Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-mzp66 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:58:34.420Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Disentangling learning from knowing: Does associative learning ability underlie performances on cognitive test batteries?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 August 2017

Jayden O. van Horik
Affiliation:
Centre for Research in Animal Behaviour, Washington Singer Laboratories, Department of Psychology, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG, United Kingdomjayden.van.horik@gmail.coms.e.g.lea@exeter.ac.ukwww.jayden-van-horik.blogspot.co.ukhttp://people.exeter.ac.uk/SEGLea/
Stephen E. G. Lea
Affiliation:
Centre for Research in Animal Behaviour, Washington Singer Laboratories, Department of Psychology, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Exeter EX4 4QG, United Kingdomjayden.van.horik@gmail.coms.e.g.lea@exeter.ac.ukwww.jayden-van-horik.blogspot.co.ukhttp://people.exeter.ac.uk/SEGLea/

Abstract

Are the mechanisms underlying variations in the performance of animals on cognitive test batteries analogous to those of humans? Differences might result from procedural inconsistencies in test battery design, but also from differences in how animals and humans solve cognitive problems. We suggest differentiating associative-based (learning) from rule-based (knowing) tasks to further our understanding of cognitive evolution across species.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

In the target article, Burkart et al. highlight the importance of identifying variations in domain-general intelligence across species. However, with the exception of mice and possibly primates, there remains little evidence suggesting that variations in domain-general intelligence (g) underlie intraspecific variations in cognitive performance in nonhuman animals. Moreover, such an attribution remains debatable as procedural differences in test battery design may confound interpretations of the underlying mechanism. Our concern is exacerbated where support for a g factor is sparse and limited to studies that predominantly test subjects in the wild. For example, the mechanisms underlying success on test batteries designed to assess performances of birds in the wild (Isden et al. Reference Isden, Panayi, Dingle and Madden2013; Keagy et al. Reference Keagy, Savard and Borgia2011; Shaw et al. Reference Shaw, Boogert, Clayton and Burns2015) bear little resemblance to those effective in tasks presented to non-avian species tested in captivity (Herrmann et al. Reference Herrmann, Hernandez-Lloreda, Call, Hare and Tomasello2010b).

To accurately address whether it is meaningful to talk about domain-general intelligence in animals, it is important that the inherent design of the items within a cognitive test battery accurately capture domain-specific cognitive abilities, independent of procedural factors, and that relevant testing paradigms are used to assess the cognitive performances of subjects in the wild as well as in captivity. Direct comparisons between species are unavoidably difficult as different animals possess different adaptive specialisations; for example, a human cognitive test battery may assess verbal skills whereas nonhuman test batteries cannot. Test batteries, therefore, also need to consider the inherent differences in cognitive processes between species.

Performances on nonhuman cognitive test batteries, particularly those presented to subjects in the wild, require individuals to first interact with a novel apparatus before experiencing its affordances. Accordingly, such test batteries often use tasks that involve trial-and-error learning to quantify subjects' performances and assess their ability to learn to attend to cues based on reward contingencies. For example, subjects may be presented with tasks that assess how quickly they can learn to differentiate rewarded from unrewarded colours, or learn about the spatial location of concealed rewards. Although performances on such tasks are considered to capture domain-specific abilities, success will inevitably also be mediated by fundamental processes of learning that are common to the inherent design of these problems. As a result, an individual may perform well when learning both colour and spatial discrimination problems, not because this individual excels in anything we would want to call intelligence but because it is a relatively rapid learner of all kinds of association, including those involved in the two novel problems. Hence, what seems to be evidence for domain-general intelligence may reflect individual consistency in speed of associative learning, rather than individual consistency in cognition across different domains.

Between-species comparisons may be further confounded because associative learning ability plays a greater role in task performance in animals than it does in humans, and may play a greater role in some nonhuman species than others. Such differences may be particularly pronounced between evolutionarily disparate species such as primates and birds. Pigeons consistently show purely associative solutions to problems that humans, and to some extent nonhuman primates, tend to solve by the use of rules (e.g., Lea & Wills Reference Lea and Wills2008; Lea et al. Reference Lea, Wills, Leaver, Ryan, Bryant and Millar2009; Maes et al. Reference Maes, De Filippo, Inkster, Lea, De Houwer, D'Hooge, Beckers and Wills2015; Meier et al. Reference Meier, Lea and McLaren2016; Smith et al. Reference Smith, Ashby, Berg, Murphy, Spiering, Cook and Grace2011; Reference Smith, Berg, Cook, Murphy, Crossley, Boomer, Spiering, Beran, Church, Ashby and Grace2012; Wills et al. Reference Wills, Lea, Leaver, Osthaus, Ryan, Suret, Bryant, Chapman and Millar2009). In humans, preferential attention to rules may expedite performances on rule-based tasks (Danforth et al. Reference Danforth, Chase, Dolan and Joyce1990), but may also impair responses to experienced contingencies (Fingerman & Levine Reference Fingerman and Levine1974; Hayes et al. Reference Hayes, Brownstein, Zettle, Rosenfarb and Korn1986). Consequently, as different cognitive processes govern the performances of different species on psychometric test batteries, analogous performances between human and nonhuman animals may be difficult to capture.

To overcome these issues, we highlight the importance of differentiating between performances on tasks that require subjects to “learn” to solve a problem, from performances on tasks that assess whether subjects “know” the solution to a problem. We therefore advocate the use not only of associative tasks such as discrimination learning of colour cues that require trial-and-error experience to solve, but also of tasks that require subjects to be trained beforehand to a particular learning criterion, so that their performance on a subsequent novel test or “generalization” condition can be assessed. Such conditions provide a controlled version of the tests of “insightful” or “spontaneous” problem solving that, from the time of Köhler (Reference Köhler1925) on, have often been considered critical in assessing animal intelligence.

Learning tasks are particularly relevant when assessing individual differences in associative performances and may be more relevant when investigating the cognitive performances of nonhuman animals. Binary discriminations involving spatial or colour cues can be presented to subjects and their rates of learning quantified across these different cognitive domains. Although rates of associative learning may differ across domains (Seligman Reference Seligman1970), individual differences in such tasks may still be correlated, leading to a general factor reflecting associative learning ability (hereafter “a”). However, for reliable comparisons, it also remains important to show that subjects' performances are consistent within domains.

Knowing tasks, by contrast, can be designed to assess the more flexible cognitive processes associated with rule-based learning or generalisation and may be more relevant when assessing cognition in humans. Such tasks require training subjects to a predetermined criterion of success to standardise their understanding of the problem, and then presenting subjects with a single test trial using novel cues. Importantly, performances on knowing tasks may highlight whether the mechanism underlying g in humans resembles that which may be found in nonhuman animals.

By incorporating both learning and knowing tasks into cognitive test batteries, we can address whether a general factor of cognitive performance in human and nonhuman animals is better represented by g or a. Distinguishing learning and knowing problems, therefore, provides a measure of individual variation in both domain-specific and domain-general abilities that do not just reflect speed of associative learning, and so can be used to assess whether variation in nonhuman cognitive performance reflects a dimension of general intelligence of the same kind as is thought to underlie human variation.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

JvH was funded by an ERC Consolidator grant awarded to Joah Madden (616474).

References

Danforth, J. S., Chase, P. N., Dolan, M. & Joyce, J. H. (1990) The establishment of stimulus-control by instructions and by differential reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 54:97112. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1990.54-97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fingerman, P. & Levine, M. (1974) Nonlearning: The completeness of the blindness. Journal of Experimental Psychology 102:720–21. doi: 10.1037/h0036096.Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Brownstein, A. J., Zettle, R. D., Rosenfarb, I. & Korn, Z. (1986) Rule-governed behavior and sensitivity to changing consequences of responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 45(3):237–56. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1986.45-237.Google Scholar
Herrmann, E., Hernandez-Lloreda, M. V., Call, J., Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (2010b) The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychological Science 21(1):102–10.Google Scholar
Isden, J., Panayi, C., Dingle, C. & Madden, J. (2013) Performance in cognitive and problem-solving tasks in male spotted bowerbirds does not correlate with mating success. Animal Behaviour 86(4):829–38. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.024.Google Scholar
Keagy, J., Savard, J. F. & Borgia, G. (2011) Complex relationship between multiple measures of cognitive ability and male mating success in satin bowerbirds, Ptilonorhynchus violaceus. Animal Behaviour 81(5):1063–70.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1925) The mentality of apes. Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner.Google Scholar
Lea, S. E. G. & Wills, A. J. (2008) Use of multiple dimensions in learned discriminations. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews 3:115–33. doi: 10.3819/ccbr.2008.30007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lea, S. E. G., Wills, A. J., Leaver, L. A., Ryan, C. M. E., Bryant, C. M. L. & Millar, L. (2009) A comparative analysis of the categorization of multidimensional stimuli: II. Strategic information search in humans (Homo sapiens) but not in pigeons (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Psychology 123(4):406–20. doi: 10.1037/a0016851.Google Scholar
Maes, E., De Filippo, G., Inkster, A. B., Lea, S. E. G., De Houwer, J., D'Hooge, R., Beckers, T. & Wills, A. J. (2015) Feature- versus rule-based generalization in rats, pigeons and humans. Animal Cognition 18(6):1267–84. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0895-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, C., Lea, S. E. G. & McLaren, I. P. L. (2016) A stimulus-location effect in contingency-governed, but not rule-based, discrimination learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 42(2):177–86. doi: 10.1037/xan0000098.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. (1970) On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review 77(5):406–18. doi: 10.1037/h0029790.Google Scholar
Shaw, R. C., Boogert, N. J., Clayton, N. S. & Burns, K. C. (2015) Wild psychometrics: Evidence for “general” cognitive performance in wild New Zealand robins, Petroica longipes. Animal Behaviour 109:101–11. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.001.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Ashby, F. G., Berg, M. E., Murphy, M. S., Spiering, B., Cook, R. G. & Grace, R. C. (2011) Pigeons' categorization may be exclusively nonanalytic. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18(2):414–21. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0047-8.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Berg, M. E., Cook, R. G., Murphy, M. S., Crossley, M. J., Boomer, J., Spiering, B., Beran, M. J., Church, B. A., Ashby, F. G. & Grace, R. C. (2012) Implicit and explicit categorization: A tale of four species. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36(10):2355–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.003.Google Scholar
Wills, A. J., Lea, S. E. G., Leaver, L. A, Osthaus, B., Ryan, C. M. E., Suret, M. B., Bryant, C. M. L., Chapman, S. J. A. & Millar, L. (2009) A comparative analysis of the categorization of multidimensional stimuli: I. Unidimensional classification does not necessarily imply analytic processing; evidence from pigeons (Columba livia), squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis), and humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology 123(4):391405. doi: 10.1037/a0016216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar