Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T01:11:23.600Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The crow in the room: New Caledonian crows offer insight into the necessary and sufficient conditions for cumulative cultural evolution

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Alex H. Taylor
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland1010, New Zealand. Alexander.taylor@auckland.ac.nzhttps://www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/en/about/animal-minds.html
Sarah Jelbert
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, BristolBS8 1TU, UK. Sarah.jelbert@bristol.ac.ukhttp://www.bris.ac.uk/expsych/people/sarah-a-jelbert/overview.html

Abstract

New Caledonian (NC) crow populations have developed complex tools that show suggestive evidence of cumulative change. These tool designs, therefore, appear to be the product of cumulative technological culture (CTC). We suggest that tool-using NC crows offer highly useful data for current debates over the necessary and sufficient conditions for the emergence of CTC.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

The tools of New Caledonian (NC) crows offer some of best evidence for cumulative cultural evolution in non-human animals (Hunt & Gray Reference Hunt and Gray2003). These birds manufacture tools from a range of materials for extractive foraging and other purposes (Hunt Reference Hunt1996; St-Clair et al. Reference St Clair, Klump, Sugasawa, Higgott, Colegrave and Rutz2018; Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Hunt and Gray2012a; Reference Taylor, Miller and Gray2012b; Wimpenny et al. Reference Wimpenny, Weir and Kacelnik2011), and show considerable diversity in tool design across different populations, which cannot be explained by ecological conditions alone (Hunt & Gray Reference Hunt and Gray2003). Some wild NC crows incorporate hooks into their tool designs, a level of technology not seen in our closest relatives, the chimpanzee, and that appears to have only emerged in our species around 100 kya (Yellen et al. Reference Yellen, Brooks, Cornelissen, Mehlman and Stewart1995). In particular, NC crows incorporate naturally occurring barbs into the leaf tools they make from pandanus plants (Hunt Reference Hunt1996; Reference Hunt2000). These pandanus tools are particularly impressive because of their stepped design: crows cut steps into the edge of pandanus leaf tools as they fashion them, with the number of steps cut into the leaf edges showing large variation between populations. It is this variation in tool designs that provides strong evidence for cumulative cultural. These tool designs show evidence of cumulative improvement between populations, are not created by naïve juveniles and the variation between populations is not explained by ecological correlates (Hunt Reference Hunt2000; Hunt & Gray Reference Hunt and Gray2003; Kenward et al. Reference Kenward, Weir, Rutz and Kacelnik2005).

However, work on this bird species has failed to find evidence for many of the social cognitive mechanisms that have been claimed to be required for cumulative technological culture (CTC), such as imitation, teaching, and language (Jelbert et al. Reference Jelbert, Hosking, Taylor and Gray2018; Logan et al. Reference Logan, Breen, Taylor, Gray and Hoppitt2016). For example, Logan et al. (Reference Logan, Breen, Taylor, Gray and Hoppitt2016) presented a classic two-action task to NC crows, but found evidence only of stimulus enhancement and individual trial and error learning, rather than evidence of imitation. There is also currently little evidence for metacognition or theory of mind in this species (Taylor Reference Taylor2014).

Instead, recent work suggests that CTC in this species is best explained by a form of reverse engineering (or goal emulation), which we termed “mental template matching” (Jelbert et al. Reference Jelbert, Hosking, Taylor and Gray2018; Logan et al. Reference Logan, Breen, Taylor, Gray and Hoppitt2016). We have suggested that crows form a mental template of a particular tool design from observing or using tools made by conspecifics, and then reverse engineer this design when they manufacture their own tools. A recent paper-ripping study provides good evidence that NC crows have the cognitive capacity for mental template matching (Jelbert et al. Reference Jelbert, Hosking, Taylor and Gray2018). Crows that learned that either pre-made large or small pieces of card could be inserted into a vending machine to obtain food rewards, subsequently manufactured pieces of card that matched the sizes of the previously rewarded examples. This behaviour occurred even though subjects were rewarded at random for the items they manufactured at test, and there was no available tool design to copy, meaning only the crow's mental representation of the correct tool design could guide manufacture. The presence of this cognitive capacity, coupled with observations showing adult-like pandanus manufacture develops slowly over the first year of life (Holzhaider et al. Reference Holzhaider, Hunt and Gray2010b), that juveniles regularly borrow their parents tools (Holzhaider et al. Reference Holzhaider, Sibley, Taylor, Singh, Gray and Hunt2011), and the parallels between template matching and song bird learning (Jelbert et al. Reference Jelbert, Hosking, Taylor and Gray2018; Slater Reference Slater1983) provide strong support for the hypothesis that NC crows pass on and update tool designs through reverse engineering in the absence of advanced social cognition.

Importantly, although NC crows do not appear to have remarkable social cognition (Jelbert et al. Reference Jelbert, Singh, Gray and Taylor2015a; Reference Jelbert, Taylor and Gray2015b; Logan et al. Reference Logan, Breen, Taylor, Gray and Hoppitt2016), their technical reasoning abilities are substantial and have been the focus of much research. In line with Osiurak and Reynaud's (O&R) technical reasoning hypothesis, there is good evidence for a range of different types of technical intelligence in this species. NC crows show evidence for the ability to transfer causal knowledge between perceptually distinct problems (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Hunt, Holzhaider and Gray2007; Reference Taylor, Hunt, Medina and Gray2009), though it is not clear if this is via functional generalisation or true analogical reasoning. They also can mentally preplan solutions to problems (Gruber et al. Reference Gruber, Schiestl, Boeckle, Frohnwieser, Miller, Gray, Clayton and Taylor2019) and make inferences, such as reasoning by exclusion (Jelbert et al. Reference Jelbert, Singh, Gray and Taylor2015a; Reference Jelbert, Taylor and Gray2015b) and inferring the weight of novel objects from observing their movements in the wind (Jelbert et al. Reference Jelbert, Miller, Schiestl, Boeckle, Cheke, Gray and Clayton2019). There is also some evidence that NC crows might be capable of representing hidden causal mechanisms (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Hunt and Gray2012a; Reference Taylor, Miller and Gray2012b; but see Boogert et al. Reference Boogert, Arbilly, Muth and Seed2013; Dymond et al. Reference Dymond, Haselgrove and McGregor2013; Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Miller and Gray2013a; Reference Taylor, Miller and Gray2013b), but there is currently no conclusive evidence that they form representations of unobservable ones (Neilands et al. Reference Neilands, Jelbert, Breen, Schiestl and Taylor2016; Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Hunt, Medina and Gray2009), nor that these birds possess the ability to make causal interventions (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Cheke, Waismeyer, Meltzoff, Miller, Gopnik, Clayton and Gray2014; but see Jacobs et al. Reference Jacobs, von Bayern, Martin-Ordas, Rat-Fischer and Osvath2015; Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Cheke, Waismeyer, Meltzoff, Miller, Gopnik and Gray2015).

This pattern of results provides strong general support for the technical reasoning hypothesis raised by O&R. NC crows show a profile of high technical reasoning abilities, less developed social intelligence, yet strong indications of CTC. This profile is in line with the hypothesis that CTC initially arises because of technical intelligence, rather than social intelligence. However, it also suggests that not all the technical reasoning skills of humans are necessary and sufficient for CTC. Work on NC crows suggests that CTC can emerge not only in the absence of imitation, teaching, and language, but also in the absence of analogical reasoning, reasoning about unobservable causal mechanisms, and possibly also the ability to make causal interventions. Instead, it appears that a more limited causal understanding, along with functional generalisation, inference, and mental simulation, might be a sufficient suite of technical intelligence skills for cumulative cultural evolution to emerge via template matching. Clearly though, far more work is needed to test this hypothesis further. This is not to say that many other abilities are not required for the full complexity of the cumulative cultural evolution we currently see in humans, but that the necessary and sufficient cognitive requirements for this process to start in a species might be simpler than the full suite of technical reasoning skills O&R suggest.

Financial support

None.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Boogert, N. J., Arbilly, M., Muth, F. & Seed, A. M. (2013) Do crows reason about causes or agents? The devil is in the controls. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110(4):E273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dymond, S., Haselgrove, M. & McGregor, A. (2013) Clever crows or unbalanced birds? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110(5):E336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, R., Schiestl, M., Boeckle, M., Frohnwieser, A., Miller, R., Gray, R. D., Clayton, N. S. & Taylor, A. H. (2019) New Caledonian crows use mental representations to solve metatool problems. Current Biology 29(4):686–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holzhaider, J. C., Hunt, G. R. & Gray, R. D. (2010b) The development of pandanus tool manufacture in wild New Caledonian crows. Behaviour 147:553–86.Google Scholar
Holzhaider, J. C., Sibley, M. D., Taylor, A. H., Singh, P. J., Gray, R. D. & Hunt, G. R. (2011) The social structure of New Caledonian crows. Animal Behaviour 81(1):8392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, G. R. (1996) Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature 379(6562):249–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, G. R. (2000) Human-like, population-level specialization in the manufacture of pandanus tools by New Caledonian crows Corvus moneduloides. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 267(1441):403–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, G. R. & Gray, R. D. (2003) Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 270(1517):867–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, G. R. & Gray, R. D. (2004) The crafting of hook tools by wild New Caledonian crows. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 271:S8890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, I. F., von Bayern, A., Martin-Ordas, G., Rat-Fischer, L. & Osvath, M. (2015) Corvids create novel causal interventions after all. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 282(1806):20142504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jelbert, S. A., Hosking, R. J., Taylor, A. H. & Gray, R. D. (2018) Mental template matching is a potential cultural transmission mechanism for New Caledonian crow tool manufacturing traditions. Scientific Reports 8:8956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jelbert, S. A., Miller, R., Schiestl, M., Boeckle, M., Cheke, L. G., Gray, R. D., … & Clayton, N. S. (2019) New Caledonian crows infer the weight of objects from observing their movements in a breeze. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 286(1894):20182332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jelbert, S. A., Singh, P. J., Gray, R. D. & Taylor, A. H. (2015a) New Caledonian crows rapidly solve a collaborative problem without cooperative cognition. PLoS ONE 10(8).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jelbert, S. A., Taylor, A. H. & Gray, R. D. (2015b) Reasoning by exclusion in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) cannot be explained by avoidance of empty containers. Journal of Comparative Psychology 129(3):283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenward, B., Weir, A. A. S., Rutz, C. & Kacelnik, A. (2005) Tool manufacture by naive juvenile crows. Nature 433(7022):121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, C. J., Breen, A. J., Taylor, A. H., Gray, R. D. & Hoppitt, W. J. S. E. (2016) How New Caledonian crows solve novel foraging problems and what it means for cumulative culture. Learning & Behavior 44(1):1828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neilands, P. D., Jelbert, S. A., Breen, A. J., Schiestl, M. & Taylor, A. H. (2016) How insightful is ‘insight’? New Caledonian crows do not attend to object weight during spontaneous stone dropping. PLoS ONE 11(12):e0167419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slater, P. J. B. (1983) Bird song learning: Theme and variations. Perspectives in Ornithology 12:475–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
St Clair, J. J. H., Klump, B. C., Sugasawa, S., Higgott, C. G., Colegrave, N. & Rutz, C. (2018) Hook innovation boosts foraging efficiency in tool-using crows. Nature Ecology and Evolution 2(3):441–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, A. H. (2014) Corvid cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 5(3):361–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Taylor, A. H., Cheke, L. G., Waismeyer, A., Meltzoff, A. N., Miller, R., Gopnik, A., Clayton, N. S. & Gray, R. D. (2014) Of babies and birds: Complex tool behaviours are not sufficient for the evolution of the ability to create a novel causal intervention. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281(1787):20140837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A. H., Cheke, L. G., Waismeyer, A., Meltzoff, A., Miller, R., Gopnik, A., … & Gray, R. D. (2015) No conclusive evidence that corvids can create novel causal interventions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 282(1813):20150796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R. & Gray, R. D. (2012a) Context-dependent tool use in New Caledonian crows. Biology Letters 8(2):205207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R., Holzhaider, J. C. & Gray, R. D. (2007) Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows. Current Biology 17:1504–507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R., Medina, F. S. & Gray, R. D. (2009) Do New Caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 276(1655):247–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, A. H., Miller, R. & Gray, R. D. (2012b) New Caledonian crows reason about hidden causal agents. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109(40):16389–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A. H., Miller, R., Gray, R. D. (2013a) Reply to Boogert et al.: The devil is unlikely to be in association or distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110(4):E274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A. H., Miller, R., Gray, R. D. (2013b) Reply to Dymond et al.: Clear evidence of habituation counters counterbalancing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110(5):E337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Bayern, A. M. P., Danel, S., Auersperg, A. M. I., Mioduszewska, B. & Kacelnik, A. (2018) Compound tool construction by New Caledonian crows. Scientific Reports 8(1):15676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimpenny, J. H., Weir, A. A. & Kacelnik, A. (2011) New Caledonian crows use tools for non-foraging activities. Animal Cognition 14(3):459–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yellen, J. E., Brooks, A. S., Cornelissen, E., Mehlman, M. J. & Stewart, K. (1995) A middle stone age worked bone industry from Katanda, Upper Semliki Valley, Zaire. Science (New York, N.Y.) 268(5210):553–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed