Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T12:09:29.980Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Individual-level psychology and group-level traits

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2014

Michael Muthukrishna
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. michael@psych.ubc.caschaller@psych.ubc.cahttp://www2.psych.ubc.ca/~schaller/schaller.htm
Mark Schaller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. michael@psych.ubc.caschaller@psych.ubc.cahttp://www2.psych.ubc.ca/~schaller/schaller.htm

Abstract

Psychological research on social influence illuminates many mechanisms through which role differentiation and collaborative interdependence may affect cultural evolution. We focus here on psychological processes that produce specific patterns of asymmetric influence, which in turn can have predictable consequences for the emergence and transmission of group-level traits.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Smaldino makes a compelling case that role differentiation, collaborative interdependence, and emergent group-level traits play an important role in cultural evolution. Smaldino offers preliminary speculations about processes through which group-level traits might emerge, but a lot remains to be specified. What specific role differentiations have implications for the emergence of important group-level traits? What specific proximal mechanisms might account for the emergence of these group-level traits and for their change over time? How might these processes be affected by specific circumstances? Answers to these questions require input from the sciences that focus on proximal mechanisms.

It may be especially useful to draw upon insights from the psychological sciences – especially research that explores the many ways that individuals influence each other during interpersonal interactions. Particularly relevant are lines of research documenting specific ways that influence outcomes differ depending on the social context – including the roles occupied by the individuals involved (Cialdini & Goldstein Reference Cialdini and Goldstein2004; Hogg Reference Hogg, Fiske, Gilbert and Lindzey2010; Wood Reference Wood2000).

It is easy to overlook this literature when addressing questions about long-term cultural outcomes, because psychological inquiry focuses on the immediate actions and outcomes of individuals and rarely considers their population-level consequences. But there are exceptions to this disciplinary restraint (Resnick et al. Reference Resnick, Levine and Teasdale1991; Schaller & Crandall Reference Schaller and Crandall2003).

Consider, for example, research on dynamic social impact theory (DSIT; Harton & Bullock Reference Harton and Bullock2007). Drawing upon a few basic principles of social geography, social interaction, and social influence, DSIT shows how mutual influence that occurs during dyadic interactions has, over time, inevitable consequences for population-level outcomes. These emergent outcomes include changes in the popularity of beliefs and behaviors and changes in the extent to which these beliefs and behaviors correlate and cluster. Furthermore, DSIT shows how the psychology of social influence can create and sustain patterns of diversity within a cultural population. Diversity – of beliefs, behaviors, aptitudes, etc. – is an outcome of particular relevance here. As Smaldino observes, diversity sets the stage for collaboration, which can then give rise to new group-level traits. Second, because diversity is the fuel that fires the engine of evolution, it has implications for cultural evolution more generally.

One important reason why the psychology of social influence produces predictable population-level consequences is because, within any social interaction, influence is rarely symmetrical. Some individuals are more influential; some individuals are more influence-able. DSIT typically treats these influence asymmetries as random variation. But they are not just random. The psychological literature documents specific kinds of role differentiations that have specific implications for patterns of asymmetric influence; this, in turn, can have predictable long-term consequences for cultural diversity and other group-level traits.

Leadership roles are one obvious example. Smaldino speculates that leadership, and its consequences, is one likely means through which group-level traits emerge. There is an extensive empirical literature on the psychology of leadership and followership and implications for group outcomes (Van Vugt et al. Reference Van Vugt, Hogan and Kaiser2008). By applying insights from this literature, it will be possible to predict group-level traits with greater precision.

Other forms of asymmetric influence arise from a variety of other social distinctions – some obvious and some not – that connote differences in power, dominance, expertise, or prestige. These differences have consequences for individual-level cognition and additional consequences for social influence (Cheng et al. Reference Cheng, Tracy, Foulsham, Kingstone and Henrich2013; Fiske Reference Fiske, Fiske, Gilbert and Lindzey2010; Galinsky et al. Reference Galinsky, Magee, Gruenfeld, Whitson and Liljenquist2008). Because of these asymmetrical influence implications, there will be further consequences for the emergence and transmission of group-level traits.

It may also be productive to explore the implications of sex differences. The male/female distinction is perhaps the most fundamental form of collaborative interdependence within the human species (in the sense that men and women collaborate interdependently to produce offspring). The mating game is a dynamically unfolding process in which individuals' thoughts and actions are influenced by the presumed thoughts and actions of other men and women in the immediate vicinity. But men and women do not influence each other in exactly the same way; their influence is predictably asymmetrical. The implication, explored in research on “dynamical evolutionary psychology,” is that the specific distributions of men and women within a population, and the specific characteristics of those men and women (e.g., the extent to which they are available or unavailable as mates), can affect the emergent properties of the entire population (Kenrick et al. Reference Kenrick, Li and Butner2003). The implications of sex differences for group-level traits are not limited to the domain of mating behavior. The male/female distinction has profound implications for division of labor and distribution of knowledge across a wide range of behavioral domains (Fried Reference Fried1967; Wood & Eagly Reference Wood, Eagly, Fiske, Gilbert and Lindzey2010). Research that systematically integrates the psychological literatures on sex differences (Geary Reference Geary2010) and social interaction with Smaldino's perspective on group-level traits is likely to reveal additional novel implications for cultural evolution.

The psychological processes that govern social interaction and social influence – and lead to emergent group-level traits – are themselves moderated by additional features of local ecologies. For example: the prevalence of infectious diseases in the local ecology appears to have many relevant implications. Among other outcomes, the sociological and psychological bases of asymmetric influence – rigid status hierarchies, authoritarian attitudes, etc. – are more evident under circumstances of higher disease prevalence (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Trudeau and Schaller2011; Reference Murray, Schaller and Suedfeld2013). The implication is that when diseases pose less of a threat, a more diverse set of beliefs and behaviors are likely to be expressed and maintained within a population. So, by carefully considering the specific psychological processes that govern the emergence of group-level traits, we may also be able to more fully identify connections between ecological circumstances and cultural evolution.

The take-home message is this: to realize the vast potential of the perspective outlined by Smaldino, it will be helpful to draw more fully on the vast psychological literature on social influence. As a happy corollary, this kind of conceptual integration will benefit the psychological literature too.

References

Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., Foulsham, T., Kingstone, A. & Henrich, J. (2013) Two ways to the top: Evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 104:103–25.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B. & Goldstein, N. J. (2004) Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology 55:591621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, S. T. (2010) Interpersonal stratification: Status, power, and subordination. In: Handbook of social psychology, 5th ed., ed. Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T. & Lindzey, G., pp. 941–82. Wiley.Google Scholar
Fried, M. H. (1967) The evolution of political society: An essay in political anthropology. Random House.Google Scholar
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Gruenfeld, D. H., Whitson, J. A. & Liljenquist, K. A. (2008) Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95:1450–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geary, D. C. (2010) Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences, 2nd ed. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Harton, H. C. & Bullock, M. (2007) Dynamic social impact: A theory of the origins and evolution of culture. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 1:521–40.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A. (2010) Influence and leadership. In: Handbook of social psychology, 5th ed., ed. Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T. & Lindzey, G., pp. 1166–207. Wiley.Google Scholar
Kenrick, D. T., Li, N. P. & Butner, J. (2003) Dynamical evolutionary psychology: Individual decision rules and emergent social norms. Psychological Review 110:328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, D. R., Schaller, M. & Suedfeld, P. (2013) Pathogens and politics: Further evidence that parasite prevalence predicts authoritarianism. PloS One 8:e62275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, D. R., Trudeau, R. & Schaller, M. (2011) On the origins of cultural differences in conformity: Four tests of the pathogen prevalence hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37:318–29.Google Scholar
Resnick, L. B., Levine, J. M. & Teasdale, S. D., eds. (1991) Perspectives on socially shared cognition. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, M. & Crandall, C. S., eds. (2003) The psychological foundations of culture. Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Vugt, M., Hogan, R. & Kaiser, R. B. (2008) Leadership, followership, and evolution. American Psychologist 63:182–96.Google Scholar
Wood, W. (2000) Attitude change: Persuasion and social influence. Annual Review of Psychology 51:539–70.Google Scholar
Wood, W. & Eagly, A. H. (2010) Gender. In: Handbook of social psychology, 5th ed., ed. Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T. & Lindzey, G., pp. 629–67. Wiley.Google Scholar