Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T02:27:29.951Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Considering experimental and observational evidence of priming together, syntax doesn't look so autonomous

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2017

Nicholas A. Lester
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106. nlester@umail.ucsb.edudubois@linguistics.ucsb.edustgries@linguistics.ucsb.edufmoscoso@linguistics.ucsb.eduhttp://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/faculty/dubois/http://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/faculty/stgries/http://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/people/fermin-moscoso-del-prado-martin
John W. Du Bois
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106. nlester@umail.ucsb.edudubois@linguistics.ucsb.edustgries@linguistics.ucsb.edufmoscoso@linguistics.ucsb.eduhttp://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/faculty/dubois/http://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/faculty/stgries/http://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/people/fermin-moscoso-del-prado-martin
Stefan Th. Gries
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106. nlester@umail.ucsb.edudubois@linguistics.ucsb.edustgries@linguistics.ucsb.edufmoscoso@linguistics.ucsb.eduhttp://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/faculty/dubois/http://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/faculty/stgries/http://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/people/fermin-moscoso-del-prado-martin
Fermín Moscoso del Prado Martín
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106. nlester@umail.ucsb.edudubois@linguistics.ucsb.edustgries@linguistics.ucsb.edufmoscoso@linguistics.ucsb.eduhttp://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/faculty/dubois/http://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/faculty/stgries/http://www.linguistics.ucsb.edu/people/fermin-moscoso-del-prado-martin

Abstract

We agree with Branigan & Pickering (B&P) that structural priming experiments should supplant grammaticality judgments for testing linguistic representation. However, B&P overlook a vast (corpus-)linguistic literature that converges with – but extends – the experimental findings. B&P conclude that syntax is functionally independent of the lexicon. We argue that a broader approach to priming reveals cracks in the façade of syntactic autonomy.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Branigan & Pickering (B&P) make a compelling case for the utility of experimental methods – in particular, priming – for understanding linguistic representation. We whole-heartedly support this position. As linguists, however, we must note that B&P have misrepresented the state of affairs within linguistics. The claim that linguists rely solely (i.e., “on a single method,” “dominantly,” “almost exclusively”) on acceptability judgments is an exaggeration. Said judgments have indeed been prevalent in the work of some linguists, but – especially in the last two decades – this is far from the sole method used. A glance at the papers forthcoming in Language finds just one paper using acceptability judgments, but four using analyses of observational data or corpora and two using advanced statistical techniques. Furthermore, several major schools of linguistic thought have flatly rejected the validity of acceptability judgments for more than three decades (e.g., Bybee Reference Bybee2006; Chafe Reference Chafe1994; Givón Reference Givón1983; Thompson & Mulac Reference Thompson and Mulac1991).

Much of what we discuss below relies on corpus data. Pickering and Branigan (Reference Pickering and Branigan1999) argue that such data cannot speak to the nature of priming, given their relatively low level of control compared to well-controlled experimental designs. This assumption reflects a common prejudice among experimental psychologists: That the “found data” nature of corpora makes them unsuitable for disentangling target effects from confounds. Modern statistical techniques now enable distinguishing the influences of many confounding variables. In fact, many variables important to priming are more difficult to control for in experimental paradigms than in corpus studies (e.g., distance effects between prime and target, beta-persistence [Szmrecsanyi Reference Szmrecsanyi2006]; effects of non-variable structures on variable contexts, cumulative priming effects [Jaeger & Snider Reference Jaeger and Snider2013]).

B&P argue that syntactic representations are independent of semantics and lexicon. This assertion is ambiguous (Croft Reference Croft1995). We all agree that syntactic aspects are (explicitly or implicitly) represented in the mind. However, saying that syntax is functionally and/or representationally encapsulated apart from lexicon and semantics is more contentious. B&P support this claim by showing that abstract clausal templates (e.g., prepositional-object [PO] or double-object [DO]) are primed even without lexical overlap between the prime and target. Further, semantically dissimilar but syntactically similar structures prime each other. For example, intransitive + locative-PP constructions prime passives. However, these intransitives and passives have more in common than acknowledged by the authors. For instance, ergative languages align such structures along both syntactic and semantic dimensions (Keenan Reference Keenan1984). Moreover, semantic similarity beyond lexical overlap has been found to drive syntactic choice of PO/DO, even in the absence of syntactic similarity (Hare & Goldberg Reference Hare, Goldberg, Hahn and Stones1999).

Another strong indicator of the semantic properties of clausal constructions is the statistical association between verbs and constructions (Goldberg Reference Goldberg2006; Ellis & Ferreira-Junior Reference Ellis and Ferreira-Junior2009; Stefanowitsch & Gries Reference Stefanowitsch and Gries2003). These associations co-determine the magnitude of priming (e.g., Gries Reference Gries2005). Importantly, they do not merely boost priming but may actually resist priming (and these relationships may change depending on context [Jaeger & Snider Reference Jaeger and Snider2013]). Lexical choices also often dictate syntactic choices, both in production and in comprehension (e.g., Jaeger Reference Jaeger2010; Novick et al. Reference Novick, Kim and Trueswell2003). Importantly, such choices may be influenced by syntactic information that prima facie should be irrelevant to the syntactic alternation under consideration (Wasow et al. Reference Wasow, Jaeger, Orr, Simon and Wiese2011). Furthermore, words – even syntactically impoverished bare nouns – are never processed in isolation from the entirety of their syntactic distributional information, and may prime each other via such distributions (Lester & Moscoso del Prado Martín Reference Lester, Moscoso del Prado Martín, Papafragou, Grodner, Mirman and Trueswell2016; Lester et al. Reference Lester, Feldman, Moscosodel Prado Martín, Gunzelmann, Howes, Tenbrink and Davelaar2017).

B&P survey clear evidence of priming among words, syntactic structures, and semantic structures. They also explain how simultaneous overlap between any two of these levels results in increased priming (the so-called boosts). One can account for these findings in two ways: (1) relationships among syntax, semantics, and lexicon are captured by additional interfaces whose only job is to combine information from separate modules (e.g., Jackendoff Reference Jackendoff, Hoffmann and Trousdale2013); or (2) the relationships constitute connection weights between words and structures, which are directly related in memory (Diessel Reference Diessel, Dąbrowska and Divjak2015). B&P appear to prefer the first option. However, short of undisputed neuropsychological evidence for the separation between these representational levels (which is not known to us), there is no way of distinguishing among three separated levels with connections between them, and a single level of representation with different degrees of overlap. Considering that priming effects are very similar in the three levels, and that overlap among them interacts, it seems more parsimonious to assume a single layer of representation, rather than positing three such encapsulated layers plus interconnections.

B&P's arguments rely on binary choices (such as PO/DO). However, it is unlikely that these choices could benefit from structural overlap in phrasal constituents; the critical variable depends only on where those phrases are placed. If there is no additional reason to adjust structures to accommodate the accessibility of subclausal units, then why would one? Whether there may be a task-driven confound remains a question for further study. However, notice that chronometric studies show that the locus of priming may not always be the clause, even when clause-structural overlap is present (Smith & Wheeldon Reference Smith and Wheeldon2001). Further, more comprehensive models of linguistic reproduction exist, which make distinctions beyond simple identity priming. Consider Dialogic Syntax (Du Bois Reference Du Bois2014; Du Bois et al. Reference Du Bois, Hobson and Hobson2014), which distinguishes among framing resonance, the locus of syntactic priming, and focal resonance, the aligning of meanings within syntactic alignment.

We emphasize that we are not advocating the position that syntactic priming is reducible to lexical, semantic, or pragmatic effects. To truly understand linguistic representation on the basis of processing, we must consider all possible sources of information from processing across all levels that are brought to bear on language use, including data from both experimental and observed contexts. This trend is already well underway in several major branches of linguistics. B&P's bold proposal to establish “a new basis for understanding the nature of language” stands to benefit from a full partnership with researchers drawing on a broad range of evidence to account for a system that dynamically responds to linguistic, cognitive, and interactional contexts.

References

Bybee, J. L. (2006) From usage to grammar: The mind's response to repetition. Language 82:711–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. L. (1994) Discourse, consciousness, and time: The flow and displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Croft, W. (1995) Autonomy and functionalist linguistics. Language 71:490532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, H. (2015) Usage-based construction grammar. In: Handbook of cognitive linguistics, ed. Dąbrowska, E. and Divjak, D., pp. 295321. De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Du Bois, J. W. (2014) Towards a dialogic syntax. Cognitive Linguistics 25:359410.Google Scholar
Du Bois, J. W., Hobson, R. P. & Hobson, J. A. (2014) Dialogic resonance and intersubjective engagement in autism. Cognitive Linguistics 25:411–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. & Ferreira-Junior, F. (2009) Constructions and their acquisition: Islands and the distinctiveness of their occupancy. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics 7:187220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T., ed. (1983) Topic continuity in discourse: A quantitative cross–language study. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2006) Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gries, S. Th. (2005) Syntactic priming: A corpus-based approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 34(4):365–99. doi:10.1007/s10936-005-6139-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, M. L. & Goldberg, A. E. (1999) Structural priming: Purely syntactic? In: Proceedings of the Twenty-first Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Hahn, M. & Stones, S. C., pp. 208–11. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2013) Constructions in the parallel architecture. In: The Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar, ed. Hoffmann, T. & Trousdale, G., pp. 7092. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2010) Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density. Cognitive Psychology 61:2362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeger, T. F. & Snider, N. E. (2013) Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime's prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition 127(1):5783.Google Scholar
Keenan, E. L. (1984) Semantic correlates of the ergative/absolutive distinction. Linguistics 22:197224.Google Scholar
Lester, N. A., Feldman, L. B. & Moscosodel Prado Martín, F. (2017) You can take a noun out of syntax…: Syntactic similarity effects in lexical priming. In: Proceedings of the Thirty-ninth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Gunzelmann, G., Howes, A., Tenbrink, T. & Davelaar, E., pp. 2537–42. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Lester, N. A. & Moscoso del Prado Martín, F. (2016) Syntactic flexibility in the noun: Evidence from picture naming. In: Proceedings of the Thirty-eighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Papafragou, A., Grodner, D., Mirman, D. & Trueswell, J. C., pp. 2585–90. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Novick, J. M., Kim, A. & Trueswell, J. C. (2003) Studying the grammatical aspects of word recognition: Lexical priming, parsing, and syntactic-ambiguity resolution. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 32:5775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, M. J. & Branigan, H. P. (1999) Syntactic priming in language production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3:136–41.Google Scholar
Smith, M. & Wheeldon, L. (2001) Syntactic priming in spoken sentence production: An online study. Cognition 78:123–64. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00110-4.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, A. & Gries, S. Th. (2003) Collostructions: Investigating the interaction of words and constructions. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 8:209–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szmrecsanyi, B. (2006) Morphosyntactic persistence in spoken English: A corpus study at the intersection of variationist sociolinguistics, psycholinguistics, and discourse analysis. de Gruyter Mouton. doi:10.1515/9783110197808.Google Scholar
Thompson, S. A. & Mulac, A. (1991) The discourse conditions for the use of the complementizer “that” in conversational English. Journal of Pragmatics 15:237–51.Google Scholar
Wasow, T., Jaeger, T. F. & Orr, D. M. (2011) Lexical variation in relativizer frequency. In: Expecting the unexpected: Exceptions in grammar, ed. Simon, H. J. & Wiese, H., pp. 175–96. deGruyter.Google Scholar