Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-f46jp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T15:18:48.633Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why we can talk, debate, and change our minds: Neural circuits, basal ganglia operations, and transcriptional factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 December 2014

Philip Lieberman*
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912. Philip.Lieberman@gmail.comwww.Brown.edu

Abstract

Ackermann et al. disregard attested knowledge concerning aphasia, Parkinson disease, cortical-to-striatal circuits, basal ganglia, laryngeal phonation, and other matters. Their dual-pathway model cannot account for “what is special about the human brain.” Their human cortical-to-laryngeal neural circuit does not exist. Basal ganglia operations, enhanced by mutations on FOXP2, confer human motor-control, linguistic, and cognitive capabilities.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

It has been clear for decades that aphasia never occurs without subcortical damage, and can occur absent insult to the cortex (Naeser et al. Reference Naeser, Alexander, Helm-Estabrooks, Levine, Laughlin and Geschwind1982; Stuss & Benson Reference Stuss and Benson1986). The speech production deficits of Parkinson disease and focal lesions to the basal ganglia are qualitatively similar to ones occurring in aphasia (Blumstein Reference Blumstein, Miller and Eimas1995; Blumstein et al. Reference Blumstein, Cooper, Goodglass, Statlender and Gottlieb1980; Lieberman et al. Reference Lieberman, Friedman and Feldman1990; Reference Lieberman, Kako, Friedman, Tajchman, Feldman and Jiminez1992; Pickett et al. Reference Pickett, Kuniholm, Protopapas, Friedman and Lieberman1998; Usui et al. Reference Usui, Inoue, Kimura, Kirino, Nagaoka, Abe, Nagata and Arai2004) and are not limited to aberrant laryngeal phonation. Motor control is slow and imprecise, thus degrading speech, walking, and other internally guided motor tasks (Harrington & Haaland Reference Harrington and Haaland1991; Marsden & Obeso Reference Marsden and Obeso1994). A suite of cognitive deficits occurs (Flowers & Robertson Reference Flowers and Robertson1985; Lange et al. Reference Lange, Robbins, Marsden, James, Owen and Paul1992), including impairment of cognitive inflexibility and comprehending distinctions in meaning conveyed by syntax (Grossman et al. Reference Grossman, Carvell, Gollomp, Stern, Vernon and Hurtig1991; Lieberman et al. Reference Lieberman, Friedman and Feldman1990; Reference Lieberman, Kako, Friedman, Tajchman, Feldman and Jiminez1992; Natsopoulos et al. Reference Natsopoulos, Grouios, Bostantzopoulou, Mentenopoulos, Katsarou and Logothetis1993). Similar, less pronounced, motor and cognitive deficits occur when hypoxic insult degrades the metabolically active basal ganglia (Lieberman et al. Reference Lieberman, Kanki, Protopapas, Reed and Youngs1994; Reference Lieberman, Morey, Hochstadt, Larson and Mather2005).

These behavioral deficits derive from insult to a network of segregated cortical-to-basal neural circuits linking areas of motor cortex and prefrontal cortex. Marsden and Obeso (Reference Marsden and Obeso1994), taking into account a comprehensive range of studies, concluded that the basal ganglia act as a neural “switch” in circuits linking them to the motor cortex, activating and linking submovements in internally guided acts such as walking or talking. When circumstances suggest a different motor response, the basal ganglia switch to a different sequence. The basal ganglia perform similar operations during cognitive tasks in circuits that include areas of the prefrontal cortex. fMRI studies confirm their supposition. For example, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the caudate nucleus of the basal ganglia are active when a subject is planning to change how he or she is sorting images on the basis of their shapes, to sorting them by color; or selecting words that rhyme and, instead, shifting to selecting words that have similar meanings. A cortical-to-basal ganglia circuit that includes the putamen and posterior prefrontal cortex is active during the execution of a sorting set-shift. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is involved whenever subjects make any decision, apparently monitoring whether the subjects' responses were consistent with the chosen sorting criterion (Monchi et al. Reference Monchi, Petrides, Petre, Worsley and Dagher2001; Simard et al. Reference Simard, Joanette, Petrides, Jubault, Madjar and Monchi2011). Other neuroimaging studies, reviewed in Lieberman (Reference Lieberman2000; Reference Lieberman2002; Reference Lieberman2006b; Reference Lieberman2012; Reference Lieberman2013), show that the prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia are active when subjects have to understand the meaning of a sentence, recall words from memory, subtract numbers, and cognitive tasks. All primates, including humans, appear to have similar cortical-to-basal ganglia circuits (Lehericy et al. Reference Lehericy, Ducros, Van de Moortele, Francois, Thivard, Poupon, Swindale, Ugurbil and Kim2004).

Ackermann et al. instead place great weight on a hypothetical direct cortical-to-laryngeal neural circuit that bypasses the basal ganglia, accepting a premise advanced in Fitch (Reference Fitch2010). The circuit does not exist, being based on flawed attempts to adapt a lethal tracer technique to study humans. The Nauta and Gygax (Reference Nauta and Gygax1954) technique necessitates destroying discrete neural structures in an animal's brain. After some weeks the animal is sacrificed and its brain is impregnated with a silver solution that delineates neuronal structure. Microscopic examination of sectioned brain tissue can then reveal damage to downstream neurons in circuits to the neural structure that was destroyed. Using this technique, Kuypers (Reference Kuypers1958a) and Iwatsubo et al. (Reference Iwatsubo, Kuzuhara, Kanemitsu, Shimada and Toyokura1990) claimed that changes to spinal cord neurons that enervate the larynx revealed a direct cortical-laryngeal circuit. However, the deceased patients studied had massive brain damage that included the basal ganglia and pathways to it. Similar changes to brainstem neurons occurred in patients who had died from non-neurological disease processes (Terao et al. Reference Terao, Li, Hashizume, Osano, Mitsuma and Sobue1997). Jürgens (Reference Jürgens2002b) concludes his review article on the neural bases of motor control by noting that “motor coordination of learned vocal patterns comes from the motor cortex and basal ganglia” (p. 251). Moreover, in itself, enhanced laryngeal control of phonation would not have yielded the encoding of segmental phonemes that is a unique property of human speech (Liberman et al. Reference Liberman, Cooper, Shankweiler and Studdert-Kennedy1967).

Ackermann et al. claim that basal ganglia circuits are devoted to learning “digital” linguistic contrasts in the first years of life, then shift to learning emotional prosody. However, no data are presented to support this claim, and developmental studies show that this is not the case. For example, prosodic patterns signaling intent are apparent in the first year of life in infants in a Catalan-speaking environment (Esteve-Gibert & Prieto Reference Esteve-Gibert and Prieto2013). Both lexical tones and prosodic patterns emerge in the early years of life for Mandarin-learning infants (Chen & Kent Reference Chen and Kent2009).

As my publications have pointed out, transcriptional factors such as FOXP2 may hold the key to why the human brain enables us to talk, continually create new forms of art, and possess language (Lieberman Reference Lieberman2006b; Reference Lieberman2009; Reference Lieberman2013). The basal ganglia, which initially played a role in motor control, appear to have been modified in the course of evolution. The version of FOXP2 that differs with respect to two amino acids from chimpanzees enhances synaptic plasticity in basal ganglia neurons and in the substantia nigra. It also increases dendritic connectivity. A third mutation on FOXP2 (on interon 8, close to the amino acid substitutions) appears to enhance transcription. This uniquely human mutation occurred when modern humans first appeared in Africa (Maricic et al. Reference Maricic, Günther, Georgiev, Gehre, Curlin, Schreiweis, Naumann, Burbano, Meyer, Laluela-Fox, de la Rasilla, Rosas, Gajovic, Kelso, Enard, Schaffner and Pääbo2013). It resulted in a “selective sweep.” Selective sweeps on genetic mutations, such as those that confer adult lactose tolerance (Tishkoff et al. Reference Tishkoff, Reed, Ranciaro, Voight, Babbitt, Silverman, Powell, Mortensen, Hirbo, Osman, Ibrahim, Omar, Lema, Nyambo, Ghori, Bumpstead, Pritchard, Wray and Deloukas2007), occur when a mutation enhances the survival of progeny. One of the tenets of neurophysiology is that synaptic plasticity is the key to learning anything. Virtually all human knowledge is transmitted through the medium of language, and FOXP2 appears to have played a role in the evolution of human language by enhancing basal ganglia synaptic plasticity and connectivity.

It is puzzling that Ackermann et al., disputing my views on the physiology of speech production, included a direct quotation from page 289 of my 2006 book, Toward an Evolutionary Biology of Language. In pages 131 to 245 of this book I discuss, in detail, the issues noted above and other points raised by Ackermann et al.

References

Blumstein, S. E. (1995) The neurobiology of language. In: Speech, language and communication, ed. Miller, J. & Eimas, P. D., pp. 339370. Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumstein, S. E., Cooper, W. E., Goodglass, H., Statlender, S. & Gottlieb, J. E. (1980) Production deficits in aphasia: A voice-onset time analysis. Brain and Language 9:153–70.Google Scholar
Chen, L.-M. & Kent, R. D. (2009) Development of prosodic patterns in Mandarin-learning infants. Journal of Child Language 36(1):7384.Google Scholar
Esteve-Gibert, N. & Prieto, P. (2013) Prosody signals the emergence of intentional communication in the first year of life: Evidence from Catalan-babbling infants. Journal of Child Language 40(5):919–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitch, W. T. (2010) The evolution of language. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flowers, K. A. & Robertson, C. (1985) The effects of Parkinson's disease on the ability to maintain a mental set. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 48(6):517–29.Google Scholar
Grossman, M., Carvell, S., Gollomp, S., Stern, M. B., Vernon, G. & Hurtig, H. I. (1991) Sentence comprehension and praxis deficits in Parkinson's disease. Neurology 41(10):1620–26.Google Scholar
Harrington, D. L. & Haaland, K. Y. (1991) Sequencing in Parkinson's disease: Abnormalities programming and controlling movement. Brain 114:99115.Google ScholarPubMed
Iwatsubo, T., Kuzuhara, S., Kanemitsu, A., Shimada, H. & Toyokura, Y. (1990) Corticofugal projections to the motor nuclei of the brainstem and spinal cord in humans. Neurology 40(2):309–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jürgens, U. (2002b) Neural pathways underlying vocal control. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 26:235–58.Google Scholar
Kuypers, H. G. J. M. (1958a) Corticobulbar connection to the pons and lower brain-stem in man. Brain 81:364–88.Google Scholar
Lange, K. W., Robbins, T. W., Marsden, C. D., James, M., Owen, A. & Paul, G. M. (1992) L-dopa withdrawal in Parkinson's disease selectively impairs cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. Psychopharmacology 107:394404.Google Scholar
Lehericy, S., Ducros, M., Van de Moortele, P-F., Francois, C. L., Thivard, L., Poupon, C., Swindale, N., Ugurbil, K. & Kim, D. S. (2004) Diffusion tensor fiber tracking shows distinct corticostriatal circuits in humans. Annals of Neurology 55(4):522–29.Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P. & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967) Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review 74:431–61.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2000) Human language and our reptilian brain: The subcortical bases of speech, syntax, and thought. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2002) On the nature and evolution of the neural bases of human language. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 45:3662.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2006b) Toward an evolutionary biology of language. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2009) FOXP2 and human cognition. Cell 137:800802.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2012) Vocal tract anatomy and the neural bases of talking. Journal of Phonetics 40:608–22.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (2013) The unpredictable species: What makes humans unique. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., Friedman, J. & Feldman, L. S. (1990) Syntactic deficits in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 178:360–65.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., Kako, E. T., Friedman, J., Tajchman, G., Feldman, L. S. & Jiminez, E. B. (1992) Speech production, syntax comprehension, and cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. Brain and Language 43:169–89.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., Kanki, B. G., Protopapas, A., Reed, E. & Youngs, J. W. (1994) Cognitive defects at altitude. Nature 372:325.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., Morey, A., Hochstadt, J., Larson, M. & Mather, S. (2005) Mount Everest: A space-analog for speech monitoring of cognitive deficits and stress. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine 76:198207.Google Scholar
Maricic, T., Günther, V., Georgiev, O., Gehre, S., Curlin, M., Schreiweis, C., Naumann, R., Burbano, H. A., Meyer, M., Laluela-Fox, C., de la Rasilla, M., Rosas, A., Gajovic, S., Kelso, J., Enard, W., Schaffner, W. & Pääbo, S. (2013) A recent evolutionary change affects a regulatory element in the human FOXP2 gene. Molecular Biology and Evolution 30(4):844–52. doi: 10.1093/molbev/mss271.Google Scholar
Marsden, C. D. & Obeso, J. A. (1994) The functions of the basal ganglia and the paradox of sterotaxic surgery in Parkinson's disease. Brain 117:877–97.Google Scholar
Monchi, O., Petrides, M., Petre, V., Worsley, K. & Dagher, A. (2001) Wisconsin card sorting revisited: Distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience 21(19):7733–41.Google Scholar
Naeser, M. A., Alexander, M. P., Helm-Estabrooks, N., Levine, H. L., Laughlin, S. A. & Geschwind, N. (1982) Aphasia with predominantly subcortical lesion sites: Description of three capsular/putaminal aphasia syndromes. Archives of Neurology 39(1):214.Google Scholar
Natsopoulos, D., Grouios, G., Bostantzopoulou, S., Mentenopoulos, G., Katsarou, Z. & Logothetis, J. (1993) Algorithmic and heuristic strategies in comprehension of complement clauses by patients with Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 31(9):951–64.Google Scholar
Nauta, W. J. H. & Gygax, P. A. (1954) Silver impregnation of degenerating axons in the central nervous system: A modified technic. Stain Technology 29:9193.Google Scholar
Pickett, E. R., Kuniholm, E., Protopapas, A., Friedman, J. & Lieberman, P. (1998) Selective speech motor, syntax and cognitive deficits associated with bilateral damage to the putamen and the head of the caudate nucleus: A case study. Neuropsychologia 36:173–88.Google Scholar
Simard, F., Joanette, Y., Petrides, M. Jubault, T., Madjar, C. & Monchi, O. (2011) Fronto-striatal contributions to lexical set-shifting. Cerebral Cortex 21(5):1084–93.Google Scholar
Stuss, D. T., Benson, D. F. (1986) The frontal lobes. Raven.Google Scholar
Terao, S., Li, M., Hashizume, Y., Osano, Y, Mitsuma, T. & Sobue, G. (1997) Upper motor neuron lesions in stroke patients do not induce anterograde transneuronal degeneration in spinal anterior horn cells. Stroke 28(12):2553–56.Google Scholar
Tishkoff, S. A., Reed, F., Ranciaro, A., Voight, B. F., Babbitt, C. C., Silverman, J. S., Powell, K., Mortensen, H. M., Hirbo, J. B., Osman, M., Ibrahim, M., Omar, S. A., Lema, G., Nyambo, T. B., Ghori, J., Bumpstead, S., Pritchard, J. K., Wray, G. A. & Deloukas, P. (2007) Convergent adaptation of human lactase persistence in Africa and Europe. Nature Genetics 39(1):3140.Google Scholar
Usui, C., Inoue, Y., Kimura, M., Kirino, E., Nagaoka, S., Abe, M., Nagata, T. & Arai, H. (2004) Irreversible subcortical dementia following high altitude illness. High Altitude Medicine and Biology 5(1):7781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed