Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T15:25:55.362Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Studying the relationship between dreaming and sleep-dependent memory processes: Methodological challenges

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 November 2013

Michael Schredl*
Affiliation:
Sleep Laboratory, Central Institute of Mental Health, D-68159 Mannheim, Germany. Michael.Schredl@zi-mannheim.dehttp://www.dreamresearch.de

Abstract

The hypothesis that dreaming is involved in off-line memory processing is difficult to test because major methodological issues have to be addressed, such as dream recall and the effect of remembered dreams on memory. It would be fruitful – in addition to studying the ancient art of memory (AAOM) in a scanner – to study the dreams of persons who use AAOM regularly.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Based on the analogy of the ancient art of memory (AAOM) techniques with dream content, Llewellyn hypothesizes in the target article that an off-line memory processing – termed elaborative encoding– is taking place during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and that this is instigated during non-REM (NREM) sleep and facilitates the recall of episodic memories. That there is more to off-line memory processing than simple consolidation (repeating the newly acquired information exactly) is in line with the current theories (see Stickgold & Walker Reference Stickgold and Walker2013). The major challenge posed by this hypothesis is the idea that dreaming, especially REM dreaming, reflects or participates in this function of REM sleep since one can argue that a lot of processes occur during this sleep stage, not all of which are reflected in dreaming – if recalled.

Looking at the AAOM examples, the parallel between AAOM and dream content is less than clear for me: According to Llewellyn, AAOM techniques were used to learn episodic memories that were not related directly to the person by combining those memories with personal material. Dreaming, however, often reflects personal concerns (Domhoff Reference Domhoff2003) and emotional salient daytime experiences (Schredl Reference Schredl2006).

The parallel between dreams and AAOM also does not explain why dreams are quite easily forgotten (Schredl Reference Schredl, Barrett and McNamara2007). It is important to keep in mind that dreams are only memories of the dreaming process (Schredl Reference Schredl2008). One study (Schredl Reference Schredl2000), for example, did not find a correlation between dream salience, dream bizarreness,, and dream recall frequency. The theory that best explains the problems the waking mind has with recalling dreams was proposed by Koukkou and Lehmann (Reference Koukkou and Lehmann1983). The functional state-shift theory is based on the empirical evidence on state-dependent learning: the waking state differs from the REM-sleep state regarding cholinergic modulation (Hobson et al. Reference Hobson, Pace-Schott and Stickgold2000), and thus there are difficulties in remembering the dreamed experiences. In addition, the fact that the sleep profile of the night shows brief REM periods at the beginning with short, mundane dreams and longer REM periods with the more bizarre and elaborate dreams at the end of the night (Antrobus Reference Antrobus1991; Dement & Kleitman Reference Dement and Kleitman1957a) does not completely support the idea of instigating the REM sleep processes in subsequent NREM sleep. Furthermore, sleep-onset dreams, NREM dreams, and even daydreams often cannot be differentiated from REM dreams on a phenomenological level (Foulkes & Fleisher Reference Foulkes and Fleisher1975; Nielsen Reference Nielsen2000). The Kekulé example cited by Llewellyn (sect. 4.2.1, para. 6), for instance, is a sleep-onset dream (Strunz Reference Strunz1993). These issues are, however, not as important as is the question about how to test whether dreaming – defined as subjective experiencing during sleep – has a function additional to the function of sleep or REM sleep.

To illustrate the problems within this line of research, I complement the overview of Llewellyn, who cited only the nap study by Wamsley et al. (Reference Wamsley, Tucker, Payne, Benavides and Stickgold2010). The first pilot study linking dream content to overnight memory consolidation was conducted by De Koninck et al. (Reference De Koninck, Prevost and Lortie-Lussier1996), who found that task-related dream content is related to increased performance in the procedural task (e.g., performing a task while wearing goggles that invert the visual field) the next day. Unfortunately, this result was not confirmed by a subsequent study using another procedural task: mirror tracing (Schredl & Erlacher Reference Schredl and Erlacher2010). A completely different approach was applied by Erlacher and Schredl (Reference Erlacher and Schredl2010): Lucid dreamers were instructed to train for a coin-tossing task within their dreams, and those who successfully did this exhibited improved performance the next morning.

The major problem with these studies is that you have to interrupt sleep (several REM awakenings) to get a clear picture of the person's dreams that night, and this might interfere with the off-line memory processes. If only spontaneously recalled dreams were elicited (Wamsley et al. Reference Wamsley, Tucker, Payne, Benavides and Stickgold2010), there is a problem that the trait-variable dream recall might be associated with memory performance and not the recall of a particular dream about the task. The main methodological issue, however, is that you have to elicit dream content (verbal or written reports) in order to know whether the person had dreamed about the memory task (or the episodic memories that the researcher is interested in), and thus it cannot be ruled out that the memory-enhancing effect is not due to the dreaming during the night or can be attributed only to the remembered dream. In the Erlacher and Schredl study, for example, the successful lucid dreamers might have been more confident because of their lucid dreaming and thus performed better in the task. Having these methodological issues in mind, the suggestions by Llewellyn as to how her hypothesis might be tested have to be evaluated very carefully. Studying patients with brain lesions who report loss of dreaming (Solms Reference Solms1997) has several drawbacks because one does not know what other cognitive processes were affected by the lesions. But the main question is how one can be sure that they are not dreaming and are only unable to recall any dreams.

If dreaming is defined as subjective experiencing during sleep (similar to the subjective experiencing during wakefulness), a complete cessation of dreaming might not be possible – at least in patients able to perform memory tasks or speak about their subjective processes while awake. The most promising approach suggested by Llewellyn is to study AAOM processes in the waking state by using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques and look to see whether similar brain activation patterns can be found during REM sleep. This, however, can still not prove that dreaming is involved in this off-line memory processing. As a dream researcher, I would like to study the dreams of persons who use AAOM techniques in their waking lives regularly or very gifted persons. If their dreams are completely different with regard to bizarreness or content related to episodic memories, this would not support the AAOM analogy to “normal” dreaming.

To summarize, the hypothesis proposed by Llewellyn is stimulating for the field, but many important methodological issues need to be addressed before making progress in this line of research.

References

Antrobus, J. S. (1991) Dreaming: Cognitive processes during cortical activation and high afferent thresholds. Psychological Review 98(1):96121.Google Scholar
De Koninck, J., Prevost, F. & Lortie-Lussier, M. (1996) Vertical inversion of the visual field and REM sleep mentation. Journal of Sleep Research 5:1620.Google Scholar
Dement, W. & Kleitman, N. (1957a) Cyclic variations in EEG during sleep and their relation to eye movements, body motility, and dreaming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 9:673–90.Google Scholar
Domhoff, G. W. (2003) The scientific study of dreams: Neural networks, cognitive development, and content analysis. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erlacher, D. & Schredl, M. (2010) Practicing a motor task in a lucid dream enhances subsequent performance: A pilot study. Sport Psychologist 24:157–67.Google Scholar
Foulkes, D. & Fleisher, S. (1975) Mental activity in relaxed wakefulness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 84:6675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F. & Stickgold, R. (2000) Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23(6):793842; discussion 904–1121.Google Scholar
Koukkou, M. & Lehmann, D. (1983) Dreaming: The functional state-shift hypothesis. British Journal of Psychiatry 142:221–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, T. A. (2000) A review of mentation in REM and NREM sleep: “Covert” REM sleep as a possible reconcilation of two opposing models. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23:851–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schredl, M. (2000) The relationship between dream recall and dream content: Negative evidence for the salience hypothesis. North American Journal of Psychology 2:243–46.Google Scholar
Schredl, M. (2006) Factors affecting the continuity between waking and dreaming: Emotional intensity and emotional tone of the waking-life event. Sleep and Hypnosis 8:15.Google Scholar
Schredl, M. (2007) Dream recall: Models and empirical data. In: The new science of dreaming, vol. 2: Content, recall, and personality correlates, ed. Barrett, D. & McNamara, P., pp. 79114. Praeger.Google Scholar
Schredl, M. (2008) Traum. Reinhardt/UTB.Google Scholar
Schredl, M. & Erlacher, D. (2010) Is sleep-dependent memory consolidation of a visuo-motor task related to dream content? International Journal of Dream Research 3:7479.Google Scholar
Solms, M. (1997) The neuropsychology of dreams: A clinical–anatomical study. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stickgold, R. & Walker, M. P. (2013) Sleep-dependent memory triage: Evolving generalization through selective processing. Nature Neuroscience 16(2):139–45.Google Scholar
Strunz, F. (1993) Preconscious mental activity and scientific problem-solving: A critique of the Kekule dream controvery. Dreaming 3:281–94.Google Scholar
Wamsley, E. J., Tucker, M., Payne, J. D., Benavides, J. A. & Stickgold, R. (2010) Dreaming of a learning task is associated with enhanced sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Current Biology 20:850–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed