Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-07T06:16:30.296Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Communal sharing/identity fusion does not require reflection on episodic memory of shared experience or trauma – and usually generates kindness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 December 2018

Lotte Thomsen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Norway. lotte.thomsen@psykologi.uio.nohttp://www.sv.uio.no/psi/personer/vit/lottetho/ Department of Political Science, Aarhus University, Denmark
Alan P. Fiske
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095. afiske@ucla.eduhttps://www.anthro.ucla.edu/faculty/alan-page-fiske

Abstract

Identity fusion is remarkably similar to the extensively validated construct of communal sharing, proposed in 1991. Both posit that notions of oneness/unity/equivalence with others underpin altruism. However, we argue that oneness/equivalence instantiates an evolved, innate relational form, marked and constituted by cultural practices making participants’ bodies substantially the same. It is intuitive from earliest development, often encompasses persons whom one has never met, and results mostly in caring.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Whitehouse proposes that altruistic self-sacrifice stems from identity fusion, “a visceral feeling of oneness with the group,” which stems from shared, traumatic, or at least life-altering experience and “perceptions of shared essence” resulting in an all-or-nothing relationship, where you are either included or not.

Relational models theory (Fiske Reference Fiske1991; Reference Fiske1992) posits that communal sharing (CS) is a relationship of equivalence and unity, in which people feel they have a common essence. They communally coordinate resources, decisions, responsibilities, tasks, or whatever matters. Underpinning this relationship of altruistic solidarity – prototypically implemented among close kin – is the mathematical structure of an equivalence relation, homologous to a nominal scale of measurement that results in categorical in–out distinctions. The validity of the CS construct has been established in hundreds of studies by hundreds of authors using all kinds of methods (http://www.sscnet.ucla.edu/anthro/faculty/fiske/RM_PDFs/RM_bibliography.htm). Fiske and Rai (Reference Fiske and Rai2014) have shown that CS and the other three relational models are moral frameworks that motivate most violence across cultures.

Although identity fusion (IF) is remarkably similar to the CS construct, we fundamentally differ from Whitehouse in our theory of the origins and functional scope of this sense of equivalence.

In Whitehouse's proposal, it is unclear why traumatic experience with present others should result specifically in “visceral oneness” with them, rather than any other relationship, such as relative ranking (e.g., Who is most brave, most loyal, or even dominant?). In contrast, relational models theory explains the range and specificity of experiences of consubstantial assimilation that bond persons in CS relationships by making their bodies equivalent or contiguous (Fiske Reference Fiske and Haslam2004). CS results from giving birth, nursing, feeding, commensalism, sharing bodily substances (e.g., blood brotherhood rituals), caressing, cuddling and sleeping together, intimate sex, synchronous rhythmic movement, and marking or modifying the body (e.g., circumcision or clitoridectomy), thereby creating the impression of one merged, social body containing the same essence. These bonding experiences are rarely painful, fearful, or in any way traumatic: Shared traumatic experience is not necessary for even the most intense CS, nor is reflection on episodic memory. Furthermore, CS relationships are adaptively essential to, and universally used for, the coordination of labor, consumption, use of resources and land, and everything else social: The feeling of oneness is not an adaptive specialization primarily for killing or dying for the group.

In contrast to our view that CS relationships implement and instantiate an evolved, innate, intuitive relational form, Whitehouse posits that identity fusion results from individual cognitive deliberation. It arises from “internal processes of reflection and individual learning” (sect. 6, para. 7) about dysphoric experiences, where episodic memory of traumatic or life-altering events binds the self to the particular people who participated and to their common social identity. This view predicts that infants could not feel or understand the altruistic implications of social oneness/equivalence, insofar as they are not yet capable of extended cognitive reflection, they have not yet solidified a self-concept, and their explicit episodic memories are limited. But CS and its accompanying altruism are readily intuitive to infants. Indeed, we posit this must be so if they are to solve the fundamental learning problem of figuring out who relates to whom and how (Fiske Reference Fiske1991; Thomsen & Carey Reference Thomsen, Carey, Banaji and Gelman2013).

For example, reflecting the role played by synchronous motion, spatial closeness, and looking and acting alike in constituting CS relations of equivalence, infants expect individuals who move in synchrony and close to each other to later act alike. They infer that imitation, as well as shared ritualistic actions, marks social affiliation motives and group membership (Liberman et al. Reference Liberman, Kinzler and Woodward2018; Powell & Spelke Reference Powell and Spelke2013; Reference Powell and Spelke2018). They prefer those who help similar and hinder dissimilar others (Hamlin et al. Reference Hamlin, Mahajan, Liberman and Wynn2013) and copy the food preferences of helpers over hinderers (Hamlin & Wynn Reference Hamlin and Wynn2012). They expect altruistic support to be selectively directed to in-group members who are marked to look alike (Jin & Baillargeon Reference Jin and Baillargeon2017), overriding fairness considerations (Bian et al. Reference Bian, Sloane and Baillargeon2018). They also respond with increased helping to primes of closeness and affectionate touch between dolls (Over & Carpenter Reference Over and Carpenter2009).

Whitehouse cites experimental evidence that episodic recall of dysphoric experience correlates with altruistic self-sacrifice and that this is mediated by measures of fusion. From this, he infers that traumatic (or at least life-altering) experiences are somehow the essential root of feeling one. However, these effects need not imply that the basic representation of unity and the communal motivations and expectations it entails are created by shared dysphoric, life-altering experiences. Only a small proportion of the people who have had a shared dysphoric experience kill or die because of it. On the contrary, collective trauma typically leads to extraordinary kindness and compassion to everyone around (James Reference James1906; Lessa Reference Lessa1964; Oliver-Smith Reference Oliver-Smith1986; Solnit Reference Solnit2009), and only a small proportion of those who kill or die for others have ever met most of the people they kill or die for.

Whitehouse proposes that fusion to imagined communities results from projecting local bonds from personally shared experiences to entire social categories such as nation and religion. However, rather than scaling up a local bond to the whole category, recent empirical evidence demonstrates that people may also fuse to ethnic out-groups across the globe with whom they share political ideology, but no dysphoric experience, let alone shared participation in a face-to-face event (Kunst et al. Reference Kunst, Boos, Kimel, Obaidi, Shani and Thomsen2018). This suggests that what is “projected” cannot be concrete personal experience, but must be an innate, intuitive, social relationship of CS. Indeed, intense CS relationships are ubiquitous among people who have never had a traumatic experience or ever met, yet will kill or die for each other (Fiske Reference Fiske1991; Fiske & Rai Reference Fiske and Rai2014; Ginges et al. Reference Ginges, Atran, Medin and Shikaki2007).

Personally shared experiences of trauma are an important bonding mechanism, and people surely reflect on such experiences. However, few, if any survivors of school shootings, for example, are thereby motivated to kill and die for their schoolmates. Feeling and understanding communal sharing are fundamental to human living but do not require or typically stem from cognitive reflection on episodic memory of shared traumatic experiences.

References

Bian, L., Sloane, S. & Baillargeon, R. (2018) Infants expect ingroup support to override fairness when resources are limited. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 115(11):2705–10. Available at: www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1719445115.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (1991) Structures of social life: The four elementary forms of social relations. Free Press.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (1992) The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review 99:689723.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (2004) Four modes of constituting relationships: Consubstantial assimilation; space, magnitude, time, and force; concrete procedures, & abstract symbolism. In: Relational models theory: A contemporary overview, ed. Haslam, N., pp. 52142. Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Fiske, A. P. & Rai, T. S (2014) Virtuous violence: Hurting and killing to create, sustain, and end honor social relationships. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ginges, J., Atran, S., Medin, D. & Shikaki, K. (2007) Sacred bounds on rational resolution of violent political conflict. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104(18):7357–60.Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Mahajan, N., Liberman, Z. & Wynn, K. (2013) Not like me=bad: Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychological Science 24(4):589–94.Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K. & Wynn, K. (2012) Infants fail to match the food preferences of antisocial others. Cognitive Development 27:227–39.Google Scholar
James, W. (1906) On some mental effects of the earthquake. The Library of America Story of the Week. Reprinted from William James: Writings 1902–1910 (The Library of America, 1987), pp. 1215–22. First published in the June 7, 1906, edition of Youth's Companion. Accessed August 9, 2018. Available at: http://storyoftheweek.loa.org/2010/08/on-some-mental-effects-of-earthquake.html.Google Scholar
Jin, K. & Baillargeon, R. (2017) Infants possess an abstract notion of ingroup support. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 114:8199–204.Google Scholar
Kunst, J. R., Boos, B., Kimel, S., Obaidi, M., Shani, M. & Thomsen, L. (2018) Engaging in extreme activism in support of others' political struggles: The role of politically motivated fusion with out-groups. PLoS ONE 13(1):e0190639. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0190639.Google Scholar
Lessa, W. A. (1964) The social effects of typhoon Ophelia (1960) on Ulithi. Micronesica 1(1/2):147.Google Scholar
Liberman, Z., Kinzler, K. & Woodward, A. (2018) The early social significance of shared ritual actions. Cognition 17:4251.Google Scholar
Oliver-Smith, A. (1986) The martyred city: Death and rebirth in the Andes. University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Over, H. & Carpenter, M. (2009) Eighteen-month-old children show increased helping following priming with affiliation. Psychological Science 20(10):1189–93.Google Scholar
Powell, L. J. & Spelke, E. S. (2013) Preverbal infants expect members of social groups to act alike. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 110:E3965–72.Google Scholar
Powell, L. J. & Spelke, E. S. (2018) Human infants' understanding of social imitation: Inferences of affiliation from third-party observations. Cognition 170:3148.Google Scholar
Rai, T. S. & Fiske, A. P. (2011) Moral psychology is relationship regulation: moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review 118(1):5775.Google Scholar
Solnit, R. (2009) A paradise built in hell: The extraordinary communities that arise in disaster. Penguin.Google Scholar
Thomsen, L. & Carey, S. (2013) Core cognition of relational models. In: Navigating the social world: What infants, children and other species can teach us, ed. Banaji, M. & Gelman, S.. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar