Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T05:50:26.394Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Separation/connection procedures: From cleansing behavior to numerical cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 February 2021

Arianna Felisatti
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany14476martinf@uni-potsdam.de;  ariannafelisatti@uni-potsdam.de;   kulkova@uni-potsdam.de;  kkuehne@uni-potsdam.de; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/arianna-felisatti; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/prof-martin-fischer-phd; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/elena-kulkova; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/katharina-kuehne
Martin H. Fischer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany14476martinf@uni-potsdam.de;  ariannafelisatti@uni-potsdam.de;   kulkova@uni-potsdam.de;  kkuehne@uni-potsdam.de; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/arianna-felisatti; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/prof-martin-fischer-phd; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/elena-kulkova; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/katharina-kuehne
Elena Kulkova
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany14476martinf@uni-potsdam.de;  ariannafelisatti@uni-potsdam.de;   kulkova@uni-potsdam.de;  kkuehne@uni-potsdam.de; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/arianna-felisatti; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/prof-martin-fischer-phd; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/elena-kulkova; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/katharina-kuehne
Katharina Kühne
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany14476martinf@uni-potsdam.de;  ariannafelisatti@uni-potsdam.de;   kulkova@uni-potsdam.de;  kkuehne@uni-potsdam.de; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/arianna-felisatti; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/prof-martin-fischer-phd; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/elena-kulkova; https://www.uni-potsdam.de/en/pecog/members/katharina-kuehne
Alexej Michirev
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychology, German Sport University, Cologne, Germany50933.   a.michirev@dshs-koeln.de; https://www.dshs-koeln.de/visitenkarte/person/alexej-michirev/

Abstract

Lee and Schwarz (L&S) suggest that separation is the grounded procedure underlying cleansing effects in different psychological domains. Here, we interpret L&S's account from a hierarchical view of cognition that considers the influence of physical properties and sensorimotor constraints on mental representations. This approach allows theoretical integration and generalization of L&S's account to the domain of formal quantitative reasoning.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Lee and Schwarz (L&S) argue that “much like cognitive capacities in general are grounded in sensorimotor ones, […] mental procedures in particular are grounded in physical procedures […]” (sect. 3.1, para. 5). Their grounded procedures are generalizable because brains are incorporated in bodies that interact with the environment, so embodied cognition integrates physical properties of the world, sensorimotor constraints of our body, and contextual factors. Despite broad agreement, Matheson and Barsalou (Reference Matheson, Barsalou, Wixted and Thompson-Schill2018) recently diagnosed that an “overarching theory of embodiment and grounded cognition” is impeded by rather heterogeneous contributions from different disciplines. A hierarchical distinction between grounded, embodied, and situated cognition, initially proposed for the domain of numerical cognition (Fischer, Reference Fischer2012), can accomplish the desired theoretical integration.

The proposed hierarchy (see also Myachykov, Scheepers, Fischer, & Kessler, Reference Myachykov, Scheepers, Fischer and Kessler2014; Pezzulo et al., Reference Pezzulo, Barsalou, Cangelosi, Fischer, McRae and Spivey2013) first considers universal physical constraints on cognition, resulting from our environment (grounding through physical laws that shaped our nervous systems). A second level establishes embodiment of cognition through our sensory-motor history, including learned procedures. Finally, representing specific task instructions situates cognition and explains flexible performance signatures. Importantly, our conceptual distinction explains performance biases across domains, as we now illustrate.

Consider grounding first. L&S wrote about “causal links between physical cleansing and various psychological variables” and asked: “Empirically, how robust are they?” (Abstract). Because of its evolutionary origin, physical cleansing grounded on basic emotions will be most robust and hold universally for all separation/connection procedures based on approach and avoidance, for example, disgust towards rotten food (Ekman, Sorenson, & Friesen, Reference Ekman, Sorenson and Friesen1969). In fact, the feeling of disgust is triggered not only by physical contaminants, but also by moral impurities and “form(s) part of a behavioural loss aversion system aimed at protecting valuable resources, including the integrity of one's body” (Schnall, Reference Schnall2017, p. 50).

Equally grounded is the fact that object accumulations produce higher piles because physical laws prevent objects from penetrating each other. The universal association “more is up” consequently informs metaphorical language (Lakoff & Johnson, Reference Lakoff and Johnson1980) and arithmetic intuitions (Lakoff & Núñez, Reference Lakoff and Núñez2000) and also induces judgment biases when accepting results that exceed the correct sum (“addition is more”; Shaki, Pinhas, & Fischer, Reference Shaki, Pinhas and Fischer2018).

Consider embodiment next. L&S describe how cleansing separates the self from failure (sect. 3.1, para. 6) and how physical and mental connecting procedures are related (sect. 5). Again, we find similar embodiment signatures in mathematical cognition, where physical procedures of separation and connection prime subtraction and addition solutions, respectively (Werner & Raab, Reference Werner and Raab2013; Werner, Raab, & Fischer, Reference Werner, Raab and Fischer2019). Conceptualization of addition/subtraction as connection/separation is also revealed by semantic priming between linguistic expressions defining commonly related entities and additions (Bassok, Pedigo, & Oskarsson, Reference Bassok, Pedigo and Oskarsson2008) and by the importance of gestures in math education (Sinclair & Heyd-Metzuyanim, Reference Sinclair and Heyd-Metzuyanim2014).

By distinguishing grounding from embodiment we understand cleansing behaviors both as universally grounded mechanisms of connection and separation, and as culturally learned and experienced embodied metaphors of morality or guilt. These descriptions are hierarchically organized and complementary and explain cross-domain interactions, such as increased prosocial (Ding et al., Reference Ding, Xie, Sun, Li, Wang and Zhen2016; Liao, Yam, Johnson, Liu, & Song, Reference Liao, Yam, Johnson, Liu and Song2018) or self-punishing behavior (Schei, Sheikh, & Schnall, Reference Schei, Sheikh and Schnall2019) to compensate for moral transgression.

Having transferred the hierarchical distinction between grounded and embodied cognition from calculation to cleansing, we now wish to show how mental arithmetic can equally benefit from L&S's study of separation and connection procedures. In line with a hierarchical approach to cognition, they suggested that “grounded procedures of separation can be a proximate mechanism underlying cleansing effects” (Introduction, para. 4) and “once activated, whether physically or mentally, a procedure can be applied across content domains, even in unrelated situations” (sect. 3.1, para. 5). Doing just this, we realize that “at the core of grounded procedures are physical actions, which move through space” (sect. 7, para. 4), although at the core of mathematical learning are physical manipulation of quantity across space. Indeed, abstract concepts are typically understood in terms of concrete concepts: Counting numbers originated from piling up pebbles to quantify sheep (Keranen, Reference Keranen2016, p. 12); children acquire basic arithmetic during putting objects into and out of containers, thus constructing metaphors grounded in everyday experiences (Lakoff & Núñez, Reference Lakoff and Núñez2000).

The fact that separation/connection procedures and subtraction/addition procedures are grounded on similar physical mechanisms, predicts that they may also activate emotions similarly because bodily states and perceptions form an integral part of emotional experiences (Winkielman, Niedenthal, Wielgosz, Eelen, & Kavanagh, Reference Winkielman, Niedenthal, Wielgosz, Eelen, Kavanagh, Mikulincer, Shaver, Borgida and Bargh2015). Indeed, “acts of separation are more likely to be triggered by negative entities, acts of connection are more likely to be triggered by positive entities” (sect. 5, para. 2). Confirming this extension across domains, several studies showed that separation procedures affect emotion perception by reducing negative moral emotions (Lee, Tang, Wan, Mai, & Liu, Reference Lee, Tang, Wan, Mai and Liu2015; Zhong & Liljenquist, Reference Zhong and Liljenquist2006), decreasing negative (guilt and shame) and increasing positive emotions (happiness; Tang et al., Reference Tang, Lu, Su, Liang, Mai and Liu2017) or even resetting them (clean slate effect), which consequently reduced the strictness of moral judgments (Kaspar, Krapp, & König, Reference Kaspar, Krapp and König2015). Similarly, studies demonstrating effects of emotion on arithmetic problem-solving further support this extension (Fabre & Lemaire, Reference Fabre and Lemaire2019; Schimmack & Derryberry, Reference Schimmack and Derryberry2005).

If we assume that cleansing behaviors and mathematical thinking share mechanisms of separation versus connection, then cross-domain priming paradigms can test this prediction. Thus, in the framework of embodied mathematics, subtraction presupposes separation (Lakoff & Núñez, Reference Lakoff and Núñez2000), parallel to cleansing behaviors, such as hand washing. Will participants solve subtraction tasks faster after washing their hands? Or will they slow down because they already “separated” themselves and a greater separation would mean more effort? Answering these questions will clarify the mechanisms connecting everyday behaviors.

We end this comparison with a challenge pointing towards situated influences on cognition. Decision-making, as well as mathematical reasoning, features heuristics and biases (Kahneman, Reference Kahneman2011; Shaki et al., Reference Shaki, Pinhas and Fischer2018). While L&S (sect. 7, para. 3) predicted that anchoring bias should increase through acts of connection, anchoring is instead more prevalent in subtraction than in addition (Shaki et al., Reference Shaki, Pinhas and Fischer2018). How can we explain this wrong prediction? Although the hierarchical understanding of grounded procedures invites a multi-layered analysis of behavior, pervasive heuristics/biases signal additional, context-dependent influences.

Financial support

This work was supported by the German Research Funding Council (DFG) under grant agreement FI 1915/5-2, awarded to MHF.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Bassok, M., Pedigo, S. F., & Oskarsson, A. T. (2008). Priming addition facts with semantic relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(2), 343352. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.343.Google ScholarPubMed
Ding, W., Xie, R., Sun, B., Li, W., Wang, D., & Zhen, R. (2016). Why does the “sinner” act prosocially? The mediating role of guilt and the moderating role of moral identity in motivating moral cleansing. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1317. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion. Science (New York, N.Y.), 164(3875), 8688. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.164.3875.86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabre, L., & Lemaire, P. (2019). How emotions modulate arithmetic performance: A study in arithmetic problem verification tasks. Experimental Psychology, 66(5), 368376. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, M. H. (2012). A hierarchical view of grounded, embodied, and situated numerical cognition. Cognitive Processing, 13(1), 161164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0477-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Macmillan Publishers.Google Scholar
Kaspar, K., Krapp, V., & König, P. (2015). Hand washing induces a clean slate effect in moral judgments: A pupillometry and eye-tracking study. Scientific Reports, 5, 10471. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep10471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keranen, R. (2016). Inventions in computing: From the abacus to personal computers. Cavendish Square Publishing, LLC.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press. https://books.google.com.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being (Vol. 6, 1st ed.). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lee, S. W. S., Tang, H., Wan, J., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2015). A cultural look at moral purity: Wiping the face clean. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 577. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liao, Z., Yam, K. C., Johnson, R. E., Liu, W., & Song, Z. (2018). Cleansing my abuse: A reparative response model of perpetrating abusive supervisor behavior. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(9), 10391056. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matheson, H. E., & Barsalou, L. (2018). Embodiment and grounding in cognitive neuroscience. In Wixted, J. T. & Thompson-Schill, S. (Eds.), The Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience (4th Edition, Volume 3: Language and thought). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119170174.epcn310.Google Scholar
Myachykov, A., Scheepers, C., Fischer, M. H., & Kessler, K. (2014). TEST: A tropic, embodied, and situated theory of cognition. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6(3), 442460. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L. W., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M. H., McRae, K., & Spivey, M. (2013). Computational grounded cognition: A new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 612. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schei, T. S., Sheikh, S., & Schnall, S. (2019). Atoning past indulgences: Oral consumption and moral compensation. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2103. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schimmack, U., & Derryberry, D. E. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: Threat, negativity, or arousal? Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 5(1), 5566. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnall, S. (2017). Disgust as embodied loss aversion. European Review of Social Psychology, 28(1), 5094. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2016.1259844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaki, S., Pinhas, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2018). Heuristics and biases in mental arithmetic: Revisiting and reversing operational momentum. Thinking & Reasoning, 24(2), 138156. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2017.1348987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, N., & Heyd-Metzuyanim, E. (2014). Learning number with TouchCounts: The role of emotions and the body in mathematical communication. Technology, Knowledge and Learning, 19(1–2), 8199. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10758-014-9212-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tang, H., Lu, X., Su, R., Liang, Z., Mai, X., & Liu, C. (2017). Washing away your sins in the brain: Physical cleaning and priming of cleaning recruit different brain networks after moral threat. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(7), 11491158. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, K., & Raab, M. (2013). Moving to solution. Experimental Psychology, 60(6), 403409. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, K., Raab, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2019). Moving arms: The effects of sensorimotor information on the problem-solving process. Thinking & Reasoning, 25(2), 171191. doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2018.1494630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winkielman, P., Niedenthal, P., Wielgosz, J., Eelen, J., & Kavanagh, L. C. (2015). Embodiment of cognition and emotion. In Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Borgida, E., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), APA Handbooks in psychology®. APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Vol. 1. Attitudes and social cognition (pp. 151175). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14341-004.Google Scholar
Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science (New York, N.Y.), 313(5792), 14511452. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1130726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed