Article contents
The malleability of linguistic representations poses a challenge to the priming-based experimental approach
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 10 November 2017
Abstract
Recent findings show that experience with a syntactic structure has long-term consequences for how that structure will be processed in the future, which suggests that linguistic representations are not static entities that can be probed reliably without alteration. Thus, leveraging the effect of previous exposure to a syntactic structure appears to be an inappropriate method for studying invariant properties of language.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
Bock, K., Dell, G. S., Chang, F. & Onishi, K. H. (2007) Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production. Cognition
104(3):437–58. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.003.Google Scholar
Bock, K. & Griffin, Z. M. (2000) The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning?
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
129:177–92. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.177.Google Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J. & Cleland, A. A. (1999) Syntactic priming in written production: Evidence for rapid decay. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
6(4):635–40. Available at: http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212972.Google Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Stewart, A. J. & McLean, J. F. (2000) Syntactic priming in spoken production: Linguistic and temporal interference. Memory & Cognition
28(8):1297–302. Available at: http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211830.Google Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G. S. & Bock, K. (2006) Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review
113(2):234–72. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.234.Google Scholar
Chang, F., Janciauskas, M. & Fitz, H. (2012) Language adaptation and learning: Getting explicit about implicit learning. Linguistics and Language Compass
6(5):259–78. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1002/lnc3.337.Google Scholar
Fine, A. B. & Jaeger, T. F. (2016) The role of verb repetition in cumulative structural priming in comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
42:1362–76.Google Scholar
Fine, A. B., Jaeger, T. F., Farmer, T. A. & Qian, T. (2013) Rapid expectation adaptation during syntactic comprehension. PLoS ONE
8(10):e77661. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0077661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartsuiker, R. J., Bernolet, S., Schoonbaert, S., Speybroeck, S. & Vanderelst, D. (2008) Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language
58(2):214–38. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.003.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. & Snider, N. E. (2013) Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime's prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition
127(1):57–83.Google Scholar
Kamide, Y. (2012) Learning individual talkers' structural preferences. Cognition
124(1): 66–71. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.001.Google Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J. & Coyle, J. M. (2014) Long and short term cumulative structural priming effects. Language and Cognitive Processes
29(6):1–23. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.641387.Google Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J. & Jones, J. L. (2011a) Structural priming as implicit learning: Cumulative priming effects and individual differences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
18(6):1133–39. Available at: http://doi.org/10.14440/jbm.2015.54.A.Google Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J. & Schatschneider, C. (2011b) Long-term cumulative structural priming persists for (at least) one week. Memory & Cognition
39(3):381–88. Available at: http://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0042-3.Google Scholar
Long, D. L. & Prat, C. S. (2008) Individual differences in syntactic ambiguity resolution: Readers vary in their use of plausibility information. Memory and Cognition
36(2):375–91. Available at: http://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.2.375.Google Scholar
Luka, B. J. & Barsalou, L. W. (2005) Structural facilitation: Mere exposure effects for grammatical acceptability as evidence for syntactic priming in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language
52(3):444–67. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.01.013.Google Scholar
Mahowald, K., James, A., Futrell, R. & Gibson, E. (2016b) A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language
91:5–27. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2016.03.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryskin, R. A., Qi, Z., Duff, M. C. & Brown-Schmidt, S. (2017) Verb biases are shaped through lifelong learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
43(5):781–94.Google Scholar
Tooley, K. M., Swaab, T. Y., Boudewyn, M. A., Zirnstein, M. & Traxler, M. J. (2014b) Evidence for priming across intervening sentences during on-line sentence comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes
29(3):289–311. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.770892.Google Scholar
Tooley, K. M. & Traxler, M. J. (2010). Syntactic priming effects in comprehension: A critical review. Language and Linguistics Compass
4(10):925–37. Available at: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00249.x.Google Scholar
Wells, J. B., Christiansen, M. H., Race, D. S. & MacDonald, M. C. (2009) Experience and sentence processing: Statistical learning and relative clause comprehension. Cognitive Psychology
58(2):250–71. doi: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2008.08.002.Google Scholar
We agree with Branigan & Pickering (B&P) that understanding the nature of linguistic representations is a key question for psycholinguists and linguists alike, but relying on acceptability judgments imposes serious limitations on what can be learned. Further, we agree that implicit processing measures can provide valuable insights about linguistic representations. We have concerns, however, about the use of syntactic priming as the primary measure for the investigation of purportedly stable linguistic representations. The first concern is of a practical nature: Syntactic priming effects are often subtle, and many of the potential links between structures of interest may be very difficult to detect reliably, rendering the picture more uncertain rather than clearer. Second, and more important, recent evidence suggests that language representations are not stable but rather continuously updated based on recent experience. The current proposal for a unified model of language representations, as seen through the lens of syntactic priming, can provide only a partial and possibly inaccurate account if it ignores the multiple timescales and contexts in which learning, and thus, language change, takes place.
The logic of the proposed method as described in the target article requires that (1) the facilitatory effect of a prime structure on a test structure be a direct reflection of how closely related those structures are in representational space; and (2) that the underlying representations of the language not be affected by recent exposure to either of the structures or their prior co-occurrence. In order for a syntactic priming paradigm to be a useful tool for understanding the structure of linguistic representation, it must be assumed that the act of processing the prime and target structures has no effect on that same language user's syntactic representations, or that such effects are transient (Branigan et al. Reference Branigan, Pickering and Cleland1999).
In contrast, recent findings show that experience with a syntactic structure has long-term consequences for how that structure will be processed in the future, both in comprehension (e.g., Fine et al. Reference Fine, Jaeger, Farmer and Qian2013; Fine & Jaeger Reference Fine and Jaeger2016; Kamide Reference Kamide2012; Long & Prat Reference Long and Prat2008; Luka & Barsalou Reference Luka and Barsalou2005; Ryskin et al. Reference Ryskin, Qi, Duff and Brown-Schmidt2017; Tooley et al. Reference Tooley, Swaab, Boudewyn, Zirnstein and Traxler2014b; Wells et al. Reference Wells, Christiansen, Race and MacDonald2009) and production (e.g., Bock et al. Reference Bock, Dell, Chang and Onishi2007; Bock & Griffin Reference Bock and Griffin2000; Branigan et al. Reference Branigan, Pickering, Stewart and McLean2000; Hartsuiker et al. Reference Hartsuiker, Bernolet, Schoonbaert, Speybroeck and Vanderelst2008; Jaeger & Snider Reference Jaeger and Snider2013; Kaschak et al. Reference Kaschak, Kutta and Coyle2014; Reference Kaschak, Kutta and Jones2011a; Reference Kaschak, Kutta and Schatschneider2011b). These results point to an error-based learning mechanism underlying syntactic priming across modalities (Chang et al. Reference Chang, Dell and Bock2006; Chang et al. Reference Chang, Janciauskas and Fitz2012). On this account, exposure to the prime sentence makes a sentence with that same structure more expected, and this up-weighting process is cumulative, such that the target sentence also serves the same function. Further, by the same error-based learning mechanism, the co-occurrence of structures with particular lexical items in the sentences should lead to an increased expectation for that combination in the future. Indeed, repeatedly exposing listeners to a novel pairing of a verb and a syntactic structure – Rub the duck with the lollipop which is high attachment (the ambiguous PP, with the lollipop attaches to the verb) if the visual context constrains the lollipop to be an instrument, but low attachment (the ambiguous PP attaches to the head noun) if the lollipop is an accessory tied to the duck – leads listeners to form a new verb bias (Ryskin et al. Reference Ryskin, Qi, Duff and Brown-Schmidt2017). In other words, the representation of a verb and its link to a structure is malleable and shaped by the context in which that verb appears. This context may consist of the other words in the sentence, the visual environment, or even the identity of the speaker (Kamide Reference Kamide2012).
Does this present a challenge for the use of syntactic priming as a tool for studying language representations? Evidence for the cumulative learning underlying syntactic representations suggests that syntactic priming experiments often may miss the links investigators wish to uncover. In a paradigm where test stimulus A is preceded by primes B or C, the presentations of A, B, and C lead to those representations becoming more active or more probable. Given that priming experiments typically involve repeated measures, over the course of the task the activation level for all relevant structures may be brought to some ceiling level at which the additional facilitation conferred by B is no longer detectable. Indeed, cumulative priming may contribute to the small effect sizes observed in the priming literature (Mahowald et al. Reference Mahowald, James, Futrell and Gibson2016b; Tooley & Traxler Reference Tooley and Traxler2010; see Fine et al. Reference Fine, Jaeger, Farmer and Qian2013 for a similar methodological point). Continued use of syntactic priming to test more and more subtle connections between structures will likely lead to many null findings from which no conclusions about the true nature of syntactic representations can be drawn.
From a theory-building perspective, the proposed approach overlooks a large swath of the current literature and thus can provide only an incomplete picture of language representation. On many current accounts (Chang et al. Reference Chang, Dell and Bock2006; Chang et al. Reference Chang, Janciauskas and Fitz2012; Jaeger & Snider Reference Jaeger and Snider2013; Fine et al. Reference Fine, Jaeger, Farmer and Qian2013), syntactic priming effects are a byproduct of the continuous learning that shapes language representations. Based on these findings, we can infer that experiences with individual prime or target structures will accumulate to change the relative probabilities of these structures. Further, the probabilistic bindings between lexical items (e.g., verbs) and structures are subject to the same cumulative learning through verb-structure co-activation. In this context, the question of whether structure A is more similar to B or C may be ill posed. The answer provided by a syntactic priming experiment may be entirely dependent on the particular time-point at which the language user is probed and what linguistic experiences preceded this sampling. To understand the nature of linguistic representations, then, a more useful approach will be to examine how the learning prompted by interpretation of a single utterance in a given context, scales up to support language learning and language change throughout the lifespan.