Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-l4dxg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T02:50:18.379Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

If priming is graded rather than all-or-none, can reactivating abstract structures be the underlying mechanism?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2017

Laurie Beth Feldman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, State University of New York at Albany, Albany, NY 12222. lfeldman@albany.edu
Petar Milin
Affiliation:
Department of Journalism Studies, The University of Sheffield, Sheffield S1 4DT, United Kingdom. p.milin@sheffield.ac.uk

Abstract

In our commentary on Branigan & Pickering (B&P), we start by arguing that the authors implicitly adopt several assumptions, the consequence of which is to make further claims necessary and/or sufficient. Crucially, the authors assume the existence of discrete units at various levels of linguistic granularity that then must be operated upon by combinatorial mechanisms and rules (i.e., decomposition/recomposition). They further argue that structural priming provides a powerful tool to study abstract, structural representations. We provide evidence that priming effects in production are characterized better as graded than as all-or-none and that priming need not arise from a mechanism that (re)activates a shared but abstract internal structure.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Branigan & Pickering (B&P) work within a framework that posits discrete linguistic units at various levels of granularity that must be operated upon by combinatorial mechanisms and rules (i.e., decomposition/recomposition). They argue that structural priming provides a powerful tool to study abstract, structural representations, but there are alternatives that better embrace the broader communicative function of language (Baayen et al. Reference Baayen, Milin and Ramscar2016a; Reference Baayen, Shaoul, Willits and Ramscar2016b). However, even within their framework, the experimental priming methodology that B&P depend on for their argument is more nuanced than what they have explored with choice between two syntactic structures as their measure of behavior. They assert that abstract structural processes can be studied independently from the contributions of individual words, because priming arises even when words do not reappear. Often, however, priming effects are not all-or-none, and effect sizes depend on what recurs. Thus, systematically graded priming outcomes challenge the descriptive adequacy of B&P's theorizing about how lexical and syntactic knowledge interact. In contrast to B&P, we assert that priming effects in production are more informative when characterized as graded than as all-or-none and that priming need not arise from a mechanism that (re)activates a shared but abstract internal structure. We present examples from our own work that show systematic variation among the “structures” that generate priming, eschew a division between representation and process, and exploit rather than tolerate differences among words.

In a single-word inflected production task, the verb stem constitutes the structure that recurs, and the requisite production – an inflected verb form – is specified by instruction rather than by a sentence context. Admittedly, this version of structural priming is severely constrained. Nonetheless, we have demonstrated that reaction time (RT) differences between regular ed and ing productions differ significantly more when primed by a written stem than by a drawing of the action depicted by the stem (Feldman et al. Reference Feldman, Milin and Baayen2013a; Reference Feldman, Milin and Baayen2013b). These prime modality (drawing, word) differences impact the magnitude of priming rather than its presence or absence. Relative differences such as these constrain the abstractness of lemma representations and impose limitations on the independence of structural priming from lexical contributions.

In addition, differences in production times between verbs with high and low lemma frequency are larger when generating progressive (ing) than past tense (ed) inflected forms of regular verbs. These reliable differences (RT, accuracy) between various inflected word forms of the same verbs pose a challenge to an account based only on binding between a constituent structure rule and a lexical representation without reference to “features like tense, number or aspect.”

Admittedly, we confine structural priming to inflected word forms rather than sentential syntax. We emphasize, however, that this is a useful trade-off, in that the task generates RT as well as accuracy data, and having both eliminates some of the challenges that typically arise with the dichotomous data generated by the classical structural-choice priming task. At a minimum, graded priming effects across variants of the structural priming methodology highlight the potential interdependence between lexical contributions and syntactic processing, and challenge the descriptive adequacy of the B&P account of structural priming.

When verbs recur in prime and target structures, there is a benefit to production termed a lexical boost (Cleland & Pickering Reference Cleland and Pickering2003). The existence of the lexical boost argues against a purely structural account of priming in which lexical information fails to make contact with the central syntactic component. Nonetheless, B&P's structural priming account fails to anticipate graded, systematic lexical contributions due to differences among words.

In addition to manipulating degree of lexical specification (drawing, word) while matching output at production, we examined inflectional regularity. Lower accuracy for irregularly than for regularly inflected past-tense forms arose when generating a past-tense inflection from a verb stem but not from a drawing of the same action. Here, negative priming between input and output structures is possible when lexical information is specified orthographically but not by a drawing. Interactions of prime modality with regularity – such that a regularity effect manifests itself with productions from the written stem but not from a drawing of that same action – challenge the claim that the lexical boost in production derives simply from repetition of a particular lemma (e.g., dive) that is unspecified for shared features such as tense, number, or aspect (Pickering & Branigan Reference Pickering and Branigan1998). Productions that share a lemma and convey the same action but prime differently depending on the availability of the stem, set limits on the abstractness of the representations that purportedly produce structural priming.

Absent from the B&P account of structural priming, even when enhanced by lexical boost, is an appreciation of the communicative function of language and the requisite system's priority for reducing uncertainty and exploiting typicality (cf., Ramscar et al. Reference Ramscar, Dye and McCauley2013). Elsewhere, we have argued for the benefits of discrimination-based predictors in priming over more conventional lexical-distributional predictors (Milin et al. Reference Milin, Feldman, Ramscar, Hendrix and Baayen2017). Key is that priming reflects not only the “similarities” between prime and target, but also the similarities of the prime and the target to other words. Surely an appreciation of systematic differences in the probability distributions of the various alternatives deserves consideration such that all structural matches are not equivalent. Similarly, anticipating variation with respect to particular lexical entries and the syntactic relations in which they potentially participate by introducing prime and target items as random effects in analyses enriches insights into any variant of priming (Milin et al. Reference Milin, Feldman, Ramscar, Hendrix and Baayen2017).

While structural priming may provide a useful method of investigating linguistic knowledge with significant benefits over acceptability judgments, the nuances of stem-based (as distinguished from drawing-based) priming effects as well as a more functional characterization of syntactic patterning leads us to question whether the structural priming effect that B&P endorse is best characterized in terms of the presence or absence of (re)activation of purely abstract syntactic representations.

References

Baayen, R. H., Milin, P. & Ramscar, M. (2016a) Frequency in lexical processing. Aphasiology 30(11):1174–220.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Shaoul, C., Willits, J. & Ramscar, M. (2016b) Comprehension without segmentation: A proof of concept with naive discrimination learning. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience. 31(1):106–28Google Scholar
Cleland, A. A. & Pickering, M. J. (2003) The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure. Journal of Memory and Language 49(2):214–30. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00060-3.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Milin, P. & Baayen, R. H. (2013a) Frequency and regularity effects do not interact in verb production. Presentation at European Society for Cognitive Psychology. Marseilles, France. August 30, 2013.Google Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Milin, P. & Baayen, R. H. (2013b) Frequency and regularity effects do not interact in verb production: A challenge to dual mechanism accounts of inflectional processing. Poster presented at Architectures and Mechanisms of Language Processing, Budapest, Hungary. August 22, 2013.Google Scholar
Milin, P., Feldman, L. B., Ramscar, M., Hendrix, P. & Baayen, R. H. (2017) Discrimination in lexical decision. PLoS ONE 12(2):e0171935. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171935.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. & Branigan, H. P. (1998) The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language 39(4):633–51.Google Scholar
Ramscar, M., Dye, M. & McCauley, S. M. (2013) Error and expectation in language learning: The curious absence of mouses in adult speech. Language 89(4):760–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar