Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T15:03:09.320Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Food-seeking behavior has complex evolutionary pressures in songbirds: Linking parental foraging to offspring sexual selection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 March 2019

Kate T. Snyder
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37240. katherine.t.snyder@vanderbilt.edunicole.creanza@vanderbilt.edukatetsnyder.comcreanzalab.com
Nicole Creanza
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37240. katherine.t.snyder@vanderbilt.edunicole.creanza@vanderbilt.edukatetsnyder.comcreanzalab.com

Abstract

The target article addresses increased food-seeking behaviors in times of instability, particularly in passerines. We note that food instability might have intergenerational effects on birds: Nutritional stress during development affects song-learning abilities, associating parental foraging with offspring sexual selection. We explore the implications of these compounding selection pressures on food-seeking motivation during breeding, as well as the hormonal underpinnings of these behaviors.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

In their broadly relevant target article, Anselme & Güntürkün (A&G) point out that animals, particularly small passerines, are remarkably responsive to unpredictable food sources, eating and storing more food when access to it is limited or unreliable. The authors convincingly link this behavior to an adaptive evolutionary strategy to prevent death by starvation, which would certainly impose a large fitness cost on animals of reproductive age or younger. Here, we aim to extend the argument of A&G by highlighting another potential fitness cost to food unreliability for small passerines, even if the birds survive. Oscine songbirds, a clade that makes up approximately 80% of passerine species, learn their song from an adult tutor, and females use the quality of a male's song to assess his potential as a mate (Byers & Kroodsma Reference Byers and Kroodsma2009; Catchpole & Slater Reference Catchpole and Slater2003; Nowicki et al. Reference Nowicki, Searcy and Peters2002b). Importantly, reduced access to food early in development is linked to a reduction in song quality: When juvenile birds experience elevated stress levels because of food restriction or high parasite load, they produce lower-quality songs in adulthood, presumably because the stress has impeded development of the neural pathways underlying song learning (MacDonald et al. Reference MacDonald, Kempster, Zanette and MacDougall-Shackleton2006; Schmidt et al. Reference Schmidt, MacDougall-Shackleton, Kubli and MacDougall-Shackleton2014; Spencer et al. Reference Spencer, Buchanan, Leitner, Goldsmith and Catchpole2005).

In addition, stress during development has been linked to reduced fitness, measured by increased mortality and decreased reproductive output (Blas et al. Reference Blas, Bortolotti, Tella, Baos and Marchant2007; Gorman & Nager Reference Gorman and Nager2004). Taken together, these studies suggest that song-learning prowess in birds is linked to their access to food early in life, which, by extension, implies that learned vocalizations can be honest signals of fitness due to the effects of nutritional stress (Buchanan et al. Reference Buchanan, Spencer, Goldsmith and Catchpole2003; Nowicki et al. Reference Nowicki, Searcy and Peters2002a) In this sense, food instability is particularly impactful when passerine birds are raising offspring. If birds raise their chicks in times of limited food availability, the resulting stress can make their offspring less sexually attractive even if food is subsequently becomes abundant and predictable. An increase in foraging behavior in such an environment may protect against this potential decrease in (offspring) reproductive fitness. Thus, adult birds that are particularly sensitive to the motivational effects of food instability, and enhance their food-seeking behaviors in response, could have more reproductively successful offspring. Therefore, in addition to the natural selection pressure on birds to respond to food unpredictability by increasing food-seeking behavior, there could also be a sexual selection pressure on these birds’ offspring, with potential indirect fitness effects on parental behavior. In theory, the offspring with the best songs after seasons of food shortages – potentially those offspring whose parents were most active in food-seeking behaviors in times of instability – would be favored by sexual selection. Thus, the conceptual framework proposed by A&G has broad relevance to future studies in multiple evolutionary contexts.

The target article explores the causal underpinnings with respect to the direct fitness effects of food unpredictability on the individual level. We suggest that these mechanisms may be complicated when considering the intergenerational fitness effects of clutch rearing, modulated by both offspring sexual selection and survival. Adults tending to nestlings must temporarily dramatically increase their food-seeking behavior; food unpredictability during breeding causes an evolutionary dilemma regarding resource allocation to parents versus offspring. To maximize reproductive success of current offspring, parents should allocate resources overwhelmingly toward nest provisioning; however, depending on the parents’ potential for future breeding success, it may be more advantageous to instead allocate all resources toward self-preservation.

As A&G note, food unpredictability is a known stressor; accordingly, the vast majority of studies on the subject of nest provisioning during harsh conditions have focused on the role of corticosterone, the homolog of human cortisol present in birds. In some avian species, experimentally and endogenously elevated baseline corticosterone levels have been correlated with moderate increases in parental investment, even when they increase the time spent away from the nest, consistent with increased foraging (Bonier et al. Reference Bonier, Moore, Martin and Robertson2009; Reference Bonier, Moore and Robertson2011; Kitaysky et al. Reference Kitaysky, Wingfield and Piatt2001; Miller et al. Reference Miller, Vleck and Otis2009; Ouyang et al. Reference Ouyang, Muturi, Quetting and Hau2013) This may be directly tied to demonstrated inverse relationships between levels of corticosterone and prolactin, the hormone linked to maintaining nest-attendant behavior. Paradoxically, reducing the drive to tend to a brood promotes the requisite departure from the nest to forage.

Any reproductive fitness benefit from elevated corticosterone during food instability, however, seems to have an upper limit, as high levels of corticosterone have resulted in reduced provisioning or complete abandonment of offspring (Kidawa et al. Reference Kidawa, Barcikowski and Palme2017; Silverin Reference Silverin1986). When the threat to survival is severe, it is evolutionarily advantageous for an adult to reallocate all resources away from offspring toward self-preservation. In this situation, corticosterone depresses prolactin enough to overcome nest attachment and facilitate nest abandonment (for a review, see Angelier & Chastel Reference Angelier and Chastel2009).

Very few studies in birds have considered the role of dopamine in modulating reproductive behaviors during periods of food scarcity or in general (Angelier et al. Reference Angelier, Wingfield, Tartu and Chastel2016). Based on evidence from other animal taxa, dopamine may have an important and potentially context-dependent function in mediating foraging behaviors as they relate to reproduction. Studies in rodents have shown that dopamine promotes maternal attachment and that dopamine dysfunction can lead to abandonment of pups (for a review, see Dulac et al. Reference Dulac, O'Connell and Wu2014). We wholly agree with A&G's call for studies testing their predictions regarding dopamine and foraging behaviors in small passerines (sect. 6).

To complement the impressive contributions of the target article, we suggest that, in multiple ways, the discussion of food-seeking behavior is complicated by its link to reproductive success, particularly in passerine birds. Behaviorally, food-seeking behaviors in times of resource uncertainty can have an outsized effect on offspring fitness because the ability to learn song in oscine songbirds is affected by nutritional stress during development, thus linking parental foraging to offspring sexual selection. Extending from the authors’ proposed topics of study, it is worth further investigating the interaction of dopamine and corticosterone and its evolutionary links to songbirds’ foraging-mediated reproductive fitness.

References

Angelier, F. & Chastel, O. (2009) Stress, prolactin and parental investment in birds: A review. General and Comparative Endocrinology 163(1–2):142–48.Google Scholar
Angelier, F., Wingfield, J. C., Tartu, S. & Chastel, O. (2016) Does prolactin mediate parental and life-history decisions in response to environmental conditions in birds? A review. Hormones and Behavior 77:1829.Google Scholar
Blas, J., Bortolotti, G. R., Tella, J. L., Baos, R. & Marchant, T. A. (2007) Stress response during development predicts fitness in a wild, long lived vertebrate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104(21):8880–84.Google Scholar
Bonier, F., Moore, I. T., Martin, P. R. & Robertson, R. J. (2009) The relationship between fitness and baseline glucocorticoids in a passerine bird. General and Comparative Endocrinology 163(1–2):208–13.Google Scholar
Bonier, F., Moore, I. T. & Robertson, R. J. (2011) The stress of parenthood? Increased glucocorticoids in birds with experimentally enlarged broods. Biology Letters 7(6):944–46.Google Scholar
Buchanan, K. L., Spencer, K. A., Goldsmith, A. R. & Catchpole, C. K. (2003) Song as an honest signal of past developmental stress in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 270(1520):1149–56.Google Scholar
Byers, B. E. & Kroodsma, D. E. (2009) Female mate choice and songbird song repertoires. Animal Behaviour 77(1):1322.Google Scholar
Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. (2003) Bird song: Biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dulac, C., O'Connell, L. A. & Wu, Z. (2014) Neural control of maternal and paternal behaviors. Science 345(6198):765–70.Google Scholar
Gorman, H. E. & Nager, R. G. (2004) Prenatal developmental conditions have long-term effects on offspring fecundity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 271(1551):1923–28.Google Scholar
Kidawa, D., Barcikowski, M. & Palme, R. (2017) Parent-offspring interactions in a long-lived seabird, the little Auk (Alle alle): Begging and provisioning under simulated stress. Journal of Ornithology 158(1):145–57.Google Scholar
Kitaysky, A. S., Wingfield, J. C. & Piatt, J. F. (2001) Corticosterone facilitates begging and affects resource allocation in the black-legged kittiwake. Behavioral Ecology 12(5):619–25.Google Scholar
MacDonald, I. F., Kempster, B., Zanette, L. & MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A. (2006) Early nutritional stress impairs development of a song-control brain region in both male and female juvenile song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) at the onset of song learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273(1600):2559–64.Google Scholar
Miller, D. A., Vleck, C. M. & Otis, D. L. (2009) Individual variation in baseline and stress-induced corticosterone and prolactin levels predicts parental effort by nesting mourning doves. Hormones and Behavior 56(4):457–64.Google Scholar
Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. A. & Peters, S. (2002a) Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: A review and experimental test of the “nutritional stress hypothesis.” Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 188(11–12):1003–14.Google Scholar
Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. A. & Peters, S. (2002b) Quality of song learning affects female response to male bird song. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 269(1503):1949–54.Google Scholar
Ouyang, J. Q., Muturi, M., Quetting, M. & Hau, M. (2013) Small increases in corticosterone before the breeding season increase parental investment but not fitness in a wild passerine bird. Hormones and Behavior 63(5):776–81.Google Scholar
Schmidt, K. L., MacDougall-Shackleton, E. A., Kubli, S. P. & MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A. (2014) Developmental stress, condition, and birdsong: A case study in song sparrows. Integrative and Comparative Biology 54(4):568–77.Google Scholar
Silverin, B. (1986) Corticosterone-binding proteins and behavioral effects of high plasma levels of corticosterone during the breeding period in the pied flycatcher. General and Comparative Endocrinology 64(1):6774.Google Scholar
Spencer, K. A., Buchanan, K. L., Leitner, S., Goldsmith, A. R. & Catchpole, C. K. (2005) Parasites affect song complexity and neural development in a songbird. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 272(1576):2037–43.Google Scholar