Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T11:20:01.412Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

To be or not to be emotionally aware and socially motivated: How alexithymia impacts autism spectrum disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 July 2019

Luigi Pastore
Affiliation:
Department of Education, Psychology, Communication, University of Bari, 70121 Bari, Italy. luigi.pastore@uniba.ithttps://www.researchgate.net/profile/Luigi_Pastore
Sara Dellantonio
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, University of Trento, 38068 Rovereto, Italy. sara.dellantonio@unitn.itgianluca.esposito@unitn.ithttps://www.researchgate.net/profile/Sara_Dellantoniohttp://abp.dipsco.unitn.it
Claudio Mulatti
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialisation, University of Padua, 35131 Padua, Italy. claudio.mulatti@unipd.ithttp://dpss.psy.unipd.it
Gianluca Esposito
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Cognitive Sciences, University of Trento, 38068 Rovereto, Italy. sara.dellantonio@unitn.itgianluca.esposito@unitn.ithttps://www.researchgate.net/profile/Sara_Dellantoniohttp://abp.dipsco.unitn.it Social & Affiliative Neuroscience Lab, Psychology Program, Nanyang Technological University, HSS-04-14, Singapore. gianluca.esposito@ntu.edu.sghttps://blogs.ntu.edu.sg/sanlab/

Abstract

Autism often co-occurs with alexithymia, a condition characterized by no or diminished awareness of emotions that significantly impacts an individual's social relationships. We investigate how the social motivation of autistics would be eroded by comorbidity with alexithymia and why this diminished motivation would be difficult for non-autistic people to perceive and reciprocate.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Autism is a condition that often co-occurs with alexithymia: Epidemiologically, we have evidence that about 50% of autistics exceed the cutoff score for alexithymia (Berthoz & Hill Reference Berthoz and Hill2005; Hill et al. Reference Hill, Berthoz and Frith2004; Samson et al. Reference Samson, Huber and Gross2012). Alexithymics lack or experience diminished awareness of their emotions. In emotionally salient situations, they do not experience emotions, but only the bodily sensations that typically occur during emotional episodes such as increased heart rate, blood pressure, muscular tension, visceral changes, and so forth. Because they confuse emotions with bodily sensations, they are unable to identify what they feel and to express it verbally. The possible comorbidity of alexithymia and autism is thus highly relevant: The “alexithymia hypothesis” proposes that the emotional impairment exhibited by autistics is due to alexithymia rather than features of autism per se (Bird & Cook Reference Bird and Cook2013). Whether or not we support this hypothesis, it is undeniable that the emotional deficits exhibited by autistics and alexithymics are highly similar (Fitzgerald & Bellgrove Reference Fitzgerald and Bellgrove2006). Thus, knowledge we have already obtained regarding the clinical presentation of alexithymia may be effectively transferred to help us understand autism, especially high-functioning autism, because the clinical assessment of alexithymic traits is based on self-report questionnaires that require a fairly high level of linguistic competence (Wotschack & Klann-Delius Reference Wotschack and Klann-Delius2013).

Alexithymia has an extremely significant impact on an individual's attitudes and behaviors, as well as on his or her social life and social relationships (Timoney & Holder Reference Timoney and Holder2013). In spite of this relevance, Jaswal & Akhtar (J&A)’s study on the social motivation of autistics (which largely relies on autistics’ verbal self-report and thus applies primarily to high-functioning patients) does not take into consideration a possible comorbidity with alexithymia or the issue of how autistics experience their own and others’ emotions. Yet these appear to be key elements in understanding both (1) the social motivation of autistics and (2) the reason why this motivation is neither perceived nor easily reciprocated by non-autistic people.

  1. 1. An awareness of our emotions, as well as a capacity to identify and understand them, is a precondition for being able to regulate them; this is the reason why alexithymics have severe difficulties in emotion regulation (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Bagby and Parker1997). They tend to suppress the expression of what they feel rather than deploying some cognitive strategy of reappraisal (Swart et al. Reference Swart, Kortekaas and Aleman2009). This applies also to autistics who are, even in mildly frustrating situations, less able than non-autistics to use appropriate emotion regulation strategies (Samson et al. Reference Samson, Huber and Gross2012). At the same time, the capacity to regulate emotions is essential to fully participate in social life, which – under normal, uncontrolled circumstances – is characterized by a number of potentially destabilizing emotional contexts. Indeed, dysfunction of emotional regulation is very stressful and can lead to perceiving everyday sociality as unpleasant, thereby reducing social motivation (Vanheule et al. Reference Vanheule, Desmet, Megank and Bogaerts2007).

  2. 2. People who lack (full) awareness of their emotions also have difficulty showing emotions and sharing them with others through their own expression and behavior. They also struggle to recognize these emotions in others and to react appropriately to others’ emotional states through empathy. An incapacity to tune into the emotions of others can lead to a disconnection between autistics (alexithymics) and non-autistics that cannot be easily overcome. Non-autistics have difficulty understanding and relating to autistics, because the spontaneous modality through which non-autistics connect with others requires that these others be “mentally aware” of the dynamics of the relationship: of what is emotionally going on. If this spontaneous mindreading does not work (Gallagher & Varga Reference Gallagher and Varga2015), non-autistic people may perceive autistics to be socially uninterested and thus become less interested in relating to them.

J&A's discussion of how autistics tend to engage in eye contact much less frequently than non-autistics is an excellent example of the points made in (1) and (2): Eye contact shows that attention is shared; for the non-clinical population, it is an essential element in perceiving others to be mentally aware and engaged in a relationship. Avoiding eye contact is thus naturally interpreted as a sign of lack of interest in a relationship. The authors suggest, however, that autistics do not avoid eye contact because they lack social motivation, but rather, because they seek to “reduce or avoid stress” (p. 13). Indeed, evidence from people suffering from both alexithymia and autism shows that eye contact arouses emotions, which generate anxiety, discomfort, and avoidance (Bird & Richardson Reference Bird and Richardson2011; Bird et al. Reference Bird, Silani, Brindley, White, Frith and Singer2010; Fujiwara Reference Fujiwara2018; Hadjikhani et al. Reference Hadjikhani, Åsberg Johnels, Zürcher, Lassalle, Guillon, Hippolyte, Billstedt, Ward, Lemonnier and Gillberg2017). This leads to an impasse: For typically developing individuals, mutual gaze is an essential means for tuning into non-verbal participation in a relationship; but while autistics who are also alexithymics also experience the power of mutual gaze, they cannot deal with the emotions it arouses, and therefore they avoid it.

In our view, this suggests that, even though autistics are socially motivated, social interactions might be much less rewarding for them than they are for non-autistics. The problem of how to make such interactions less difficult, by avoiding conditions that may potentially lead to emotional dysregulation, still remains unsolved. On a clinical level, it also indicates the need to train non-autistics as well as autistics to interact with each other. In fact, given that certain behaviors typical of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are naturally interpreted by non-autistics as signs of avoidance in a relationship, non-autistics must be taught to systematically ignore these signals. On the other hand, given that autistics have emotion recognition deficits (possibly pursuant to their alexithymia – Oakley et al. Reference Oakley, Brewer, Bird and Catmur2016), they must be trained to understand the emotional expressions of others by using alternative, externally focused cognitive means. One such strategy is the use of explicit instructions to infer the mental states of others in emotionally salient situations and to respond guided by adhering to prescriptions for specific patterns of behavior (Lumley et al. Reference Lumley, Neely and Burger2007). More generally, a deeper assessment of the interaction between alexithymia and ASD may provide an opportunity to test J&A's hypothesis and may also lead to suggestions on how to treat autistics (Poquérusse et al. Reference Poquérusse, Pastore, Dellantonio and Esposito2018).

References

Berthoz, S. & Hill, E. L. (2005) The validity of using self-reports to assess emotion regulation abilities in adult with autism spectrum disorder. European Psychiatry 20:291–98.Google Scholar
Bird, G. & Cook, R. (2013) Mixed emotions: The contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Translational Psychiatry 3:e285. doi:10.1038/tp.2013.61.Google Scholar
Bird, R. & Richardson, D.C. (2011) The role of alexithymia in reduced eye-fixation in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 41:556–64.Google Scholar
Bird, R., Silani, G., Brindley, G., White, S., Frith, U. & Singer, T. (2010) Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain 133(5):1515–25.Google Scholar
Fitzgerald, M. & Bellgrove, M. A., (2006) The overlap between alexithymia and Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36(4):573–76. 10.1007/s10803-006-0096-z.Google Scholar
Fujiwara, E. (2018) Looking at the eyes interferes with facial emotion recognition in alexithymia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 127(6):571–77. doi:10.1037/abn0000361.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. & Varga, S. (2015) Conceptual issues in autism spectrum disorder. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 28(2):127–32.Google Scholar
Hadjikhani, N., Åsberg Johnels, J., Zürcher, N. R., Lassalle, A., Guillon, Q., Hippolyte, L., Billstedt, E., Ward, N., Lemonnier, E. & Gillberg, C. (2017) Look me in the eyes: Constraining gaze in the eye-region provokes abnormally high subcortical activation in autism. Scientific Reports 7:3163. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-03378-5.Google Scholar
Hill, E., Berthoz, S. & Frith, U. (2004) Cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34(2):229–35.Google Scholar
Lumley, M. A., Neely, L. C. & Burger, A. J. (2007) The assessment of alexithymia in medical settings: Implications for understanding and treating health problems. Journal of Personality Assessment 89(3):230–46. doi:10.1080/00223890701629698.Google Scholar
Oakley, B. F. M., Brewer, R., Bird, G. & Catmur, C. (2016) Theory of mind is not theory of emotion: A cautionary note on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 125(6):818–23. doi:10.1037/abn0000182.Google Scholar
Poquérusse, J., Pastore, L., Dellantonio, S. & Esposito, G. (2018) Alexithymia and autism spectrum disorders: A complex relationship. Frontiers in Psychology. 9:1196. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01196.Google Scholar
Samson, A. C., Huber, O. & Gross, J. J. (2012) Emotion regulation in Asperger's syndrome and high-functioning autism. Emotion 12(4):659–65. doi:10.1037/a0027975.Google Scholar
Swart, M., Kortekaas, R. & Aleman, A. (2009) Dealing with feelings: Characterization of trait alexithymia on emotion regulation strategies and cognitive-emotional processing. PLOS ONE 4(6):e5751. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005751.Google Scholar
Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M. & Parker, J. D. A. (1997) Disorders of affect regulation: Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Timoney, R. L. & Holder, M. D. (2013) Emotional processing: Deficits and happiness. Assessing the measurement, correlates, and well-being of people with alexithymia. Springer.Google Scholar
Vanheule, S., Desmet, M., Megank, R. & Bogaerts, S. (2007) Alexithymia and interpersonal problems. Journal of Clinical Psychology 63:109–17.Google Scholar
Wotschack, C. & Klann-Delius, G. (2013) Alexithymia and the conceptualization of emotions: A study of language use and semantic knowledge. Journal of Research in Personality 47:514–23.Google Scholar