Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T03:46:55.045Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ecological priming: Convergent evidence for the link between ecology and psychological processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 August 2013

Michele J. Gelfand
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742. mgelfand@umd.eduhttp://www.bsos.umd.edu/psyc/gelfand/janettalun@gmail.com
Janetta Lun
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742. mgelfand@umd.eduhttp://www.bsos.umd.edu/psyc/gelfand/janettalun@gmail.com

Abstract

This commentary describes the use of ecological priming methods to address the limitations of the correlational research discussed in the target article. We provide examples from our own work on cultural tightness–looseness to illustrate how we can bring ecological and societal conditions into the laboratory in order to study the impact of ecological threats on psychological processes experimentally.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Van de Vliert's tour de force illustrates an important interaction between ecology and societal resources that replicates across numerous studies. The extent to which nations are able to use monetary resources to meet climatic demands leads to fundamentally different psychological and behavioral adaptations. The research is consistent with our work on cultural tightness–looseness, which shows that other aspects of the ecology that threaten human survival (e.g., resource scarcity, natural disasters, disease, territorial threats) can be met with other societal resources, namely, the strength of social norms and punishments (Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Raver, Nishii, Leslie, Lun, Lim, Duan, Almaliach, Ang, Arnadottir, Aycan, Boehnke, Boski, Cabecinhas, Chan, Chhokar, D'Amato, Ferrer, Fischlmayr, Fischer, Fülöp, Georgas, Kashima, Kashima, Kim, Lempereur, Marquez, Othman, Overlaet, Panagiotopoulou, Peltzer, Perez-Florizno, Ponomarenko, Realo, Schei, Schmitt, Smith, Soomro, Szabo, Taveesin, Toyama, Van de Vliert, Vohra, Ward and Yamaguchi2011), which affects a wide range of cultural differences.

Nonetheless, both our fieldwork and Van de Vliert's are limited, as he aptly notes, given the inherent correlational nature of cross-cultural fieldwork. Can we use other research methods that go beyond correlational studies to ascertain whether ecology causes differences in freedom of self-expression, ingroup favoritism, and outgroup derogation? Testing a theory with multiple methodologies affords more confidence in results, particularly in cross-cultural research that presents many rival hypotheses (Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Raver, Holcombe Ehrhart and Rogelberg2002). To that end, we (Lun et al. Reference Lun, Gelfand and Mohr2012) implemented an ecological priming paradigm to examine whether one can prime ecological and historical threats that occur naturally in the real world in controlled laboratory settings. By making societal threats temporarily accessible, we can observe the impact of ecology on psychological processes and examine whether the effects are similar to those found in large-scale correlational studies. Although scholars have long argued that cultural differences can be primed using classic social cognition methods (Oyserman & Lee Reference Oyserman and Lee2008; Trafimow et al. Reference Trafimow, Triandis and Goto1991), the application of priming methods to study ecological influences on behavior is surprisingly limited (see Mortensen et al. Reference Mortensen, Becker, Ackerman, Neuberg and Kenrick2010).

In our field research, we showed that cultures vary considerably in the degree to which they are tight – that is, have strong norms and a low tolerance for deviant behavior – versus loose – that is, have weak norms and a high tolerance for deviant behavior (Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Raver, Nishii, Leslie, Lun, Lim, Duan, Almaliach, Ang, Arnadottir, Aycan, Boehnke, Boski, Cabecinhas, Chan, Chhokar, D'Amato, Ferrer, Fischlmayr, Fischer, Fülöp, Georgas, Kashima, Kashima, Kim, Lempereur, Marquez, Othman, Overlaet, Panagiotopoulou, Peltzer, Perez-Florizno, Ponomarenko, Realo, Schei, Schmitt, Smith, Soomro, Szabo, Taveesin, Toyama, Van de Vliert, Vohra, Ward and Yamaguchi2011). Tightness–looseness is related to a broad array of ecological and human-made threats that nations have (or have not) historically encountered. As compared to loose cultures, tight cultures have greater resource scarcity, more vulnerability to natural disasters, higher disease prevalence, higher population density, and a greater degree of threats from neighboring countries. Psychologically speaking, people in tight cultures have more of a prevention focus (Higgins Reference Higgins1996) and have greater impulse control (Baumeister & Heatherton Reference Baumeister and Heatherton1996), need for structure (Neuberg & Newsom Reference Neuberg and Newsom1993), and self-monitoring (Snyder & Gangestad Reference Snyder and Gangestad1986) as compared to people in loose cultures. In tight cultures, people also find socially deviant behavior much less justifiable and have more ethnocentric attitudes (Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Raver, Nishii, Leslie, Lun, Lim, Duan, Almaliach, Ang, Arnadottir, Aycan, Boehnke, Boski, Cabecinhas, Chan, Chhokar, D'Amato, Ferrer, Fischlmayr, Fischer, Fülöp, Georgas, Kashima, Kashima, Kim, Lempereur, Marquez, Othman, Overlaet, Panagiotopoulou, Peltzer, Perez-Florizno, Ponomarenko, Realo, Schei, Schmitt, Smith, Soomro, Szabo, Taveesin, Toyama, Van de Vliert, Vohra, Ward and Yamaguchi2011).

To establish a causal link between ecology and psychological processes, we conducted a series of studies in which we made ecological and societal threats accessible in the laboratory. In one study, we tested whether high versus low population density would make people more or less tolerant of socially deviant behavior (e.g., taking drugs, having casual sex, littering, stealing, and talking loudly at a library). We randomly assigned participants to read one of the two versions of an article presumably to be printed in the local school newspaper. The article discussed how the campus of the participants' university is one of the highest (or lowest) in population density compared to other similar universities. The message was supported by statistical graphs and quotes of student life throughout the article. We then asked participants questions regarding social deviance and assessed ethnocentric attitudes. Consistent with the field data, those who were primed to think that their university campus has high population density were more likely to consider socially deviant behavior to be less justifiable than those primed with low population density. In addition, the measure of ethnocentric attitudes adapted from the Pew Global Attitudes Project (reported in Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Raver, Nishii, Leslie, Lun, Lim, Duan, Almaliach, Ang, Arnadottir, Aycan, Boehnke, Boski, Cabecinhas, Chan, Chhokar, D'Amato, Ferrer, Fischlmayr, Fischer, Fülöp, Georgas, Kashima, Kashima, Kim, Lempereur, Marquez, Othman, Overlaet, Panagiotopoulou, Peltzer, Perez-Florizno, Ponomarenko, Realo, Schei, Schmitt, Smith, Soomro, Szabo, Taveesin, Toyama, Van de Vliert, Vohra, Ward and Yamaguchi2011) showed that people primed with high population density were more likely to agree with such statements as “We should restrict and control entry of people into our country more than we do now”; “When jobs are scarce, employers should give priority to American people over immigrants”; “Our people are not perfect, but our culture is superior to others”; and “Our way of life needs to be protected against foreign influence” than those primed with low population density.

We conducted another experimental study on a different societal threat that we examined naturally in the field, namely, external threats to one's territory, and found similar effects. Participants were randomly assigned to read a school newspaper article, much like the one in the population density study, about a terrorist threat warning system that was being implemented either at one's own university or at another university in a different country. Consistent with our field research on territorial threats (Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Raver, Nishii, Leslie, Lun, Lim, Duan, Almaliach, Ang, Arnadottir, Aycan, Boehnke, Boski, Cabecinhas, Chan, Chhokar, D'Amato, Ferrer, Fischlmayr, Fischer, Fülöp, Georgas, Kashima, Kashima, Kim, Lempereur, Marquez, Othman, Overlaet, Panagiotopoulou, Peltzer, Perez-Florizno, Ponomarenko, Realo, Schei, Schmitt, Smith, Soomro, Szabo, Taveesin, Toyama, Van de Vliert, Vohra, Ward and Yamaguchi2011), we found that individuals who were primed with threats to their own territory were much tighter than those primed with threats to another country's territory; they showed more ethnocentric attitudes and a stronger desire to punish social norm violators. They also showed greater implicit negative attitudes toward a socially marginalized group (i.e., overweight people) than a nonmarginalized group (i.e., slim people). As another example, we have expanded the ecological priming paradigm to examine additional threats (i.e., pathogens) using quasi-experimental designs. We approached individuals who were either about to see the movie Contagion (a movie about the spread of pathogens) or who had just seen the movie outside of movie theaters. As we predicted, people who had just seen the movie Contagion were much tighter; they had more negative reactions to social deviance.

In all, these examples of the ecological priming paradigm show that ecological conditions that form the macro basis of cultural differences across nations can be primed in the laboratory. Although experimental research has its own limitations, it can provide convergent evidence regarding the role of ecology in predicting psychological processes that complements correlational research reported in large-scale cross-cultural field studies.

References

Baumeister, R. F. & Heatherton, T. F. (1996) Self-regulation failure: An overview. Psychological Inquiry 7:115.Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L. & Holcombe Ehrhart, K. (2002) Methodological issues in cross cultural organizational research. In: Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology research methods, ed. Rogelberg, S., pp. 216–41. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., Duan, L., Almaliach, A., Ang, S., Arnadottir, J., Aycan, Z., Boehnke, K., Boski, P., Cabecinhas, R., Chan, D., Chhokar, J., D'Amato, A., Ferrer, M., Fischlmayr, I. C., Fischer, R., Fülöp, M., Georgas, J., Kashima, E. S., Kashima, Y., Kim, K., Lempereur, A., Marquez, P., Othman, R., Overlaet, B., Panagiotopoulou, P., Peltzer, K., Perez-Florizno, L. R., Ponomarenko, L., Realo, A., Schei, V., Schmitt, M., Smith, P. B., Soomro, N., Szabo, E., Taveesin, N., Toyama, M., Van de Vliert, E., Vohra, N., Ward, C., & Yamaguchi, S. (2011) Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science 332:1100–104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T. (1996) The “self digest”: Self-knowledge serving self-regulatory functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71:1062–83.Google Scholar
Lun, J., Gelfand, M. & Mohr, R. (2012) Attitudes toward deviance in tight and loose cultures. Poster presented at the Culture Preconference at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology (SPSP), San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Mortensen, C. R., Becker, D. V., Ackerman, J. M., Neuberg, S. L. & Kenrick, D. T. (2010) Infection breeds reticence: The effects of disease salience on self-perceptions of personality and behavioral avoidance tendencies. Psychological Science 21:440–47.Google Scholar
Neuberg, S. L. & Newsom, J. T. (1993) Personal need for structure: Individual differences in the desire for simple structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65:113–31.Google Scholar
Oyserman, D. & Lee, S. W. S. (2008) Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin 134:311–42.Google Scholar
Snyder, M. & Gangestad, S. (1986) On the nature of self-monitoring: Matters of assessment, matters of validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51:125–39.Google Scholar
Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C. & Goto, S. G. (1991) Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60:649–55.Google Scholar