Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-l4dxg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T18:48:50.878Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Multisensory control of ingestive movements and the myth of food addiction in obesity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2017

David A. Booth*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QH, United Kingdom. d.a.booth@sussex.ac.ukhttp://www.sussex.ac.uk/profiles/335100

Abstract

Some individuals have a neurogenetic vulnerability to developing strong facilitation of ingestive movements by learned configurations of biosocial stimuli. Condemning food as addictive is mere polemic, ignoring the contextualised sensory control of the mastication of each mouthful. To beat obesity, the least fattening of widely recognised eating patterns needs to be measured and supported.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Keven & Akins (K&A) use recent evidence on the development of respiratory and ingestive movements to criticise claims that mimicry of tongue protrusion plays a role in attachment to carers. This commentary applies their criticisms to the notion that addiction to ingestion makes people unhealthily fat. Both sets of ideas are symptomatic of a syndrome of “multisensory neglect” in research. Ignorance of the configured biological and societal stimuli to each mouthful of food or drink largely accounts for the continued failure to reduce the contribution of excess energy intake to obesity and the resulting disease, disability, and distress.

Ingestion of mouthfuls is shaped and contextualised by diverse interactions among the external and internal senses (Booth Reference Booth1985; Booth et al. Reference Booth, Sharpe, Freeman and Conner2011b). As K&A describe, using just the senses within the mouth, movements of the tongue rapidly become efficient at drawing the nipple along the upper lip to the hard palate. It should be noted that a few sessions of suckling are sufficient to change the full stretch of the tongue out of the mouth (K&A Fig. 2[a]) to a slight protrusion between the lips and side-to-side movements (Steiner et al. Reference Steiner, Glaser, Hawilo and Berridge2001). These sights of the tongue in the absence of the nipple show vacuum ingestive activity, anticipatory to the tactile context of the breast between the lips and the nipple protruding into the mouth. Without independent evidence from emotional behaviour and autonomic physiology, there is no warrant for attributing sensual pleasure to the neonate from the taste of sugar on the tongue (Booth et al. Reference Booth, Higgs, Schneider and Klinkenberg2010; Booth Reference Booth2016).

K&A could have written more about the changing multisensory contexts of movement patterns as they mature. For example, in their opening paragraph, they imply that stepping disappears because of relative lack of leg muscle. The fuller account is that learned integration of gravity into the control of stepping central pattern generators (CPGs) can only begin when the legs are strong enough. To walk or run, the stepping CPGs have to be contextualised by learned coordination of proprioception with balance, touch, and sight. K&A recognise a supportive role for gravity in swallowing but could assert its necessity for locomotion.

In another of K&A's examples, the infant's orientation to a face, gaze is potently drawn and held by the iris, eyelashes, and eyebrow of each eye (sometimes plus spectacles!) by centre-surround connections in the retina and primary visual cortex (V1) on which all visual recognition depends. Talk of attractiveness, reward value, or pleasure in the eyes is otiose. We don't accuse extreme extroverts of addiction to socialising.

Consumption of drinks and foods requires vastly more complex sensory control of the movements holding in the hands, sipping, biting, masticating, and swallowing. K&A cite re-setting of the swallowing CPG by the sight of food (Leopold & Daniels Reference Leopold and Daniels2009). That word “food” hides the variety of shapes, sizes, and compressibilities of the solid and semi-solid items that the eater ingested previously, plus unique mixtures of soluble and volatile compounds (flavours), different temperatures, and haptic microstructures: that is, oral textures – both tactile (Booth Reference Booth and Dickinson2005) and auditory (Mobini et al. Reference Mobini, Platts and Booth2011). When the infant begins to select mouthfuls, the hands, vocal tract, and whole body become coordinated with the jaw, lips, tongue, and pharynx following visual anticipation of the item's multisensory identity. The appearance of an item of food is configured in memory with the levels of all its other distinguishing physical characteristics and cultural attributes (Booth & Freeman Reference Booth and Freeman1993; Booth et al. Reference Booth, Sharpe, Freeman and Conner2011b).

Therefore, an ingestive mechanism can be identified only when the social and physiological influences are specified. In research on ingestive behaviour, however, mere licking of the lips, curling of the tongue, amounts eaten, or ratings of eating are given empirically empty labels such as regulation, motivation, pleasure, hunger, and satiety, and are even assigned generic functions like reward, working memory, attention, and expectation. This systematic over-interpretation diverts thought and effort from measuring the multitude of highly specific interactions across and within sensory modalities that determine ingestion.

Investigators may implicate a sensory modality, and even a category of transduction (such as yellow color or sweet taste), but that is not enough, as K&A point out in conclusion. Action towards each sort of food or drink (or any other object) requires each afferent and efferent channel to be at a particular level of activity (e.g., Booth et al. Reference Booth, Freeman, Konle, Wainwright and Sharpe2011a). The information content which each channel transmits is combined into a limited number of types of quantitative comparison between present and past output-input relationships (Booth Reference Booth2013a). Notional cognitive-affective functions dissolve into actual causal processes within the individual's mind.

Until multisensory integration is specified, its neural basis must remain obscure. The medial edge of the subthalamic striatum, Nucleus accumbens, organises sequencing of ingestive movements via inhibitory interneurons on CPG systems. In the part involved in tongue protrusions, some cells are inhibited by tasting sucrose and excited by taste of quinine (Roitman et al. Reference Roitman, Wheeler and Carelli2005). However, such isolated tastes, smells, and textures cannot elucidate the contextualised use of combinations of specific levels of gustatory, olfactory, tactile, auditory, and proprioceptive stimuli, let alone of equally crucial signals from the viscera and the visual field (Booth Reference Booth2013b; Reference Booth and Hirsch2015).

Parents' various ratings of their infants' eating measure a single trait of responsiveness to foods, which relates to at least one of the many genes associated with obesity (Wardle & Carnell Reference Wardle and Carnell2009). After the age of 30, dopaminergic hyperactivity is associated with higher body mass index (Dang et al. Reference Dang, Samanez-Larkin, Castrellon, Perkins, Cowan and Zald2016). Dopaminergic synapses lower thresholds and raise gain in the striatum, increasing the precision of processing of sensory characteristics (Warren et al. Reference Warren, Eldar, van den Brink, Tona, van der Wee, Giltay, van Noorden, Bosch, Wilson, Cohen and Nieuwenhuis2016). That is, dopamine activity reflects responsiveness to food stimuli, as part of arousal, not the reward of learning (Benton & Young Reference Benton and Young2016; Kroemer & Small Reference Kroemer and Small2016).

To combat obesity, we need activities under multisensory control to be described in eaters' terms, not in terms only of nutrients (Booth et al. Reference Booth, Blair, Lewis and Baek2004; Booth & Laguna-Camacho Reference Booth and Laguna-Camacho2015). Evidence on which sustained changes do most for keeping slim can then be collected and disseminated (Booth & Booth Reference Booth and Booth2011).

References

Benton, D. & Young, H. A. (2016) A meta-analysis of the relationship between brain dopamine receptors and obesity: A matter of changes in behavior rather than food addiction? International Journal of Obesity 40:S1221. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2016.9.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. (1985) Food-conditioned eating preferences and aversions with interoceptive elements: Conditioned appetites and satieties. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 443:2237. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb27061.x.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. (2005) Perceiving the texture of a food: Biomechanical and cognitive mechanisms and their measurement. In: Food colloids: Interactions, microstructure and processing, ed. Dickinson, E., pp. 339–55. Royal Society of Chemistry. Available at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/236986523.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. (2013a) How a mind works. A fundamental theory of the individual's action, perception, emotion and thought. Unpublished Working Paper. Available at: https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.1.1479.6569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, D. A. (2013b) Configuring of extero- and interoceptive senses in actions on food. Multisensory Research 26:123–42. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1163/22134808-00002395.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. (2015) Chemosensory influences on eating and drinking, and their cognitive mediation. In: Nutrition and chemosensation, ed. Hirsch, A. R., pp. 221–94. CRC Press. Available at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/259344349.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. (2016) “I like it!” Preference actions separated from hedonic reactions. Journal of Sensory Studies 31:213–32. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/joss.12205.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A., Blair, A. J., Lewis, V. J. & Baek, S. H. (2004) Patterns of eating and movement that best maintain reduction in overweight. Appetite 43:277–83. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2004.06.007.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. & Booth, P. (2011) Targeting cultural changes supportive of the healthiest lifestyle patterns. A biosocial evidence-base for prevention of obesity. Appetite 56:210–21. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.12.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, D. A. & Freeman, R. P. J. (1993) Discriminative feature integration by individuals. Acta Psychologica 84:116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, D. A., Freeman, R. P. J., Konle, M., Wainwright, C. J. & Sharpe, O. (2011a) Perception as interacting psychophysical functions. Could the configuring of features replace a specialised receptor? Perception 40:509–29. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1068/p6688.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A., Higgs, S., Schneider, J. & Klinkenberg, I. (2010) Learned liking versus inborn delight. Can sweetness give sensual pleasure or is it just motivating? Psychological Science 21:1656–63. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610385356.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. & Laguna-Camacho, A. (2015) Physical versus psychosocial measurement of influences on obesity. Comment on Dhurandhar et al. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders 39(7):1177–78. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2015.62.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A., Sharpe, O., Freeman, R. P. J. & Conner, M. T. (2011b) Insight into sight, touch, taste and smell by multiple discriminations from norm. Seeing and Perceiving 24(5):485511. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1163/187847511X588773.Google Scholar
Dang, L. C., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Castrellon, J. J., Perkins, S. F., Cowan, R. L., Zald, D. H. (2016) Associations between dopamine D2 receptor availability and BMI depend on age. Neuroimage 138:176–83. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.044.Google Scholar
Kroemer, N. B. & Small, D. M. (2016) Fuel not fun: Reinterpreting attenuated brain responses to reward in obesity. Physiology and Behavior 162:3745. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.04.020.Google Scholar
Leopold, N. A. & Daniels, S. K. (2009) Supranuclear control of swallowing. Dysphagia 25(3):250–57. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00455-009-9249-5.Google Scholar
Mobini, S., Platts, R. G. & Booth, D. A. (2011) Haptic signals of texture while eating a food. Multisensory cognition as interacting discriminations from norm. Appetite 56(2):386–93. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.12.024.Google Scholar
Roitman, M. F., Wheeler, R. A. & Carelli, R. M. (2005) Nucleus accumbens neurons are innately tuned for rewarding and aversive taste stimuli, encode their predictors, and are linked to motor output. Neuron 45(4):587–97. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.055.Google Scholar
Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E. & Berridge, K. C. (2001) Comparative expression of hedonic impact: Affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35:5374. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00051-8.Google Scholar
Wardle, J. & Carnell, S. (2009) Appetite is a heritable phenotype associated with adiposity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 38(S1):S25S30. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-009-9116-5.Google Scholar
Warren, C. M., Eldar, E., van den Brink, R. L., Tona, K.-D., van der Wee, N. J., Giltay, E. J., van Noorden, M. S., Bosch, J. A., Wilson, R. C., Cohen, J. D. & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2016) Catecholamine-mediated increases in gain enhance the precision of cortical representations. Journal of Neuroscience 36(21):5699–708. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3475-15.2016.Google Scholar