Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T14:51:11.284Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reputational concerns as a general determinant of group functioning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 October 2016

Nadira S. Faber
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3UD, United Kingdomnadira.faber@psy.ox.ac.uknadirafaber.com Oxford Martin School, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3BD, United Kingdomjulian.savulescu@philosophy.ox.ac.uk Oxford Uehiro Centre for Practical Ethics, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 1PT, United Kingdomwww.practicalethics.ox.ac.uk
Julian Savulescu
Affiliation:
Oxford Martin School, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3BD, United Kingdomjulian.savulescu@philosophy.ox.ac.uk Oxford Uehiro Centre for Practical Ethics, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 1PT, United Kingdomwww.practicalethics.ox.ac.uk
Paul A. M. Van Lange
Affiliation:
Department of Social and Organizational Psychology, VU University Amsterdam, 1081 BT Amsterdam, Netherlands.pam.van.lange@psy.vu.nlwww.paulvanlange.com

Abstract

To understand a group's (dys)functionality, we propose focusing on its members' concerns for their reputation. The examples of prosocial behavior and information exchange in decision-making groups illustrate that empirical evidence directly or indirectly suggests that reputational concerns play a central role in groups. We argue that our conceptualization fulfills criteria for a good theory: enhancing understanding, abstraction, testability, and applicability.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

General determinants of group functioning

When does a group do better than we would expect from the sum of its individual members, when worse? This is the ultimate question many researchers ask, whether they are concerned with group performance, information exchange and decision-making in groups, or prosocial and antisocial behavior (cf., Allport Reference Allport1962). A major achievement of Baumeister et al.'s stimulating paper is that they bridge these fields that too often are regarded as distinct, instead of capitalizing on each other's knowledge.

Baumeister et al. suggest what they call “differentiation of selves” as a general determinant for the functioning of groups in different contexts: The more individual group members become differentiated selves, the better the group functions. Baumeister et al. offer a variety of factors that make selves differentiated, such as group members having distinct roles and specialization; being individually identifiable, accountable and responsible; getting individual rewards; and being in competition with each other. Yet, important questions remain. How do these factors differ in effectiveness across different group contexts? What are the psychological mechanisms underlying their effects?

Clearly, Baumeister et al.'s framework is a useful step toward a greater understanding of groups. Yet, we propose an alternative conceptualization as a general determinant for group functioning: reputational concerns. Our conceptualization might do equally well in terms of (a) enhancing understanding and (b) applicability to important social settings. However, it might do even better in terms of (c) abstraction (i.e., parsimony, the notion of explaining a lot with a little) and (d) testability – all of which have been emphasized as criteria for a good psychological theory (Van Lange Reference Van Lange2013).

Reputational concerns

Anthropologist Ralph Linton (Reference Linton1945, p. 9) wrote that “there is very little organized human behavior” which is not to some degree directed toward fulfilling the “need for eliciting favorable responses from others.” Indeed, concerns about one's own reputation have since, under different terms, been recognized as a prime human motive in the biological, behavioral, and social sciences. Baumeister (Reference Baumeister1982) himself suggested the relevance of what he called “self-presentational motives” for different social phenomena. More recent empirical evidence indicates the importance of reputational concerns in groups – both explicitly and implicitly.

First, in the broad domain of prosocial behavior, the role of reputational concerns is explicitly recognized by different research traditions. In behavioral economics, prosocial behavior is seen as partly driven by the “image” or “signaling” motivation that one is a good person (Ariely et al. Reference Ariely, Bracha and Meier2009). Evolutionary accounts argue that concerns to build a reputation as a trustworthy cooperation partner enable indirect reciprocal helping within a group (Nowak & Sigmund Reference Nowak and Sigmund2005). Also, in social psychology some have demonstrated that a motivation to present oneself in a good light is decisive for prosociality, for example in research on “moral hypocrisy” (Batson et al. Reference Batson, Kobrynowicz, Dinnerstein, Kampf and Wilson1997).

Second, in the field of information exchange in decision-making groups reputational concerns are addressed rather implicitly, even though behavior in such situations is recognized as serving different individual and group goals (De Dreu et al. Reference De Dreu, Nijstad and van Knippenberg2008). It has been argued, for example, that group members aim to gain status or influence within the group (Wittenbaum et al. Reference Wittenbaum, Hollingshead and Botero2004). Empirical research, although not directly addressing this, also implies reputational concerns are a motive in information exchange. Group members' communication is driven by their wish to be seen as having comprehensible reasoning (Faulmüller et al. Reference Faulmüller, Mojzisch, Kerschreiter and Schulz-Hardt2012), and they tend to adjust the information they share so that they are perceived as more competent (Mojzisch et al. Reference Mojzisch, Kerschreiter, Faulmüller, Vogelgesang and Schulz-Hardt2014), both leading to biased information exchange. When group members are familiar with one another and hence have to worry less about their social acceptance within the group, communication becomes less biased (Gruenfeld et al. Reference Gruenfeld, Mannix, Williams and Neale1996).

These examples illustrate that empirical evidence either directly shows a central role of reputational concerns in explaining group functioning or can be interpreted in that light. Many other examples can be found, ranging from Hollander's (Reference Hollander1958) “idiosyncrasy credit” – the idea that status can be earned by conforming to the group's expectations and used up by deviant behavior – to empirical evidence that “impression management” could underlie the increased effort weak performers show in group settings (Kerr et al. Reference Kerr, Messé, Park and Sambolec2005). In sum, many group phenomena seem to be driven at least partly by the attempt to be seen favorably by others.

A focus on reputational concerns allows a more flexible view on group functioning across time than the two sequential steps Baumeister et al. advocate: first cohesive identification, later differentiation. Reputational concerns can explain why the same person within the same group can contribute both positively and negatively to this group's functionality – not depending on the group's long-term development but varying with contextual factors. For example, a timid team member in a company might not mention any ideas at the team's brainstorming session out of fear of appearing stupid (reputational concerns as impairment for group functioning). But within the same meeting, this person might contribute generously to the team's collection of charitable donations out of fear of appearing stingy (reputational concerns as enhancement for group functioning). And reputational concerns can partly explain why moderators of individual behavior can have different effects in groups (Faber et al. Reference Faber, Häusser and Kerr2015).

Practical aspects

For these theoretical and empirical reasons, we propose reputational concerns as a general determinant of group functioning in different contexts. Furthermore, we argue that such a focus fulfills the two more practical criteria for a good theory mentioned above: testability and applicability. In empirical research, reputational concerns usually are easy to operationalize. And they can be addressed in interventions ranging from small group to state level. Letting citizens develop their own cooperative norms in local communities (Van Lange et al. Reference Van Lange, Joireman, Parks and Van Dijk2013) or implementing policy interventions that nudge behavior that benefits the individual or the whole group (Thaler & Sunstein Reference Thaler and Sunstein2008) may serve as examples.

We do not argue that reputational concerns are sufficient to explain all cases of groups being more or less than the sum of their members. But we are confident this motive deserves broad theoretical, empirical, and practical consideration.

References

Allport, F. H. (1962) A structuronomic conception of behavior: Individual and collective. I. Structural theory and the master problem of social psychology. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 64:330. doi: 10.1037/h0043563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ariely, D., Bracha, A. & Meier, S. (2009) Doing good or doing well? Image motivation and monetary incentives in behaving prosocially. The American Economic Review 99:544–55. doi: : 10.1257/aer.99.1.544.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., Kobrynowicz, D., Dinnerstein, J. L., Kampf, H. C. & Wilson, A. D. (1997) In a very different voice: Unmasking moral hypocrisy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72:1335–48. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (1982) A self-presentational view of social phenomena. Psychological Bulletin 91:326. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.91.1.3.Google Scholar
De Dreu, C. K. W., Nijstad, B. A. & van Knippenberg, D. (2008) Motivated information processing in group judgment and decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Review 12(1):2249. doi: 10.1177/1088868307304092.Google Scholar
Faber, N. S., Häusser, J. A. & Kerr, N. L. (2015) Sleep deprivation impairs and caffeine enhances my performance, but not always our performance: How acting in a group can change the effects of impairments and enhancements. Personality and Social Psychology Review. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/1088868315609487.Google Scholar
Faulmüller, N., Mojzisch, A., Kerschreiter, R. & Schulz-Hardt, S. (2012) Do you want to convince me or to be understood? Preference-consistent information sharing and its motivational determinants. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 38:1684–96. doi: 10.1177/0146167212458707.Google Scholar
Gruenfeld, D. H., Mannix, E. A., Williams, K. Y. & Neale, M. A. (1996) Group composition and decision making: How member familiarity and information distribution affect process and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 67:115. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1996.0061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollander, E. (1958) Conformity, status, and idiosyncrasy credit. Psychological Review 65:117–27. doi: 10.1037/h0042501.Google Scholar
Kerr, N. L., Messé, L. M., Park, E. S. & Sambolec, E. (2005) Identifiability, performance feedback and the Köhler effect. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 8:375–90. doi: 10.1177/1368430205056466.Google Scholar
Linton, R. (1945) The cultural background of personality. Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
Mojzisch, A., Kerschreiter, R., Faulmüller, N., Vogelgesang, F. & Schulz-Hardt, S. (2014) The consistency principle in interpersonal communication: Consequences of preference confirmation and disconfirmation in collective decision-making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 106:961–77. doi: 10.1037/a0036338.Google ScholarPubMed
Nowak, M. A. & Sigmund, K. (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature 437:1291–98. doi: 10.1038/nature04131.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. H. & Sunstein, C. R. (2008) Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Van Lange, P. A. M. (2013) What we should expect from theories in social psychology: Truth, abstraction, progress, and applicability as standards (TAPAS). Personality and Social Psychology Review 17:4055. doi: 10.1177/1088868312453088.Google Scholar
Van Lange, P. A. M., Joireman, J., Parks, C. D. & Van Dijk, E. (2013) The psychology of social dilemmas: A review. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 120:125–41. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.11.003.Google Scholar
Wittenbaum, G. M., Hollingshead, A. B. & Botero, I. C. (2004) From cooperative to motivated information sharing in groups: Moving beyond the hidden profile paradigm. Communication Monographs 71:286310. doi: 10.1080/0363452042000299894.Google Scholar