Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T23:57:22.089Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Converging on a theory of language through multiple methods

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2017

Mónica González-Márquez
Affiliation:
Institute for English, American and Romance Studies, RWTH-Aachen University, 52056 Aachen, Germany. monica.gonzalez-marquez@ifaar.rwth-aachen.deliane.stroebel@ifaar.rwth-aachen.de
Michele I. Feist
Affiliation:
Department of English, University of Louisiana at Lafayette, Lafayette, LA 70504. feist@louisiana.edu
Liane Ströbel
Affiliation:
Institute for English, American and Romance Studies, RWTH-Aachen University, 52056 Aachen, Germany. monica.gonzalez-marquez@ifaar.rwth-aachen.deliane.stroebel@ifaar.rwth-aachen.de

Abstract

Assuming that linguistic representation has been studied only by linguists using grammaticality judgments, Branigan & Pickering (B&P) present structural priming as a novel alternative. We show that their assumptions are incorrect for cognitive-functional linguistics, exposing converging perspectives on form/meaning pairings between generativists and cognitive-functional linguists that we hope will spark the cross-disciplinary discussion necessary to produce a cognitively plausible model of linguistic representation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

We agree with Branigan & Pickering's (B&P's) general argument that, to understand how language works, we must take into account both representation and processing. However, we take issue with several of the specific arguments put forth by the authors. These are:

  • That acceptability judgments are the only tool available to linguists;

  • That structural priming is the only alternative to acceptability judgments;

  • That linguists and psychologists don't work together to investigate linguistic representation.

These arguments are true only if we assume that linguistic representation is as defined by generative grammar: the result of an encapsulated system in which syntactic structure is divorced from meaning, and meaning is construed in formalist terms (Heim & Kratzer Reference Heim and Kratzer1998; Schiffer Reference Schiffer2015). Vis-à-vis the broader language research community, the generativist view is mischaracterized as enjoying global consensus when, in fact, it has been opposed rigorously for decades by other linguistic traditions – in particular, by researchers within the cognitive-functional tradition. The direct consequence of this myopia is imagining that the limitations built into the generativist paradigm, in fact, are limitations on the entire field.

It is a truism in science that how a question is asked determines the type of answers that can be sought. Cognitive functional linguistics diverges from the generative school by assuming that there is an intimate connection between the form of language and the meaning that it communicates, with the consequence that neither form nor meaning can be studied in isolation. With this assumption comes Lakoff's (Reference Lakoff1990) “Cognitive Commitment”: to ensure that what is posited regarding linguistic structure and representation is in accord with findings from other disciplines regarding the mind and the brain, along with a recognition that the development of cognitively plausible theories of language will require dialog with scholars in neighboring disciplines (cf., Tomasello Reference Tomasello1998; Reference Tomasello2003b). These commitments establish from the outset that understanding linguistic representation must be a multidisciplinary undertaking. The fulfillment of these commitments can be found in the growing number of research articles and books investigating linguistic representation using dozens of methodologies either borrowed from, or developed in conjunction with, multiple neighboring disciplines. These commitments are so important that there is a long-standing workshop series, the Empirical Methods in Cognitive Linguistics Workshops, whose primary goal is to bring together researchers from a wide variety of fields who seek to strengthen their methodological repertoires for the study of language and cognition.

As a field, cognitive-functional linguistics has brought together the following methods, among others:

This short list of methods used in the study of linguistic representation is representative of the massive collective efforts that have yielded a view of language in which:

  1. 1. Prototype structures and schemas are evident in representational categories at multiple linguistic levels, including semantic (e.g., Lakoff Reference Lakoff1987; Spivey et al. Reference Spivey, Richardson, Gonzalez-Marquez, Zwaan and Pecher2004), morphological (Bybee & Moder Reference Bybee and Moder1983), and phonological (Bybee Reference Bybee1994);

  2. 2. There is no clear separation between levels of linguistic representation (Fillmore et al. Reference Fillmore, Kay and O'Connor1988; Goldberg Reference Goldberg2003);

  3. 3. Meaning is based on usage and experience, resulting in the existence of highly interconnected conceptual networks (Fillmore Reference Fillmore1976; Ströbel Reference Ströbel2016);

  4. 4. Meaning is context-dependent and dynamically created online, fed by multiple sources of available information (Feist & Duffy Reference Feist and Duffy2015; Lupyan & Casasanto Reference Lupyan and Casasanto2015).

There is a gentle irony to the conclusion B&P reach at the end of their article. Their chosen method, structural priming, led them away from the classical generativist proposal to conclude that structure and meaning cannot be studied in isolation, making their views ultimately consistent with the cognitive-functional tradition. This convergence suggests a way forward: that we all come to the table with our disparate lines of research to create a cognitively plausible model of linguistic representation. A similar suggestion was made by Jackendoff (Reference Jackendoff2002) when he attempted to create a cornerstone for the convergence of linguistic theory. Unfortunately, little came of his efforts, because of, in part, the sparseness of outreach to researchers in the cognitive-functional tradition (cf., Spivey & Gonzalez-Marquez Reference Spivey and González-Márquez2003). In reintroducing this issue, we hope to spark conversations that will help advance our understanding of the representation and processing of language.

In sum, we don't take issue with the view of language B&P ultimately propose; in fact, we tend to agree with it. Our point is to underline that this view is not novel, and that in integrating these findings with other proposals and findings, we can broaden our understanding of the linguistic representations that underlie speakers' capabilities.

References

Bergen, B. & Coulson, S. (2006) Frame-shifting humor in simulation-based language understanding. IEEE Intelligent Systems 21(2):5962.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. L. (1994) A view of phonology from a cognitive and functional perspective. Cognitive Linguistics 5(4):285305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. L. & Moder, C. L. (1983) Morphological classes as natural categories. Language 59:251–89.Google Scholar
Coulson, S. & Van Petten, C. (2002) Conceptual integration and metaphor: An event-related potential study. Memory & Cognition 30:958–68.Google Scholar
Croft, W. & Poole, K. T. (2008) Inferring universals from grammatical variation: Multidimensional scaling for typological analysis. Theoretical Linguistics 34:137.Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (2014) Words that go together: Measuring individual differences in native speakers' knowledge of collocations. The Mental Lexicon 9(3):401–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Divjak, D. S. & Arppe, A. (2013) Extracting prototypes from exemplars What can corpus data tell us about concept representation? Cognitive Linguistics 24(2):221–74.Google Scholar
Feist, M. I. (2008) Space between languages. Cognitive Science 32(7):1177–99.Google Scholar
Feist, M. I. (2010) Inside in and on: Typological and psycholinguistic perspectives. In: Language, Cognition, and Space, ed. Evans, V. & Chilton, P., pp. 95114. Equinox.Google Scholar
Feist, M. I. (2013) Codability and cost in the naming of motion events. Rivista Italiana di Filosofia del Linguaggio 7(3):4561.Google Scholar
Feist, M. I. & Duffy, S. E. (2015) Moving beyond “Next Wednesday”: The interplay of lexical semantics and constructional meaning in an ambiguous metaphoric statement. Cognitive Linguistics 26(4):633–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, C. J. (1976) Frame semantics and the nature of language. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280:2032.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. J., Kay, P. & O'Connor, M. C. (1988) Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let alone . Language 64(3):501–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2003) Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(5):219–24. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00080-9 Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M. (2008) Frequency vs. iconicity in explaining grammatical asymmetries. Cognitive Linguistics 19:133.Google Scholar
Heim, I. & Kratzer, A. (1998) Semantics in generative grammar. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Huette, S., Winter, B., Matlock, T., Ardell, D. & Spivey, M. J. (2014) Eye movements during listening reveal spontaneous grammatical processing. Frontiers in Psychology 5:410. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00410.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2002) Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1990) The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reason based on image-schemas? Cognitive Linguistics 1(1):3974.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. & Meira, S. (2003) “Natural concepts” in the spatial topological domain – Adpositional meanings in crosslinguistic perspective: An exercise in semantic typology. Language 79:485516.Google Scholar
Liu, N. & Bergen, B. (2016) When do language comprehenders mentally simulate locations? Cognitive Linguistics 27(2):181203.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. (2012) Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: The label feedback hypothesis. Frontiers in Cognition 3(54):113. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00054.Google Scholar
Lupyan, G. & Casasanto, D. (2015) Meaningless words promote meaningful categorization. Language and Cognition 7(2):167–93.Google Scholar
Núñez, R. & Sweetser, E. (2006) With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science 30(3):401–50.Google Scholar
Perry, L. & Lupyan, G. (2014) The role of language in multi-dimensional categorization: Evidence from transcranial direct current stimulation and exposure to verbal labels. Brain and Language 135:6672.Google Scholar
Saj, A., Fuhrman, O., Vuilleumier, P. & Boroditsky, L. (2014) Patients with left spatial neglect also neglect the “left side” of time. Psychological Science 25(1):207–14.Google Scholar
Schiffer, S. (2015) Meaning and formal semantics in generative grammar. Erkenntnis 80(1):6187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M. & González-Márquez, M. (2003) Rescuing generative linguistics: Too little, too late? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26(06):690–1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M., Richardson, D. & Gonzalez-Marquez, M. (2004) On the perceptual-motor and image-schematic underpinnings of real-time language processing. In: The grounding of cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking, ed. Zwaan, R. & Pecher, D., pp. 246–81. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ströbel, L., ed. (2016) Sensory-motor concepts in language & cognition. Duesseldorf University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1998) The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure, vol. 1. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003b) The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure, vol. 2. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van Petten, C., Coulson, S., Plante, E., Rubin, S. & Parks, M. (1999) Timecourse of word identification and semantic integration in spoken language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25(2):394417.Google Scholar
Walker, E., Bergen, B. & Núñez, R. (2013) Investigating spatial axis recruitment in temporal reckoning through acoustic stimuli and non-spatial responses. Center for Research in Language Technical Report, University of California, San Diego 25:110.Google Scholar
Winawer, J., Huk, A. & Boroditsky, L. (2008) A motion aftereffect from still photographs depicting motion. Psychological Science 19(3):276–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, B. & Matlock, T. (2013) Making judgments based on similarity and proximity. Metaphor & Symbol 28:114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoon, J. & Gries, S. Th., eds. (2016) Corpus-based approaches to construction grammar. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar