Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T18:54:46.278Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does early motor development contribute to speech perception?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2017

Dawoon Choi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. jwerker@psych.ubc.cahttp://infantstudies.psych.ubc.ca
Padmapriya Kandhadai
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. jwerker@psych.ubc.cahttp://infantstudies.psych.ubc.ca
D. Kyle Danielson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. jwerker@psych.ubc.cahttp://infantstudies.psych.ubc.ca
Alison G. Bruderer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. jwerker@psych.ubc.cahttp://infantstudies.psych.ubc.ca
Janet F. Werker
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. jwerker@psych.ubc.cahttp://infantstudies.psych.ubc.ca

Abstract

At the end of the target article, Keven & Akins (K&A) put forward a challenge to the developmental psychology community to consider the development of complex psychological processes – in particular, intermodal infant perception – across different levels of analysis. We take up that challenge and consider the possibility that early emerging stereotypies might help explain the foundations of the link between speech perception and speech production.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Following their detailed elucidation of early appearing stereotypies in tongue movements, Keven & Akins (K&A) challenged the developmental psychology community to reconsider other early appearing complex psychological processes, such as intermodal speech perception, from this perspective. In this commentary, we take up that challenge by considering the role of early motor development in speech perception. Specifically, we address the question of whether the steps in prenatal and early postnatal development in the infant's developing control over tongue movement could provide information about the size, shape, and movement capabilities of the oral articulatory apparatus, and whether this in turn could provide a mapping onto which both heard and seen speech can be projected. We use as reference previous work from our lab showing that temporarily impeding tongue movement interferes with consonant discrimination in young infants (Bruderer et al. Reference Bruderer, Danielson, Kandhadai and Werker2015).

K&A provide persuasive argumentation that tongue protrusion and retraction (TP/R), and a host of other behaviours that are categorized as stereotypies, are functionally important for setting the foundation of the aerodigestive system. The stereotypies fall into a class of processes that is spontaneous and activity dependent. The result of these processes in development, they argue, is an initial organization of the somatosensory and motor cortices relevant for successful coordination and movements of oral-motor articulators involving the mouth and tongue, such as cupping, sucking, breathing, and swallowing. We put forward for consideration the hypothesis that these same TP/R and other related stereotypies may simultaneously provide the infant with a mapping of the shape and configurability of the (upper) vocal tract, and how that changes with movement of the articulators. This, in turn, although not necessarily essential for speech perception as it is for aerodigestion, may underlie the infant's integration of heard, seen, and self-produced oral-motor movements even before the onset of babbling.

There is experimental evidence of sensorimotor influences on speech perception in the first six months of life. As highlighted in the target article, infants as young as 4 months (Kuhl & Meltzoff Reference Kuhl and Meltzoff1982) and even 2 months (Patterson & Werker Reference Patterson and Werker2003) can match auditory and visual speech, looking longer to the face that matches the sound they are hearing. At 4 to 5 months of age, infants' auditory-visual matching of the vowels “oo” and “ee” is modified if infants purse their lips around a pacifier or their caregiver's finger (into an “oo” configuration) versus if the infants' lips are stretched by a teething toy or their caregiver's finger (into an “ee” configuration) (Yeung & Werker Reference Yeung and Werker2013).

It has also long been known that from birth infants can discriminate many non-native, and therefore unfamiliar speech sounds, and that an important part of speech perception development entails a decline in non-native discrimination by the end of the first year of life (see Werker & Hensch Reference Werker and Hensch2015). Recently, we found that at 6 months of age, impeding tongue movement can disrupt auditory discrimination of non-native consonants (Bruderer et al. Reference Bruderer, Danielson, Kandhadai and Werker2015). We tested infants' ability to discriminate the Hindi dental /d̪a/ versus retroflex /ɖa/ distinction, a non-native, and therefore unfamiliar distinction that English-learning infants can discriminate at 6 months of age. These consonants are produced by placing the tongue tip either against the back of the front teeth (dental /d̪/), or against the roof of the mouth (retroflex /ɖ/). We prevented tongue movements in the experimental condition by having caregivers hold a flat teether over their infants' tongues, and allowed tongue movements in the control condition by having caregivers hold a soft u-shaped teether against their infants' gums. Tongue-impeded infants failed to discriminate the non-native speech sound distinction, whereas tongue-unimpeded infants successfully discriminated the consonant contrast.

Neuroimaging studies using diffusion track imaging (DTI) in neonates show that the establishment of white matter fiber tract connections that characterize the language pathways in the adult brain has already begun in utero (Dubois et al. Reference Dubois, Poupon, Thirion, Simonnet, Kulikova, Leroy, Hertz-Pannier and Dehaene-Lambertz2015; Leroy et al. Reference Leroy, Glasel, Dubois, Hertz-Pannier, Thirion, Mangin and Dehaene-Lambertz2011; Perani et al. Reference Perani, Saccuman, Scifo, Anwander, Spada, Baldoli, Poloniato, Lohmann and Friederici2011). Just 2 days after birth, newborn brains are marked by an established ventral pathway, which will later be responsible for sound-to-meaning mapping; a superior dorsal pathway, extending from the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) to the premotor areas; and a diminutive inferior dorsal pathway that connects the pSTS to Broca's area (Perani et al. Reference Perani, Saccuman, Scifo, Anwander, Spada, Baldoli, Poloniato, Lohmann and Friederici2011). These dorsal speech pathways underlie speech sound-to-motor processing (e.g., Poeppel Reference Poeppel2012). Within the first few postnatal months, the maturation of the inferior dorsal pathway outpaces that of any other regions within the language network (Leroy et al. Reference Leroy, Glasel, Dubois, Hertz-Pannier, Thirion, Mangin and Dehaene-Lambertz2011). Indeed, motor areas in the inferior frontal cortex – those connected by the inferior dorsal pathway to the auditory cortex – are activated by 7 months of age when infants listen to speech (Kuhl et al. Reference Kuhl, Ramirez, Bosseler, Lin and Imada2014). The presence of these dorsal pathways may be key to our reported behavioural results suggesting motor influences on auditory-visual (Yeung & Werker Reference Yeung and Werker2013) and auditory speech perception (Bruderer et al. Reference Bruderer, Danielson, Kandhadai and Werker2015).

Prenatal preparation for speech is evident not only in DTI studies of language networks, but also in auditory specialization to both simple sounds and speech sounds by 28–32 weeks gestational age (wGA) as tested in preterm infants with electroencephalography (EEG) (Mahmoudzadeh et al. Reference Mahmoudzadeh, Wallois, Kongolo, Goudjil and Dehaene-Lambertz2017). Orofacial movements in utero, including TP/R (which is well defined by 28 wGA), could help establish the link between the motor cortex and the auditory cortices. The reported existence of the early appearing dorsal pathway prior to birth (Perani et al. Reference Perani, Saccuman, Scifo, Anwander, Spada, Baldoli, Poloniato, Lohmann and Friederici2011), together with the rapid development of the inferior dorsal pathway in the months following birth (Leroy et al. Reference Leroy, Glasel, Dubois, Hertz-Pannier, Thirion, Mangin and Dehaene-Lambertz2011), provides a plausible means by which sound-to-motor mapping could be established prior to, or immediately upon first experience with, linguistic experiential input.

Stereotypies, including TP/R, may support the functional specialization of the speech pathways specifically dedicated to auditory-motor mapping. A key notion in the target article is that stereotypies, exemplified by TP/R, are activity-dependent processes that contribute to the organization of the somatotopy of the tongue and the lips, as well as the cortico-thalamic (Deck et al. Reference Deck, Lokmane, Chauvet, Mailhes, Keita, Niquille, Yoshida, Yoshida, Lebrand, Mann, Grove and Garel2013) and the corticobulbar connections (Sarnat Reference Sarnat2003) (pp. 50). The onset of TP/R occurs too late in development to inform neural migration, neurogenesis, or axon pathfinding to S1, but the functional circuitry of S1 is only beginning to be established when these stereotypies first appear (pp. 49). Whereas prenatal structural connections of the linguistic pathways are largely genetically determined (Kolasinski et al. Reference Kolasinski, Takahashi, Stevens, Benner, Fischl, Zöllei and Grant2013), spontaneous activity-dependent processes may contribute to their functional connectivity and integration. Therefore, the dorsal speech pathways that are present at birth may have been organized in part by the stereotypies that first appear in utero.

In summary, as K&A suggest, “infants do not ‘explore the world by mouth’ so much as explore their mouths with the world” (sect. 4.4, para. 3). This experience-expectant process (Greenough Reference Greenough, Greenough and Juraska1986) through which self-produced stereotypies and externally heard speech interact with emerging anatomical connections may be the means by which early links between perception and production – even prior to babbling – are established.

References

Bruderer, A. G., Danielson, D. K., Kandhadai, P. & Werker, J. F. (2015) Sensorimotor influences on speech perception in infancy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112(44):13531–36. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1508631112.Google Scholar
Deck, M., Lokmane, L., Chauvet, S., Mailhes, C., Keita, M., Niquille, M., Yoshida, M., Yoshida, Y., Lebrand, C., Mann, F., Grove, E. A. & Garel, S. (2013) Pathfinding of corticothalamic axons relies on a rendezvous with thalamic projections. Neuron 77(3):472–84. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.031.Google Scholar
Dubois, J., Poupon, C., Thirion, B., Simonnet, H., Kulikova, S., Leroy, F., Hertz-Pannier, L. & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2015) Exploring the early organization and maturation of linguistic pathways in the human infant brain. Cerebral Cortex 26(5):2283–98. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv082.Google Scholar
Greenough, W. T. (1986) What's special about development? Thoughts on the bases of experience-sensitive synaptic plasticity. In: Developmental neuropsychobiology, ed. Greenough, W. T. & Juraska, J. M., pp. 387407. Academic.Google Scholar
Kolasinski, J., Takahashi, E., Stevens, A. A., Benner, T., Fischl, B., Zöllei, L. & Grant, P. E. (2013) Radial and tangential neuronal migration pathways in the human fetal brain: Anatomically distinct patterns of diffusion MRI coherence. Neuroimage 79:412–22. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.125.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. & Meltzoff, A. (1982) The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science 218(4577):1138–41.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Ramirez, R. R., Bosseler, A., Lin, J. F. L. & Imada, T. (2014) Infant brain responses to speech suggest analysis by synthesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 111(31):11238–45.Google Scholar
Leroy, F., Glasel, H., Dubois, J., Hertz-Pannier, L., Thirion, B., Mangin, J. F. & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2011) Early maturation of the linguistic dorsal pathway in human infants. The Journal of Neuroscience 31(4):1500–506. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4141-10.2011.Google Scholar
Mahmoudzadeh, M., Wallois, F., Kongolo, G., Goudjil, S. & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2017) Functional maps at the onset of auditory inputs in very early preterm human neonates. Cerebral Cortex 27(4):2500–12. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw103.Google Scholar
Patterson, M. L. & Werker, J. F. (2003) Two-month-old infants match phonetic information in lips and voice. Developmental Science 6(2):191–96. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00271.Google Scholar
Perani, D., Saccuman, M. C., Scifo, P., Anwander, A., Spada, D., Baldoli, C., Poloniato, A., Lohmann, G. & Friederici, A. D. (2011) Neural language networks at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 108(38):16056–61. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1102991108.Google Scholar
Poeppel, D. (2012) The maps problem and the mapping problem: Two challenges for a cognitive neuroscience of speech and language. Cognitive Neuropsychology 29(1–2):3455. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2012.710600.Google Scholar
Sarnat, H. B. (2003) Functions of the corticospinal and corticobulbar tracts in the human newborn. Journal of Pediatric Neurology 1(01):38.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Hensch, T. K. (2015) Critical periods in speech perception: New directions. Annual Review of Psychology 66:173–96. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015104.Google Scholar
Yeung, H. H. & Werker, J. F. (2013) Lip movements affect infant audiovisual speech perception. Psychological Science 24(5):603–12. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612458802.Google Scholar