Obesity is rising globally and in all regions of the world (UN Food and Agriculture Organisation et al. 2017). However, within countries, obesity is unequally distributed, with lower socioeconomic status often associated with higher prevalence of obesity. The inverse social gradient in obesity is not restricted to high-income countries in North America and Europe; it is also found in lower- and middle-income countries across all continents (Popkin & Gordon-Larsen Reference Popkin and Gordon-Larsen2004). Based on associations between food insecurity and high body weight, it has been argued that food insecurity could be a causal factor (Nettle et al. Reference Nettle, Andrews and Bateson2017). Anselme & Güntürkün (A&G) advance a theory compatible with this position, proposing mechanisms that might underlie the relationship between food insecurity and obesity. However, problems arise from the application of their model, based on animal behavior, to a human context.
It cannot be assumed that food security as represented in A&G's model is equivalent to routine measures of human food security. A&G use a quantitative measure relating to availability of food in the environment. However, widely used human measures such as the U.S. Department of Agriculture's Household Security Survey Module (Nord et al. Reference Nord, Andrews and Carlson2009) require individuals to assess their qualitative experiences of having enough food to eat, as well as provide cognitive evaluation of their food situation (e.g., being worried about running out of food). Questions asking about actual (as opposed to perceived) food conditions still require explicit evaluation, introducing potential confounding variables between the objective food situation and the response.
While such measures provide insight into attitudes towards food availability, they do not necessarily reflect the same underlying variable as objective measures of food security in nonhuman models. Although this variable might be characterised as perceived food security for humans, it could reflect other (implicit or explicit) attitudes towards food. Implicit and explicit attitudes are not always consistent within individuals (Rydell & McConnell Reference Rydell and McConnell2006), including in the context of food and eating (Hoefling & Strack Reference Hoefling and Strack2008). Further, respondents’ explicit evaluations of their food conditions are not always consistent with objective measures: A recent U.S. Department of Agriculture study found that experiential measures of food security did not match nutrition-based measures. More than two thirds of households that were undernourished in calories did not report experiencing food insecurity; conversely, around a third of households classified as being adequately nourished reported experiencing mild food insecurity (Broussard & Tandon Reference Broussard and Tandon2016).
Potential discrepancies between quantitative and qualitative measures suggest that theories proposing food security as a causal factor must define their concept of food security and ensure that this is consistent with the evidence relied upon. Even so, A&G's model does not provide a satisfactory explanation for the positive association between human food insecurity and obesity, which applies only to women in high-income countries (Nettle et al. Reference Nettle, Andrews and Bateson2017). This is problematic for A&G's hypothesis, which is based on general psychological mechanisms and thus should be generally applicable. If food insecurity increases behaviors that lead to overeating, the hypothesis predicts that an association between food insecurity and obesity should be observed generally.
To evaluate the relevance of A&G's model to human obesity, we can refer to data that are more directly comparable with food security as it is defined in nonhuman contexts, such as their model of foraging behavior. For example, when food security is assessed by calories available per capita, global trends indicate that food insecurity is decreasing, with daily caloric supply increasing steadily in all regions since 1969 (Alexandratos & Bruinsma Reference Alexandratos and Bruisma2012). This is not consistent with A&G's hypothesis, which predicts that as food insecurity decreases, obesity should also fall. However, human obesity has been rising rapidly at the same time as food insecurity (when quantified in a manner analogous to that used by A&G) is decreasing.
Just as food security in A&G's model may not correspond to the notion of food security in human populations, the assumption that the simple foraging behavior represented in the model is applicable to humans may not be warranted. Humans possess a combination of relatively large brains, social structures, food-sharing behaviors, and complex foraging techniques not observed in other species (Hill et al. Reference Hill, Walker, Bozicevic, Eder, Headland, Hewlett, Hurtado, Marlowe, Wiessner and Wood2011; Schuppli et al. Reference Schuppli, Graber, Isler and van Schaik2016), which may reflect changes in brain metabolism and diet hypothesised to have arisen with the evolution of the genus Homo (Leonard & Robertson Reference Leonard and Robertson1994). Therefore, extrapolation from animal models of foraging to human behavior requires explicit justification, which A&G do not provide.
Today's foodscape differs dramatically from the food environment to which human foraging behaviors were, presumably, well adapted. Consequently, food-related human behavior today may diverge even further from patterns that can be explained by nonhuman models. For example, humans, in common with other species, can discriminate the energy density of foods that occur naturally (i.e., in unprocessed form) in the environment (Gibson & Wardle Reference Gibson and Wardle2003). This ability is important for successful foraging (Brunstrom & Cheon Reference Brunstrom and Cheon2018). Foods occurring naturally during early human evolution were typically low in energy density (<1.75 kcal/g). However in modern, industrialised food environments, common processed foods can be more than twice as energy dense; and when it comes to evaluating foods with an energy density that would have been unusually high historically, human ability to differentiate breaks down (Brunstrom et al. Reference Brunstrom, Drake, Forde and Rogers2018). This suggests that although evolutionary thinking and recourse to psychological mechanisms might be helpful in explaining associations between the food environment and patterns of obesity, characteristics specific to humans and human foodscapes may need to be taken into account.
Although A&G's hypothesis may be useful for explaining foraging behaviors in passerines, the authors’ extrapolation from an animal model to human food behavior is neither justified theoretically nor well supported by the patterns of food supply and obesity observed in human populations. To make progress in revealing potential relationships between food security and obesity, it will be necessary to determine precisely what the standard food security measures in humans represent, taking into account psychological processes alongside socioeconomic factors and characteristics of the food environment.
Obesity is rising globally and in all regions of the world (UN Food and Agriculture Organisation et al. 2017). However, within countries, obesity is unequally distributed, with lower socioeconomic status often associated with higher prevalence of obesity. The inverse social gradient in obesity is not restricted to high-income countries in North America and Europe; it is also found in lower- and middle-income countries across all continents (Popkin & Gordon-Larsen Reference Popkin and Gordon-Larsen2004). Based on associations between food insecurity and high body weight, it has been argued that food insecurity could be a causal factor (Nettle et al. Reference Nettle, Andrews and Bateson2017). Anselme & Güntürkün (A&G) advance a theory compatible with this position, proposing mechanisms that might underlie the relationship between food insecurity and obesity. However, problems arise from the application of their model, based on animal behavior, to a human context.
It cannot be assumed that food security as represented in A&G's model is equivalent to routine measures of human food security. A&G use a quantitative measure relating to availability of food in the environment. However, widely used human measures such as the U.S. Department of Agriculture's Household Security Survey Module (Nord et al. Reference Nord, Andrews and Carlson2009) require individuals to assess their qualitative experiences of having enough food to eat, as well as provide cognitive evaluation of their food situation (e.g., being worried about running out of food). Questions asking about actual (as opposed to perceived) food conditions still require explicit evaluation, introducing potential confounding variables between the objective food situation and the response.
While such measures provide insight into attitudes towards food availability, they do not necessarily reflect the same underlying variable as objective measures of food security in nonhuman models. Although this variable might be characterised as perceived food security for humans, it could reflect other (implicit or explicit) attitudes towards food. Implicit and explicit attitudes are not always consistent within individuals (Rydell & McConnell Reference Rydell and McConnell2006), including in the context of food and eating (Hoefling & Strack Reference Hoefling and Strack2008). Further, respondents’ explicit evaluations of their food conditions are not always consistent with objective measures: A recent U.S. Department of Agriculture study found that experiential measures of food security did not match nutrition-based measures. More than two thirds of households that were undernourished in calories did not report experiencing food insecurity; conversely, around a third of households classified as being adequately nourished reported experiencing mild food insecurity (Broussard & Tandon Reference Broussard and Tandon2016).
Potential discrepancies between quantitative and qualitative measures suggest that theories proposing food security as a causal factor must define their concept of food security and ensure that this is consistent with the evidence relied upon. Even so, A&G's model does not provide a satisfactory explanation for the positive association between human food insecurity and obesity, which applies only to women in high-income countries (Nettle et al. Reference Nettle, Andrews and Bateson2017). This is problematic for A&G's hypothesis, which is based on general psychological mechanisms and thus should be generally applicable. If food insecurity increases behaviors that lead to overeating, the hypothesis predicts that an association between food insecurity and obesity should be observed generally.
To evaluate the relevance of A&G's model to human obesity, we can refer to data that are more directly comparable with food security as it is defined in nonhuman contexts, such as their model of foraging behavior. For example, when food security is assessed by calories available per capita, global trends indicate that food insecurity is decreasing, with daily caloric supply increasing steadily in all regions since 1969 (Alexandratos & Bruinsma Reference Alexandratos and Bruisma2012). This is not consistent with A&G's hypothesis, which predicts that as food insecurity decreases, obesity should also fall. However, human obesity has been rising rapidly at the same time as food insecurity (when quantified in a manner analogous to that used by A&G) is decreasing.
Just as food security in A&G's model may not correspond to the notion of food security in human populations, the assumption that the simple foraging behavior represented in the model is applicable to humans may not be warranted. Humans possess a combination of relatively large brains, social structures, food-sharing behaviors, and complex foraging techniques not observed in other species (Hill et al. Reference Hill, Walker, Bozicevic, Eder, Headland, Hewlett, Hurtado, Marlowe, Wiessner and Wood2011; Schuppli et al. Reference Schuppli, Graber, Isler and van Schaik2016), which may reflect changes in brain metabolism and diet hypothesised to have arisen with the evolution of the genus Homo (Leonard & Robertson Reference Leonard and Robertson1994). Therefore, extrapolation from animal models of foraging to human behavior requires explicit justification, which A&G do not provide.
Today's foodscape differs dramatically from the food environment to which human foraging behaviors were, presumably, well adapted. Consequently, food-related human behavior today may diverge even further from patterns that can be explained by nonhuman models. For example, humans, in common with other species, can discriminate the energy density of foods that occur naturally (i.e., in unprocessed form) in the environment (Gibson & Wardle Reference Gibson and Wardle2003). This ability is important for successful foraging (Brunstrom & Cheon Reference Brunstrom and Cheon2018). Foods occurring naturally during early human evolution were typically low in energy density (<1.75 kcal/g). However in modern, industrialised food environments, common processed foods can be more than twice as energy dense; and when it comes to evaluating foods with an energy density that would have been unusually high historically, human ability to differentiate breaks down (Brunstrom et al. Reference Brunstrom, Drake, Forde and Rogers2018). This suggests that although evolutionary thinking and recourse to psychological mechanisms might be helpful in explaining associations between the food environment and patterns of obesity, characteristics specific to humans and human foodscapes may need to be taken into account.
Although A&G's hypothesis may be useful for explaining foraging behaviors in passerines, the authors’ extrapolation from an animal model to human food behavior is neither justified theoretically nor well supported by the patterns of food supply and obesity observed in human populations. To make progress in revealing potential relationships between food security and obesity, it will be necessary to determine precisely what the standard food security measures in humans represent, taking into account psychological processes alongside socioeconomic factors and characteristics of the food environment.