Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T09:51:26.785Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Don't throw motivation out with the black box: The value of a good theory revisited

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2025

Jutta Heckhausen*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Science, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA heckhaus@uci.edu https://faculty.sites.uci.edu/heckhausen/
Falko Rheinberg
Affiliation:
Department Psychologie, Universität Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany motivation@psych.uni-potsdam.de
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

Murayama and Jach claim that current motivational constructs do not specify causal processes (black-box problem) and that mental computational processes solve this problem. We argue, process-focused research requires theoretical frameworks addressing situational variations, individual differences, and their interaction. Classic achievement motivation theory provides comprehensive models with empirically measurable process-related constructs and predictions. Recent developments build on this, addressing motivation, action, and their socio-cultural and lifespan context. Theory-free mental computational processes cannot do any of that.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

A focus on process is laudable, but…

All psychological research should aim at identifying the specific processes that underly human behavior. However, “mental computational processes” championed by Murayama and Jach (M&J) lack a theoretical framework that is comprised of propositions about how these processes emerge, what influences them, and how they affect experience and behavior. Neuronal activity and the “mental computations” they can compose are not functional in and of themselves. They emerge as functional processes in the context of an individual's interactions with the environment.

Nothing is as process-focused as a good theory

Good theories comprise theoretical constructs that are clearly defined and operationalized, and that have a specific function in the theoretical model. An excellent example of a theory comprising elaborated processes that are integrated into a person-by-situation interaction framework following Lewin's (Reference Lewin and Carmichael1946) axiom, is achievement motivation theory (Atkinson, Reference Atkinson1957; Heckhausen, Reference Heckhausen1977; McClelland, Atkinson, Clark, & Lowell, Reference McClelland, Atkinson, Clark and Lowell1953) with its risk-taking model in terms of anticipated self-evaluation after success and failure. This theory solves the alleged black-box problem by making distinct predictions about the effects of situational incentives on behavior, such as task choice, effort, and task performance for individuals with predominant hope for success versus predominant fear of failure. Achievement situations with intermediate challenge are the most informative about one's own competence and therefore the most attractive for success seekers and the most threatening for failure avoiders (Brunstein & Heckhausen, Reference Brunstein, Heckhausen, Heckhausen and Heckhausen2018). Their task choices, effort investment, emotional responses, causal attributions, and performance vary accordingly along predicted patterns. Such person-by-situation interactions are based on the theoretically proposed and empirically tested process variables of goal setting, causal attribution, and self-evaluation (Brunstein & Heckhausen, Reference Brunstein, Heckhausen, Heckhausen and Heckhausen2018; Heckhausen, Reference Heckhausen1977; Stiensmeyer-Pelster & Heckhausen, Reference Stiensmeyer-Pelster, Heckhausen, Heckhausen and Heckhausen2018). These processes develop as mutually stabilizing individual differences in cognitive and emotional predispositions during goal-directed interactions with the environment in childhood (Heckhausen, Reference Heckhausen, Sarason and Spielberger1975; Heckhausen & Heckhausen, Reference Heckhausen, Heckhausen, Heckhausen and Heckhausen2018). Further compelling evidence for achievement motivation theory comes from the effective use of its models for intervention and change of motive dispositions and associated biases in cognition, emotion, and behavior (Rheinberg & Engeser, Reference Rheinberg, Engeser, Schultheiss and Brunstein2010).

Hence, even several decades ago, motivation was not at all a black box. Instead, theoretical models of specific and functionally interrelated processes interfacing person and situation were developed, operationalized, and thus made accessible for empirical research. More recent developments in achievement motivation research associated with Eccles and Wigfield's Situated Expectancy-Value Theory (Eccles & Wigfield, Reference Eccles and Wigfield2002, Reference Eccles and Wigfield2020) differentiate among different value components as a function of social developmental context and individual preference. This allows for more differentiated and developmentally as well as culturally informed modeling of achievement-motivated behavior.

“Mental computation” or functionality of motivational mindsets

Mental computation and cognitive processes are important, but the energizing and directional function of motivation critically relies on experienced and anticipated affect and its change (e.g., anticipated enjoyment of activity, anticipated pride about own competence). According to the Rubicon model of action-phases, cognition and affect work hand in hand, but have shifting priorities depending on whether the individual is trying to determine the optimal goal (deliberative phase of action cycle) or pursuing an already chosen goal (implemental phase) (Gollwitzer, Reference Gollwitzer, Higgins and Sorrentino1990; Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, Reference Heckhausen and Gollwitzer1987). During the pre-decision phase, objectivity and breadth of mental computation is essential and thus a deliberative mindset is activated, whereas during the post-decision phases, biased information processing following an implementation mindset shields the intended action from distractions and conflicting tendencies (Achtziger & Gollwitzer, Reference Achtziger, Gollwitzer, Heckhausen and Heckhausen2018).

Motivation is a product of evolution and development

M&J address the evolution of motivation but ignore individual differences. Motivational mental processes are a product of phylogenetically evolved pre-adaptations and ontogenetically developed strategies and patterns in a specific individual. They are only in part universal outcomes of behavioral evolution at a phylogenetic scale (e.g., classical and operant conditioning and mastery striving, Heckhausen, Reference Heckhausen2000). The non-universal motivational processes reflect the ontogeny of individuals and their unique experiences with affective change (self and other-regulated), bearing a strong influence of preverbal exposure to affect–change patterns in the context of the parent–child dyad (Heckhausen & Heckhausen, Reference Heckhausen, Heckhausen, Heckhausen and Heckhausen2018; Kuhl & Völker, Reference Kuhl, Völker and Keller1998). Formative developmental conditions are associated with transitions, for instance the transition from intra-individual to inter-individual reference frames when starting school with its dominant evaluative framework of social comparison. As individuals become more self-regulated in adolescence and adulthood, their potential to follow the motivational predispositions acquired earlier increases exponentially and further stabilizes them. In this process, individuals become increasingly nimble in regulating and optimizing their own motivation in a given situational set of incentives, based on their extensive experience with self-regulation of motivation (Rheinberg & Engeser, Reference Rheinberg, Engeser, Schultheiss and Brunstein2010).

Levels of analyses: From micro to macro to micro

As M&J demand, we do need to get closer to the actual processes that are at play in specific situations. However, we should not throw out the conceptual and empirical progress we already made, just because we cannot yet successfully capture the more molecular processes. On the other hand, having such higher order constructs should not spare us from digging deeper into the more molecular processes. In our discovery endeavor to identify and link up processes at different levels of analysis, different approaches can complement each other. An example is the combination of situated expectancy-value theory (Eccles & Wigfield, Reference Eccles and Wigfield2002, Reference Eccles and Wigfield2020) which addresses goal choice, and motivational theory of lifespan development (Heckhausen, Wrosch, & Schulz, Reference Heckhausen, Wrosch and Schulz2010, Reference Heckhausen, Wrosch and Schulz2019), which addresses how long-term goals are pursued and changed across the life course (von Keyserlingk, Rubach, Lee, Eccles, & Heckhausen, Reference von Keyserlingk, Rubach, Lee, Eccles and Heckhausen2022).

Conclusion

We can best examine the specific processes that bring about motivational experiences and behavior, if we have a theoretical framework that is guided by the function of the phenomena to be explained. A computational analysis per se solves no problems. The good news is, there is no black-box problem and no need to reinvent the wheel. We can follow the guidance of motivational scholars in the 1970s and 80s who pioneered and developed achievement motivation research. Uncovering person by situation interactions puts us motivational psychologists ahead of many in more unidimensional fields such as personality or social psychology.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interests

None.

References

Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2018). Motivation and volition in the course of action. In Heckhausen, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 485528). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunstein, J. C., & Heckhausen, H. (2018). Achievement motivation. In Heckhausen, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 221304). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2020). From expectancy-value theory to situated expectancy-value theory: A developmental, social cognitive, and sociocultural perspective on motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61, 101859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 5392). Guilford.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1975). Fear of failure as a self-reinforcing motive system. In Sarason, I. G. & Spielberger, C. (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. II, pp. 117128). Hemisphere.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 283329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2000). Evolutionary perspectives on human motivation. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 10151029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and cognitive functioning in motivational and volitional states of mind. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Heckhausen, H. (2018). Development of motivation. In Heckhausen, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 679743). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychological Review, 117, 3260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2019). Agency and motivation in adulthood and old age. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 191217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, J., & Völker, S. (1998). Entwicklung und Persönlichkeit [Development and personality]. In Keller, H. (Ed.), Lehrbuch der entwicklungspsychologie (pp. 207240). Huber.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1946). Behavior and development as a function of the total situation. In Carmichael, L. (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (pp. 791844). John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheinberg, F., & Engeser, S. (2010). Motive training and motivational competence. In Schultheiss, O. C. & Brunstein, J. C. (Eds.) Implicit motives (pp. 510548). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiensmeyer-Pelster, J., & Heckhausen, H. (2018). Causal attribution of behavior and achievement. In Heckhausen, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Motivation and action (pp. 623678). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Keyserlingk, L., Rubach, C., Lee, H. R., Eccles, J. S., & Heckhausen, J. (2022). College students’ motivational beliefs and use of goal-oriented control strategies: Integrating two theories of motivated behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 46, 601620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar