Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T06:00:03.716Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Leveraging individual differences to understand grounded procedures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 February 2021

Adam K. Fetterman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, TX77204-5022akfetterman@uh.edu;  https://fettermanlab.weebly.com/
Michael D. Robinson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, North Dakota State University, Psychology, NDSU Dept. 2765, Fargo, ND58108-6050Michael.D.Robinson@ndsu.edu; https://www.ndsu.edu/centers/cvcn/labs/robinson/MRobinson/index.html
Brian P. Meier
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Gettysburg College, Gettysburg, PA17325-1400. bmeier@gettysburg.edu; https://sites.google.com/view/brianpmeier

Abstract

We applaud the goals and execution of the target article, but note that individual differences do not receive much attention. This is a shortcoming because individual differences can play a vital role in theory testing. In our commentary, we describe programs of research of this type and also apply similar thinking to the mechanisms proposed in the target article.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Lee and Schwarz (L&S) push the work on cleansing forward by providing a cohesive theoretical account of grounded procedures of separation (vs. connection) that organizes the diversity of findings in this area although suggesting new directions for research. Linking cleansing to a broader class of separation procedures is a meaningful development, and is likely to propel this literature into a stronger theoretical context. Yet, issues of mechanism remain uncertain. This is in part because of the diversity of findings that exist (L&S count some 500 effects focused on cleansing) and in part because a number of different processes could be responsible for the effects of an independent variable on a dependent variable, particularly in the domain of embodied cognition. In this context, individual differences have a great deal to offer in that they can target the mechanisms thought to be involved in a way that experiments cannot (Underwood, Reference Underwood1975). In our commentary, we highlight the value of individual difference approaches to grounded cognition although speaking to the mechanisms thought to be involved in cleansing phenomena.

Experimental methods – especially in social psychology, which favors the between-subjects design – can be finicky and unreliable, leading to replicability issues (Fetterman, Reference Fetterman2016). Also, the results of these studies often have unknown external validity. One deceptively simple way of complementing the experimental approach, in embodiment research, is to turn the relevant behavior (or skill: Herbert & Pollatos, Reference Herbert and Pollatos2012) into an individual difference. If cleansing has psychological meaning, for example, individuals who clean themselves more often or who desire to clean themselves more often should differ in other ways that will allow us to understand the psychological functions of cleansing. Similar approaches have been taken with respect to other bodily gestures such as arm-crossing. Although some research had suggested that crossing one's arms might serve as a signal of pride, Fetterman, Bair, and Robinson (Reference Fetterman, Bair and Robinson2015) thought it more likely that arm-crossing, similar to other “closed” postures, is allied with defensive forms of motivation. To test this idea, Fetterman et al. (Reference Fetterman, Bair and Robinson2015) designed an arm-crossing questionnaire that simply asked individuals, in several ways, how often they cross their arms and feel like crossing their arms. As hypothesized, individuals who engaged in this gesture more frequently were socially submissive and averse to taking both social and physical risks. The correlates of cleansing frequency are relatively unknown, but relevant studies would provide important clues into why this behavior is performed as well as what psychological functions that it serves.

Individual differences can also provide key insights into the mechanisms involved in a particular experimental effect. To the extent that purity concerns motivate cleansing behavior, for example, individuals who value purity more – as a moral foundation (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Nosek, Haidt, Iyer, Koleva and Ditto2011) – should display the effect to a greater extent. Indeed, one might find that individuals low in purity concerns do not show the effect at all. It is difficult to overstate the explanatory value of such a pattern. First, it would establish moderator conditions for the phenomenon, which is an important next-generation question in embodiment research (Meier, Schnall, Schwarz, & Bargh, Reference Meier, Schnall, Schwarz and Bargh2012), particularly given the heterogeneity in effect sizes documented by L&S. Second, it would provide critical evidence for mechanism: If purity concerns are involved, as is proposed, then individuals who have such concerns to a greater extent should display the phenomenon to a greater extent. If they do not, one might need to rethink the mechanism that links the manipulation to the dependent measure (Underwood, Reference Underwood1975).

As an example of this type, consider Study 2 of Fetterman, Bair, Werth, Landkammer, and Robinson (Reference Fetterman, Bair, Werth, Landkammer and Robinson2016). Meier, Robinson, and Clore (Reference Meier, Robinson and Clore2004) had shown that negative words were evaluated more quickly when in a black font color and positive words were evaluated more quickly when in a white font color. Meier et al. (Reference Meier, Robinson and Clore2004) proposed that the relevant mechanism was metaphoric cognition because darker colors are metaphorically bad (e.g., “dark times”) and lighter colors are metaphorically good (e.g., “bright person”). If such effects are driven by metaphoric cognition, then people who use metaphors more often – in their everyday speech and thought – should be more susceptible to effects of this type. Fetterman et al. (Reference Fetterman, Bair, Werth, Landkammer and Robinson2016) examined this hypothesis by creating a Metaphor Use Measure that asked individuals whether they would use literal (e.g., “I was very sad”) or metaphoric (“my heart was broken”) language to characterize a series of events and feelings. There were 30 of these pairs and individuals were consistent in their tendencies toward literal versus metaphoric conceptions. Of particular importance, Fetterman et al. (Reference Fetterman, Bair, Werth, Landkammer and Robinson2016) found that assigning relatively neutral words to a lighter (vs. darker) font color resulted in more positive evaluations, but only among individuals who tend to think, speak, and write metaphorically. These findings, which are displayed in Figure 1, confirm the relevance of metaphoric thinking to the phenomena identified by Meier et al. (Reference Meier, Robinson and Clore2004).

Figure 1. Word evaluations as a function of font color and Metaphor Usage (±1 SD), recreated from Fetterman et al. (Reference Fetterman, Bair, Werth, Landkammer and Robinson2016).

The cleansing literature, we suggest, would benefit from similar analyses because a number of mechanisms have been proposed, but definitive individual difference studies have not been consistently carried out. If the phenomena previously identified (e.g., Schnall, Haidt, Clore, & Jordan, Reference Schnall, Haidt, Clore and Jordan2008) co-opt the disgust system, the relevant effects should be more pronounced among disgust-sensitive individuals. If they involve embodiment or metaphor, they may be more pronounced among individuals who exhibit greater embodiment (Häfner, Reference Häfner2013) or who use metaphors more often (Fetterman et al., Reference Fetterman, Bair, Werth, Landkammer and Robinson2016). As described, the psychological causes of cleansing behavior seem to involve avoidance motivation and, if so, the relevant effects should interact with avoidance motivation rather than approach motivation (Carver, Reference Carver2006). On the contrary, the consequences of cleansing may involve mechanisms (like psychological separation) that are more difficult to characterize and an individual difference approach could help in clarifying these processes. In general, then, we suggest that individual differences can play a key role in theorizing and mechanism evaluation within the cleansing literature specifically and embodied literature more broadly.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Carver, C. S. (2006). Approach, avoidance, and the self-regulation of affect and action. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 105110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fetterman, A. K. (2016). On god-belief and feeling clean: Feelings of cleanliness are associated with feelings and behavior in daily life, particularly for those high in god belief. Social Psychology and Personality Science, 7, 552559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fetterman, A. K., Bair, J. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2015). Submissive, inhibited, avoidant, and prone to escape: The correlates and consequences of crossing one's arms. Motivation Science, 1, 3746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fetterman, A. K., Bair, J. L., Werth, M., Landkammer, F., & Robinson, M. D. (2016). The scope and consequences of metaphoric thinking: Using individual differences in metaphor usage to understand the utility of conceptual metaphors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 458476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 366385. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Häfner, M. (2013). When body and mind are talking: Interoception moderates embodied cognition. Experimental Psychology, 60, 255259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, B. M., & Pollatos, O. (2012). The body in the mind: On the relationship between interoception and embodiment. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 692704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2004). Why good guys wear white: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on brightness. Psychological Science, 15, 8287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, B. P., Schnall, S., Schwarz, N., & Bargh, J. A. (2012). Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 705716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 10961109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Underwood, B. J. (1975). Individual differences as a crucible in theory construction. American Psychologist, 30, 128134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Word evaluations as a function of font color and Metaphor Usage (±1 SD), recreated from Fetterman et al. (2016).