rochat et al. Reference Rochat, Dias, Guo, MacGillivray, Passos-Ferreira, Winning and Berg2009; Ross Reference Ross1996). The ability to modulate one's possessive behaviors is thus particularly important to favor the kind of social harmony required in collaborative activities.

At the level of cognitive mechanisms, different studies indicate that humans have a rather precocious sense of possession for objects. Psychological experiments (e.g., Chen & Bargh Reference Chen and Bargh1999; Freina et al. Reference Freina, Baroni, Borghi and Nicoletti2009) reveal that when presented with positive words, participants tend to perform an approach movement, in order to attract the objects they refer to; the opposite is true for negative words. This advantage of the self for positive objects persists even when participants are asked both to take an object for themselves and to give a different one to others (Gianelli et al. Reference Gianelli, Lugli, Baroni, Nicoletti, Borghi, Kokinov, Karmiloff-Smith and Nersessian2011). This can obviously lead to competitive situations with respect to objects' possession (Gianelli et al., in press). These studies suggest that humans have developed this basic tendency to keep positive objects for themselves.

Beyond this very basic tendency to keep positive objects for ourselves, a number of results suggest that an early sense of ownership develops as well. Recent experiments we performed (Scorolli et al. Reference Scorolli, Borghi and Tummolini2012) showed that the sense of ownership is a basic mechanism, which is activated quite fast and automatically, since it emerges even in tasks in which no reference to the sense of ownership is made. We used the same context to evaluate the relative weight of different cues in determining the sense of ownership: physical proximity, discovery, and physical contact with the same originally neutral objects. In different experiments, participants were shown a virtual room with an object located on a table. In one condition two actors were alone in the scene; in another condition an external observer was present as well. The external observer was introduced in order to verify whether the sense of ownership would be modulated by the presence of a third impartial person. Immediately after the virtual scene, participants were presented with a sentence, referring to the ownership of the object (e.g., “The girl owns the book”; “The book belongs to the girl”). Their task consisted in evaluating whether or not the sentences were sensible. Analysis of response times provided evidence of the development of a basic sense of ownership based on object closeness in space (the object could be located near to the protagonist or not), on discovery (the participant would see the protagonist discovering the object), and on contact (the participant would see the protagonist touching an object). Finally, Constable et al. (Reference Constable, Kritikos and Bayliss2011) demonstrated that the automatic tendency to respond to objects' affordances (i.e., action potentialities evoked by objects) is inhibited once we know that it belongs to someone else. In a stimulus–response compatibility task (see Tucker & Ellis Reference Tucker and Ellis1998), the classic compatibility effect was abolished when participants had to respond to an object owned by the experimenter. This suggests that the action system is automatically inhibited and blind to the potential for action toward another person's possession. Taken together, these studies provide initial evidence that a fast, possibly automatic, embodied mechanism is at the basis of the development of the early sense of ownership.

It would be really difficult to explain these results by starting from the idea that the respect of property rights is motivated mainly by a biologically evolved sense of fairness. However, it is possible that the existence of a basic sense of ownership, as that for which we provide evidence, complements the influence of a socially developed sense of fairness. We propose that these two different mechanisms – the basic sense of ownership and the evolved sense of fairness – differ along various dimensions: in cognitive control (i.e., the first mechanism is automatic while the second is controlled); in time course (i.e., the first is rather precocious while the second occurs later); and in penetrability (i.e., the second can be more easily modulated by social and cultural context). So far, the results of our studies suggest that an early activation of the sense of ownership is based on different factors and is partly grounded in our sensorimotor experience. It is plausible that the tendency to keep all good things for ourselves and the acknowledgment of property rights co-occur, and that the competition between these two contrasting basic tendencies is won differently, depending on the context. In the same vein, Neary (Reference Neary2011) has suggested that children learn the appropriate contexts where to override possessive inclinations in favor of sharing with others. Thus, this ability of sharing could develop later, in contrast with the more primitive need for rigid possessive behaviors. Further experiments and studies are needed to investigate the interplay between the primitive tendency to keep good things for ourselves, the early sense of ownership, and the probably later socially developed sense of fairness.

References

Chen, M. & Bargh, J. A. (1999) Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 25:215–24.Google Scholar
Constable, M., Kritikos, A. & Bayliss, A. (2011) Grasping the concept of personal property. Cognition 119:430–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freina, L., Baroni, G., Borghi, A. M. & Nicoletti, R. (2009) Emotive concept-nouns and motor responses: Attraction or repulsion? Memory and Cognition 37:493–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, O. & Neary, K. R. (2008) Determining who owns what: Do children infer ownership from first possession? Cognition 107:829–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gianelli, C., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Nicoletti, R. & Borghi, A. M. (2011) “The object is wonderful or prickly”: How different object properties modulate behavior in a joint context. In: European perspectives on cognitive science, ed. Kokinov, B., Karmiloff-Smith, A. & Nersessian, N. J.. New Bulgarian University Press. Available at: http://nbu.bg/cogs/eurocogsci2011/proceedings/Google Scholar
Gianelli, C., Scorolli, C. & Borghi, A. M. (in press) Acting in perspective: The role of body and of language as social tools. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0401-0.Google Scholar
Gintis, H. (2007) The evolution of private property. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 64(1):116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanngiesser, P., Gjersoe, N. & Hood, B. M. (2010) The effect of creative labor on property-ownership transfer by preschool children and adults. Psychological Science 21:1236–41.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J. & Parker, G. A. (1976) The logic of asymmetric contests. Animal Behaviour 24:159–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neary, K. R. (2011) Children's and adults' reasoning in property entitlement disputes. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Waterloo.Google Scholar
Rochat, P. (2011) Possession and morality in early development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 132:2338.Google Scholar
Rochat, P., Dias, M. D. G., Guo, L., MacGillivray, T., Passos-Ferreira, C., Winning, A. & Berg, B. (2009) Fairness in distributive justice by 3- and 5-year-olds across 7 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 40(3):416–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, H. S. (1996) Negotiating principles of entitlement in sibling property disputes. Developmental Psychology 32:90101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossano, F., Rakoczy, H. & Tomasello, M. (2011) Young children's understanding of violations of property rights. Cognition 121:219–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scorolli, C., Borghi, A. M. & Tummolini, L. (2012) The owner is closer and is the first to discover objects: Exploring the embodied nature of object ownership. Paper presented at the Workshop on Concepts, Actions and Objects (CAOS 2012), Rovereto, Italy, May 24–27, 2012.Google Scholar
Tucker, M. & Ellis, R. (1998) On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 24(3):830–46.Google ScholarPubMed