Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T20:50:41.788Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Diel rhythms in shallow Mediterranean rocky-reef fishes: a chronobiological approach with the help of trained volunteers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 September 2012

Ernesto Azzurro
Affiliation:
ISPRA, National Institute of Environmental Protection and Research, Sts Livorno, Piazzale dei Marmi 2, 57123, Livorno, Italy
Jacopo Aguzzi
Affiliation:
Instituto de Ciencias del Mar (ICM-CSIC); Paseo Maritímo de la Barceloneta, 37-49. 08003 Barcelona, Spain
Francesc Maynou
Affiliation:
Instituto de Ciencias del Mar (ICM-CSIC); Paseo Maritímo de la Barceloneta, 37-49. 08003 Barcelona, Spain
Juan José Chiesa
Affiliation:
Departamento de Ciencia y Tecnología, Universidad Nacional de Quilmes/CONICET, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Dario Savini*
Affiliation:
DiSTA—Dipartimento di Scienze della Terra e dell'Ambiente, Sezione Ambiente Via S. Epifanio 14, 27100 Pavia, Italy For-Mare, Via Lovati 33, 27100 Pavia, Italy
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: D. Savini, DiSTA—Dipartimento di Scienze della Terra e dell'Ambiente, Sezione Ambiente Via S. Epifanio 14, 27100 Pavia, Italy email: dario.savini@unipv.it
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Behavioural rhythms in marine species have been mostly investigated in laboratory organisms and their expression within the animals' natural environments remains largely unknown. Here, we studied diel (i.e. 24-hours-based) and intra-diel (i.e. 12-hours-based) rhythmic variations in the abundance of seven shallow rocky-reef fish species, namely Coris julis, Epinephelus marginatus, Sarpa salpa, Serranus cabrilla, Serranus scriba, Sparisoma cretense and Thalassoma pavo, along the rocky shores of Linosa Island (Mediterranean Sea). Data were visually collected by trained volunteers along fixed transects at 3-hourly intervals throughout six consecutive 24-hours periods. Density estimates can vary greatly between consecutive days and during 24-hours periods according not only to the major day–night changeover but also to minor intra-diel variations at the daylight hours. In the case of T. pavo, C. julis, S. cabrilla and S. salpa waveform analyses showed midday troughs in abundance within the 24-hours period but significant variation within the hours of daylight was highlighted only for T. pavo. Although results were not conclusive at the intra-dial level, the employment of volunteers represented a valuable tool for chronobiology, suitable to improve our understanding of fish behaviour in natural systems.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 2012

INTRODUCTION

All living organisms studied thus far exhibit rhythms at different levels of their biological organization provoked by the rotation of the Earth on its axis and the relative position of the sun and moon (i.e. geophysical cycles: Aschoff, Reference Aschoff and Aschoff1981). In oceans, these cycles take the form of day–night light intensity variations or currents speed variations, as the product of tidal pulls (reviewed by Aguzzi et al., Reference Aguzzi, Costa, Furushima, Chiesa, Company, Menesatti, Iwase and Fujiwara2010). Accordingly, marine animals vary their activity with a diel (i.e. 24-hours-based) periodicity. That rhythmic activity may result in massive displacements and migrations of organisms within substrata or between depth zones of the water column and the continental margin (Naylor, Reference Naylor2005). Rhythms can also manifest as increases and decreases in the rate of swimming (e.g. fishes: Reebs, Reference Reebs2002), locomotion (e.g. crabs and lobsters: Palmer, Reference Palmer2000; Chiesa et al., Reference Chiesa, Aguzzi, García, Sardà and De la Iglesia2010), or crawling (e.g. polychaetes: Last et al., Reference Last, Bailhache, Kramer, Kyriacou, Rosato and Olive2009).

Chronobiology is the field of biology that examines cyclic phenomena in living organisms (Reebs, Reference Reebs2002). To date, this science has been seldom applied in field studies of both land and marine species (Naylor, Reference Naylor2005). In fact, behavioural rhythms are largely analysed in laboratory organisms, so that their expression within the animals' natural environments remains largely unknown (Mrosovsky & Hattar, Reference Mrosovsky and Hattar2005). Conversely, studies on these rhythms in the field do not often comply with daily variability (Aguzzi & Bahamon, Reference Aguzzi and Bahamon2009) with some exception (e.g. Curley et al., Reference Curley, Kingsford and Gillanders2002; Gladstone, Reference Gladstone2007). Therefore, 24-hours-based investigations in wild populations can be of importance when estimating the potential effects of diel variability of fish communities.

Fishes are excellent models for activity rhythm studies and the rhythmicity of fish behaviour is well known under controlled conditions (as reviewed by Helfman, Reference Helfman and Pitcher1993; Kasai et al., Reference Kasai, Yamamoto and Kiyohara2009; Yammouni et al., Reference Yammouni, Bozzano and Douglas2011). However, relatively little information exists regarding fish biorhythms in the wild with the exception of gross day–night changes (Thompson & Mapstone, Reference Thompson and Mapstone1997; Willis et al., Reference Willis, Badalamenti and Milazzo2006; Azzurro et al., Reference Azzurro, Pais, Consoli and Andaloro2007). Among Mediterranean fish communities day–night changeover has been evidenced using beach seines, trawl surveys and hydroacoustic techniques (see Azzurro et al., Reference Azzurro, Pais, Consoli and Andaloro2007 for a review) with documented changes in feeding or habitat types occupation from day to night (Harmelin-Vivien, Reference Harmelin-Vivien1982; Bell & Harmelin-Vivien, Reference Bell and Harmelin-Vivien1983). Nevertheless, few studies have conducted repeated sampling at different times of the day (Spyker & van den Berghe, Reference Spyker and Van den Berghe1995; Letourneur et al., Reference Letourneur, Darnaude, Salen-Picard and Harmelin-Vivien2001; Willis et al., Reference Willis, Badalamenti and Milazzo2006) and scant information on 24-hours cycles is available. Although the understanding of these rhythms is the goal of chronobiology, poor cross-communication, if any, exists between this science and ecology (Marques & Waterhouse, Reference Marques and Waterhouse2004; Morgan, Reference Morgan2004). Indeed, laboratory-based chronobiology commonly deals with time-series gathered at minute and hourly frequency over several consecutive days (Aguzzi et al., Reference Aguzzi, Costa, Furushima, Chiesa, Company, Menesatti, Iwase and Fujiwara2010) whereas field behavioural studies are particularly difficult to perform at such frequency.

Visual census is the method used most often to analyse littoral fish communities (Harmelin-Vivien et al., Reference Harmelin-Vivien, Harmelin, Chauvet, Duval and Galzin1985). This technique has been occasionally used to study diel variation in Mediterranean species (Azzurro et al., Reference Azzurro, Pais, Consoli and Andaloro2007) but some obvious complexities have emerged that make it impossible to demonstrate the existence of uniform patterns (Willis et al., Reference Willis, Badalamenti and Milazzo2006). Large amounts of data and intensive sampling designs are required to overcome this constraint. In this context, we considered collaborating with scientists and volunteers as an efficient means to perform extensive temporally scheduled surveys (Greenwood, Reference Greenwood1994; Pattengill-Semmens & Semmens, Reference Pattengill-Semmens and Semmens2003; Bonney et al., Reference Bonney, Cooper, Dickinson, Kelling, Phillips, Kenneth, Rosenberg and Shirk2009). This synergy, which is termed ‘Citizen Science’, has recently emerged as a new environmental monitoring technique (Silvertown, Reference Silvertown2009). Here we carried out a novel and interdisciplinary effort using a team of trained volunteers who collected data at multiple sampling cycles with intra-diel frequency. Our aim was to test for the existence of rhythmic patterns in the abundances of rocky fish species and to describe this variation. Specifically, we wanted to: (1) test for the presence of significant variability at the diel (i.e. 24-hours based) and intra-diel (i.e. 12-hours based) level; (2) gain an overview on the general character and robustness of the species rhythms; and (3) to statistically assess their phasing.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study area and the studied species

The study was performed along the rocky shores of Linosa (35°85′N 12°85′E), a small volcanic island located in the middle of Sicily Strait (Mediterranean Sea), 165 km from the African coast and 167 km off the coast of Sicily (Italy). Linosa was declared a Marine Protected Area in 2002. Two different locations at Linosa Island (Figure 1) were surveyed between 27 June and 2 July 2010. At that time, sunset and sunrise were at 5:47 and at 20:30 h, respectively.

Fig. 1. Study locations (A, B) at Linosa Island (35°85′N 12°85′E) within the Central Mediterranean (Sicily Strait).

We selected 7 target species on the basis of: (1) their ecological relevance; (2) their abundance in the study area; and (3) the feasibility of their visual identification (during both day and night). The species were: the dusky grouper Epinephelus marginatus (Linnaeus, 1758); the ornate wrasse, Thalassoma pavo (Linnaeus, 1758); the parrotfish, Sparisoma cretense (Linnaeus, 1758); the rainbow-wrasse, Coris julis (Linnaeus, 1758); the painted comber, Serranus scriba (Linnaeus, 1758); the comber, Serranus cabrilla (Linnaeus, 1785); and the salema, Sarpa salpa (Linnaeus, 1758). Epinephelus marginatus, T. pavo and S. cretense have recently widened their distribution, a fact that may be linked to their thermophilic habit (Azzurro et al., Reference Azzurro, Moschella and Maynou2011). Coris julis, S. scriba, S. cabrilla and S. salpa are common and widespread throughout the Mediterranean, although S. salpa is now becoming rare in many areas of the Levant Basin (Bariche et al., Reference Bariche, Letourneur and Harmelin-Vivien2004).

Trained volunteers and sampling procedures

Thirty zoology graduates (Bachelors in Biology and Natural Sciences of the University of Pavia, Italy) divided into two groups of 15, volunteered to collect the field data. Operators were intensively trained in fish recognition for two days before sampling. Sessions included species identification, examinations and visual trials based on established practices of Citizen Science (e.g. Pattengill-Semmens & Semmens, Reference Pattengill-Semmens and Semmens2003). The efficiency of the volunteers in species identification and counting was corroborated by an experienced scientist, co-authoring the present paper, who carried out preliminary trials, in tandem with each volunteer. After the surveys, the collected data were verified to test for possible outliers.

In each sampling area, 6 randomly chosen transects approximately parallel to the coast were permanently marked with a coloured tape 50 m in length, fixed on the bottom at 3 m depth. Fish censuses were performed by swimming along the centre line of each transect and counting all individuals for each targeted species within 2.5 m on either side (i.e. approximately 250 m2 of total area) for a maximum time of 5 minutes. Transects were surveyed every three hours by observers who were randomly chosen using the random number generator application of Minitab 12.0 Student Edition Software.

In each location, sampling was performed over 8 temporal windows of 30 minutes each centred around the following times: 5:30, 8:30, 11:30, 14:30, 17:30, 19:30, 22:30 and 2:30 h. Nocturnal observations were carried out using a 50 W torch, following the method of Azzurro et al. (Reference Azzurro, Pais, Consoli and Andaloro2007). Temporal censuses were repeated over three consecutive days at each location. Overall, 12 permanent transects were surveyed corresponding to a seafloor area of approximately 3000 m2. The total number of replicated trials was 288, corresponding to the sum of 72,000 m2.

Statistical analyses

The data were square root transformed overall and an adjusted Bray–Curtis measure of similarity plus an added dummy variable (=1) was used to calculate the resemblance matrix as is appropriate for datasets containing many zeros. A non-metric multidimensional scaling (nMDS) (Clarke, Reference Clarke1993) based on the Bray–Curtis similarity matrix was used to visualize the ordination of samples within a three-dimensional space.

To test for the occurrence of temporal variation in the entire set of species at the scale of days and hours we used a two-way permutational multivariate analysis of variance (PERMANOVA: Anderson, Reference Anderson2001) considering the 3-hour time periods (i.e. the term ‘Time’) as fixed with 8 levels. The 24-hours cycle (i.e. the term ‘Day’) was considered as random with 3 levels. As the participation of single volunteers was randomized with respect to the terms ‘Time’ and ‘Day’, we assumed that data variability regarding the subjectivity of species recognition had no effect on between-treatment comparisons.

To gain an overview on the general character and robustness of the species rhythms, visual observations for all selected species were represented over time. Data sets were plotted as the average number of observations of all transects for each corresponding 3-hours period, standardized over the total transect surface (250 m2). In the graphing, the timing of sunset and sunrise were also considered.

Population behavioural rhythms are intrinsically noisy, due to the variable synchronic activity of all constituting individuals (e.g. Jadot et al., Reference Jadot, Ovidio and Voss2002; Aguzzi et al., Reference Aguzzi, Costa, Furushima, Chiesa, Company, Menesatti, Iwase and Fujiwara2010). To statistically quantify the consistency (i.e. repeatability) of any putative diel fluctuation, we used periodogram analysis. Significant periodicities were detected using the Lomb–Scargle periodogram, based on the least-square fitting of sine waves to the data in the time-range from 0 to 30-hours. This method provided consistent period estimation for time-series of short duration (Schimmel, Reference Schimmel2001), such as those typically obtained in the field. In periodograms, the highest significant (P = 0.05) peak represented the maximum percentage of total data variance fitted by the corresponding periodicity. The peak value was chosen for period attribution of the analysed time-series.

To statistically assess the phasing of biorhythms in relation to day–night alternation we used waveform analysis, a methodology currently used in laboratory chronobiology (Fernández et al., Reference Fernández, Hermida and Mojón2009). Time-series were subdivided into sub-sets of 24-hours duration (i.e. 8 time-interval values in each). The data for all subsets were averaged based on corresponding time periods. Averages and their standard deviations were represented as a consensus plot over 24-hours (i.e. the waveform). A daily mean was estimated by re-averaging all waveform values. The resulting estimate was represented on the waveform plot as a threshold line. All waveform values above this line indicated a significant increment in population activity rhythm. The threshold line also indicated the temporal limits of peaks allowing the activity timings of different species to be compared with each other (Aguzzi et al., Reference Aguzzi, Bahamon and Marotta2009). This procedure was carried out similarly to the Mean Estimate Statistic Of Rhythm procedure (MESOR: Aguzzi et al., Reference Aguzzi, Chiesa, Caprioli, Cascione, Magnifico, Rimatori and Costa2006). MESOR is the value midway between the highest and the lowest values of a cosine function best fitting rhythmic time-series.

The percentage of activity occurring during daylight in relation to the total activity that each animal carried out throughout the 24-hours period was calculated based on individual waveforms and used to assess either the diurnal or nocturnal activity distribution. Behavioural patterns were identified as diurnal when photophase activity was greater than 60% and as nocturnal when less than 40% (Chiesa et al., Reference Chiesa, Aguzzi, García, Sardà and De la Iglesia2010).

To explore the occurrence of intra-diel activity patterns we used Fourier analysis because it can detect the 12-hours peak patterns in rhythms that show certain variability over consecutive days (Díez-Noguera, Reference Díez-Noguera, Madrid-Pérez and Rol de Lama2006). Time-series were analysed with that technique by setting both a fundamental harmonic with a period of 24-hours to study the diel variation and setting its submultiple at 12-hours to study the intra-diel variation. The minimum square fitting of these cosine functions onto consecutive 24-hours data sections of species time-series was estimated. For regularly sampled series, the quadratic power of the amplitude of each cosenoidal function (i.e. the harmonic) obtained from the Fourier decomposition is defined as the harmonic power content (PC). This value can be expressed as the percentage of the total variance in the time-series explained by the least-squares fitting of each harmonic (Díez-Noguera, Reference Díez-Noguera, Madrid-Pérez and Rol de Lama2006). The PC was obtained as the percentage of variance of the time series segment explained by these harmonics (PC24 and PC12). A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was designed to study differences in PC values between days and species as factors after having verified the assumptions of this test. Two-way ANOVA was also designed to study the PC harmonics and the species as factors. Multivariate analyses were performed with the PRIMER 6 + PERMANOVA software package from Plymouth Marine Laboratory, UK. The software package STATISTICA 8.0, from StatSoft, Inc, was used for univariate analyses.

RESULTS

The mean number of individuals per species at each transect of the two sampling areas is listed in Figure 2. The most abundant species were, in order of abundance, T. pavo, C. julis, S. cabrilla, S. scriba, S. salpa, S. cretense and E. marginatus.

Fig. 2. Square root-transformed mean abundances (number of individuals in 250 m2) of target fish species registered at locations A and B (see Figure 1).

Multivariate analyses

The day–night changeover was well represented by the nMDS time periods (Figure 3) which showed a clear-cut separation among diurnal and nocturnal surveys and PERMANOVA outputs (Table 1) showed significant differences for the factor ‘Time’ in both locations A and B. On the other side, 12-hours based patterns were not apparent in the multivariate ordination with no clear separation between within-night nor within-day trials. Significant between-days differences were observed only at location A, and a significant interaction between ‘Time’ and ‘Day’ was highlighted at both sites.

Fig. 3. Non-metric multi-dimensional scaling three-dimensional ordination of the site's centroids comparing the species recorded during the 8 periods. The replicate symbols indicate the 3 days for each time period.

Table 1. Permutational multivariate analysis of variance based on the Bray–Curtis dissimilarity measure for square root-transformed abundance data in locations A and B. The test was performed using 9999 permutations under the reduced model. Values for different levels of significance are reported as follows: *, P < 0.05; **, P ≤ 0.01; ***, P ≤ 0.001.

Chronobiological analyses

As expected, all the targeted fish species showed a sharp day–night fluctuation in their counts (Figure 4). Higher abundances were apparent during daylight hours, with minima commonly recorded during the central darkness hours. Lomb–Scargle periodogram analysis detected significant diel periods (i.e. close to 24-hours) in three of the studied species. Coris julis presented a peak corresponding to a period of 24.9 hours (i.e. 24-hours, 54 minutes). Serranus cabrilla exhibited a peak with a period of 23.30 hours (i.e. 23 hours, 27 minutes). Thalassoma pavo showed the highest peak of all species, with a period of 23.60 hours (i.e. 23 hours, 36 minutes). No significant peak was found in the periodogram of the remaining species given the low levels in visual counts (see Figure 4). Waveform analysis reported the phase onset and phase offset of significant (i.e. above the daily mean) increases in visual count for all targeted species (Figure 5). These coincided with the sampling time intervals that included sunset and sunrise.

Fig. 4. Time-series of visual observations (standardized for transect total area) for all selected species for the two study locations (A and B; see Figure 1). Vertical dashed lines delimit visual counts for the two locations. Grey vertical rectangles represent night.

Fig. 5. Waveform analysis findings for the time-series of visual observations of the selected species. The horizontal line is the daily mean, and values above that line represent significant increases in visual counts (i.e. the phase). Upward and downward arrows indicate the first and the last activity value above the daily mean, representing the timings of the waveform peak onset and offset, respectively.

A weaker bimodal phase with a drop in correspondence with central hours of the day was apparent, although not statistically significant, in waveform profiles of C. julis, S. cabrilla, S. scriba, and finally T. pavo for which PERMANOVA showed instead significant variation within the daylight for the term ‘Time period’ (Pseudo F = 2.223, P perm < 0.05) and a significant interaction between the terms ‘Day’ and ‘Time' period (Pseudo F = 2.5047, P perm < 0.01). The term ‘Day’ was not significant for this species (Pseudo F = 1.0985, P perm <0.05).

Waveforms featured high activity percentages during daylight hours in all species: C. julis, 78%; E. marginatus, 75%; S. cabrilla, 80%; S. cretense, 82%; S. salpa, 74%; S. scriba, 73%; and T. pavo, 82%.

Fourier analysis provided the percentage of variance explained by both 24-hours and 12-hours harmonics (i.e. the PC) in time-series sections of one-day duration (Figure 6). ANOVA indicated the occurrence of similar values of PC24 and PC12 for all days in all species. In contrast, comparisons among different species presented significant differences (PC24-hours: P < 0.05 for the repeated measures, P < 0.0001 for the species; PC12: P < 0.05 for the repeated measures, P < 0.001). Significant differences were found considering the harmonics and the species as factors in a two-way ANOVA (for the harmonics: P < 0.0001; for the species: P < 0.0001).

Fig. 6. Fourier analysis findings as the mean (± SEM) power content (PC) obtained for all species by the fitting of two harmonics of submultiple periods (i.e. 24-hours, PC24 and 12-hours, PC12) on visual count time-series segments of 24-hours duration.

DISCUSSION

Diel variability

Multivariate analysis showed an expected sharp day–night changeover in the group of chosen species. Waveform analysis was used to evaluate both the phase and the amplitude stability of the 24-hours activity pattern in our selected species based on the variability of the averaged visual count bins in relation to the daily mean (i.e. as significant increments or decrements).

First, assuming that our result is more accurate for those species showing higher abundances, a diurnal temporal niche was observed with high activity percentages (i.e. greater than 70%) during daytime in all species. Waveform profiles depicted sharp 24-hours cycles for C. julis, S. cabrilla and T. pavo, the 24-hours rhythms of which were statistically proven by periodogram analysis. This was also confirmed by the Fourier analysis, where the 24-hours harmonic component showed better time-series fits for these species. In some cases, these results confirm previously published behavioural observations. For example, C. julis leave just before dusk and return to the foraging area around dawn; in aquaria, it buries itself within the sand at night (Videler, Reference Videler, Koella, Obál and Schulz1986). Similarly, S. scriba, seems to decrease in activity at night, when many individuals were observed remaining in hiding under Posidonia oceanica leaves (March et al., Reference March, Palmer, Alós, Grau and Cardona2010). A decrease in the nocturnal abundance of T. pavo, C. julis, S. scriba, S. cretense and many other rocky-reef fishes was observed by Azzurro et al. (Reference Azzurro, Pais, Consoli and Andaloro2007), but without any further observations regarding their rhythmic activity.

Periodogram analysis of S. scriba and S. cretense failed to show any significant diel rhythm. Regardless, a diurnal niche is also evident based on waveform analysis for these species. The non-significant periodogram outputs are reasonably explained by the interaction between the terms ‘Day’ and ‘Time’ in our design (see Table 1), whereas for E. marginatus, lower count values are responsible for the non-significant periodogram results (Schimmel, Reference Schimmel2001).

Diel fluctuations in the counted fishes reported in this study can be explained in terms of changes in the behaviour of the constitutive individuals, which depend on the day–night alternation. Even though the observed nocturnal drop in counts is mostly related to the inactivity and sheltering of the studied species in response to darkness (Azzurro et al., Reference Azzurro, Pais, Consoli and Andaloro2007), these variations could also occur in relation to habitat use. This includes bathymetric shifts (Spyker & van den Berghe, Reference Spyker and Van den Berghe1995; Colmenero et al., Reference Colmenero, Aguzzi, Lombarte and Bozzano2010), which may depend on changes in feeding behaviour (Piet & Guruge, Reference Piet and Guruge1997; Letourneur et al., Reference Letourneur, Darnaude, Salen-Picard and Harmelin-Vivien2001; Carpentieri et al., Reference Carpentieri, Colloca, Belluscio, Criscoli and Ardizzone2006) and predator avoidance (Copp & Jurajda, Reference Copp and Jurajda1993; Arrington & Winemiller, Reference Arrington and Winemiller2003).

Intra-diel variability

While both, multivariate and univariate analyses were highly effective in detecting day–night variation, our study provided only a weak evidence of the existence of an intra-diel pattern in the counts of rocky-reef fishes. This finding would confirm a general and well-known difficulty in extrapolating intra-diel variability in fish assemblages globally (reviewed by Willis et al., Reference Willis, Badalamenti and Milazzo2006). Certainly, the elevated between-transect variability of visual counts was the basis of this constraint. Even if the term ‘Time’ was highly replicated in our design and the sample unit area was doubled with respect to standard strip-transects (Harmelin-Vivien et al., Reference Harmelin-Vivien, Harmelin, Chauvet, Duval and Galzin1985), the statistical power of our tests was not sufficient to detect multivariate signals at the intra-diel level. Other unexplored sources of variation, including individual changes in mobility (Jadot et al., Reference Jadot, Ovidio and Voss2002), could have masked the existence of a main effect at the intra-day level. Nevertheless some weak but apparent signals were detected in three of the study species. In fact, waveform analysis showed midday troughs in C. julis, S. scriba and T. pavo with a significant within-day variation for T. pavo. As a matter of fact, midday troughs in abundances have been highlighted for many marine organisms because of parallel decreases in their behavioural activity (as reviewed by Aguzzi et al., Reference Aguzzi, Costa, Furushima, Chiesa, Company, Menesatti, Iwase and Fujiwara2010). These decreases in animal activity at central photophase hours could be associated with crepuscular peaks in activity rhythms, as documented for some marine fishes (Jadot et al., Reference Jadot, Ovidio and Voss2002 and references therein). Sarpa salpa represents another example of within-day variability. Waveform analysis showed minimal abundance during the first diurnal time interval (Figure 4) possibly due to a late beginning of activity (Jadot et al., Reference Jadot, Donnay, Acolas, Cornet and Bégout Anras2006) or to regular migrations within the home range (Jadot et al., Reference Jadot, Ovidio and Voss2002). For the remaining species, no intra-diel pattern could be established.

Certainly, this study should be replicated on a wider spatial scale to verify the coherence and stability of observed patterns and eventually to investigate the causative factors. Fourier analysis indicated an elevated value of PC24 that was not significantly different from the PC12 in all species (see Figure 6). In some cases, both values were similar, suggesting consistency of the bimodal fluctuation within the overall 24-hours fluctuation by peak splitting. The results of the 12-hours harmonic fitting suggest the occurrence of crepuscular peaks in the time-series for C. julis, which featured the highest PC12 value and a clear 12-hours peak related to sunrise in the waveform.

Intra-diel fluctuations in visual counts could be the product of differential swimming activity at certain times of the day, which rendered individuals more or less visible in key moments of the photophase (Hobson, Reference Hobson1965; Ebeling & Bray, Reference Ebeling and Bray1976; Colton & Alevizon, Reference Colton and Alevizon1981). Moreover, the disturbance created by the observer cannot be completely disregarded. In fact, the behavioural response of fish to the diver is expected to vary at different times of the day, and this might contribute to count variability (Thompson & Mapstone, Reference Thompson and Mapstone1997).

The contribution of trained volunteers

The engagement of trained volunteers was crucial to performing a high ‘Time’ level replication within treatments and to collect substantial amounts of data in a short period of time. The manpower provided by this group of observers served to satisfy chronobiological sampling requirements. Although the use of volunteers is not novel in biodiversity (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Foster-Smith and Welch2001) and ecological (Foster-Smith & Evans, Reference Foster-Smith and Evans2003) studies their employment can be considered original for chronobiology. It allowed us to approach fish in their natural environment.

A common concern regarding Citizen Science is the quality of data. As a matter of fact, Citizen Scientists may vary in their skill, compared to professional observers. This source of bias can be reduced much after personalized training and with the adoption of simple methods, adequate sampling efforts, improving identification of species and confortable habitats to sample (Dickinson et al., Reference Dickinson, Zuckerberg and Bonter2010). On the other hand, a limited expertise of contributors and complicated tasks should be discouraged, since it can lead to the collection of poor quality or even misleading data (Fitzpatrick et al., Reference Fitzpatrick, Preisser, Ellison and Elkinton2009). When simple and standardized protocols are used, trained volunteers can provide data of comparable quality to professionals (Gillett et al., Reference Gillett, Pondella II, Freiwald, Schiff, Caselle, Shuman and Weisberg2012 and references therein). In this respect, marine fish have received considerable attention by Citizen Science through the use of volunteers to collect data regarding species occurrence and distribution (reviewed by Stallings, Reference Stallings2009; Ward-Paige et al., Reference Ward-Paige, Mora, Lotze, Pattengill-Semmens, McClenachan and Arias-Castro2010, Reference Ward-Paige, Pattengill-Semmens, Myers and Lotze2011). We engaged volunteers in a specific training and conceived a strict and easy protocol that guided them in their visual counts. Data were pooled from multiple observers with broadly similar levels of experience and this usually helps to guarantee data quality (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Walsh, Tissot and Hallacher2006). Certainly, individual differences in the efficacy of observation (e.g. Lincoln-Smith, Reference Lincoln-Smith1988) might still have contributed to augment the variability in counts, but the random selection of volunteers with respect to the design factors eliminated the possibility of bias related to this factor.

The characterization of 24-hours patterns in visual count data for wild rocky-reef fishes is a challenging research target. In this paper, we provide evidence that density estimates can vary greatly during 24-hours periods according not only to the major day–night changeover but also to minor intra-diel variations observed during daylight hours. Chronobiological analyses, although far from conclusive, helped us to illustrate bimodal cycles within the 24-hours period, at least for the most abundant species such as C. julis, S. cabrilla and T. pavo. These regular fluctuations are probably species specific and possibly related to different activity rhythms or to a different use of space with an effect on their abundance and visibility to divers. This must be taken into account for performing reliable fish visual census estimates. Nevertheless we showed that intra-diel temporal factors can be particularly weak with respect to other sources of variability of these species on a local scale, such as to the between-day variability. Together, these results show promise with regard to identifying significant intra-diel patterns in wild littoral fishes. The presence of such regulation deserves to be further investigated in field studies possibly for longer durations of time (>6 days). The present study shows that sophisticated laboratory chronobiology paradigms can be efficiently used in the context of field studies with natural populations. Hopefully, this methodological transfer from laboratory analysis to field sampling will help us to clarify diel patterns in littoral fishes, which remains one of the most neglected sources of variability in the assessment of natural populations. The future use of trained volunteers in chronobiology studies could contribute to improve our understanding of fish behaviour.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We warmly acknowledge the University of Pavia, Dr Chiara Lombardi and the team of thirty volunteers who participated in the research. We also express our gratitude to: Dr P. Moschella (CIESM) for collaborating in the formulation of didactic protocols and Dr M.J. Anderson (Institute of Information and Mathematical Sciences, Massey University, Auckland, New Zealand) for her suggestion on multivariate analysis. The present work was developed within the framework of the project CIESM Tropical Signals founded by Fondation Albert II of Monaco and the RITFIM project (CTM2010-16274) funded by the Spanish Ministry for Science and Innovation. No sampling permit or ethics clearance was needed for our research.

References

REFERENCES

Aguzzi, J. and Bahamon, N. (2009) Modelled day–night biases in decapod assessment by bottom trawling survey. Fisheries Research 100, 274280.Google Scholar
Aguzzi, J., Bahamon, N. and Marotta, L. (2009) Modulation of activity rhythms in continental margin decapods by light availability and predatory behaviour of Nephrops norvegicus . Marine Ecology 30, 366375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aguzzi, J., Chiesa, J.J., Caprioli, R., Cascione, D., Magnifico, G., Rimatori, V. and Costa, C. (2006) Preliminary evidences of circadian fan activity rhythm in Sabella spallanzanii (Gmelin, 1791) (Polychaeta: Sabellidae). Scientia Marina 70, 727–724.Google Scholar
Aguzzi, J., Costa, C., Furushima, Y., Chiesa, J.J., Company, J.B., Menesatti, P., Iwase, R. and Fujiwara, Y. (2010) Behavioural rhythms of hydrocarbon seep fauna in relation to internal tides. Marine Ecology Progress Series 418, 4756.Google Scholar
Anderson, M.J. (2001) A new method for non-parametric multivariate analysis of variance. Australian Ecology 26, 3236.Google Scholar
Arrington, D.A. and Winemiller, K.O. (2003) Diel changeover in sandbank fish assemblages in a neotropical floodplain river. Journal of Fish Biology 63, 442459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aschoff, J. (1981) Freerunning and entrained circadian rhythms. In Aschoff, J. (ed.) Handbook of behavioral neurobiology. Volume 4. Biological rhythms. New York: Plenum, pp. 8193.Google Scholar
Azzurro, E., Moschella, P. and Maynou, F. (2011) Tracking signals of change in Mediterranean fish diversity based on local ecological knowledge. Plos ONE 6, e24885. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Azzurro, E., Pais, A., Consoli, P. and Andaloro, F. (2007) Evaluating day–night changes in shallow Mediterranean rocky reef fish assemblages by visual census. Marine Biology 151, 22452253.Google Scholar
Bariche, M., Letourneur, Y. and Harmelin-Vivien, M. (2004) Temporal fluctuations and settlement patterns of native and Lessepsian herbivorous fishes on the Lebanese coast (eastern Mediterranean). Environmental Biology of Fishes 70, 8190.Google Scholar
Bell, J.D. and Harmelin-Vivien, M.L. (1983) Fish fauna of French Mediterranean Posidonia oceanica seagrass meadows. 2. Feeding habits. Tethys 11, 114.Google Scholar
Bonney, R., Cooper, C.B., Dickinson, J., Kelling, S., Phillips, T., Kenneth, V., Rosenberg, V. and Shirk, J. (2009) Citizen Science: a developing tool for expanding science knowledge and scientific literacy. BioScience 59, 977984.Google Scholar
Carpentieri, P., Colloca, F., Belluscio, A., Criscoli, A. and Ardizzone, G.D. (2006) Diel feeding periodicity and daily ration of shelf break fish species. Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 86, 853860.Google Scholar
Chiesa, J.J., Aguzzi, J., García, J.A., Sardà, F. and De la Iglesia, H. (2010) Light intensity determines temporal niche switching of behavioral activity in deep water Nephrops norvegicus (Crustacea: Decapoda). Journal of Biological Rhythms 25, 277287.Google Scholar
Clarke, K.R. (1993) Non-parametric multivariate analyses of changes in community structure. Austin Journal of Ecology 18, 117143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colmenero, A., Aguzzi, J., Lombarte, A. and Bozzano, A. (2010) Sensory constraints in temporal segregation in two species of anglerfish (Lophius budegassa and L. piscatorius). Marine Ecology Progress Series 416, 255265.Google Scholar
Colton, D.E. and Alevizon, W.S. (1981) Diurnal variability in a fish assemblage of Bahamian coral reef. Environmental Biology of Fishes 6, 341345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Copp, G.H. and Jurajda, P. (1993) Do small riverine fish move inshore at night? Journal of Fish Biology 43, 229241.Google Scholar
Curley, B., Kingsford, M.J. and Gillanders, B.M. (2002) Spatial and habitat-related patterns of temperate reef fish assemblages: implications for the design of Marine Protected Areas. Marine and Freshwater Research 53, 11971210.Google Scholar
Dickinson, J.L., Zuckerberg, B. and Bonter, D.N. (2010) Citizen Science as an ecological research tool: challenges and benefits. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 41, 149172.Google Scholar
Díez-Noguera, A. (2006) Representación gráfica y análisis de datos en cronobiología. In Madrid-Pérez, J.A. and Rol de Lama, M.A. (eds) Cronobiología básica y clínica. Madrid: Editec@Red.Google Scholar
Ebeling, A.W. and Bray, R.N. (1976) Day versus night activity of reef fishes in a kelp forest off Santa Barbara, California. Fishery Bulletin 74, 703717.Google Scholar
Evans, S.M., Foster-Smith, J. and Welch, R. (2001) Volunteers assess marine biodiversity. Biologist 48, 168172.Google ScholarPubMed
Fernández, J.R., Hermida, R.C. and Mojón, A. (2009) Chronobiological analysis techniques. Application to blood pressure. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 367, 431445.Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, M., Preisser, E., Ellison, A. and Elkinton, J. (2009) Observer bias and the detection of low-density populations. Ecological Applications 19, 16731679.Google Scholar
Foster-Smith, J. and Evans, S.M. (2003) The value of marine ecological data collected by volunteers. Biological Conservation 2, 199213.Google Scholar
Gillett, D.J., Pondella II, D.J., Freiwald, J., Schiff, K.C., Caselle, J.E., Shuman, C. and Weisberg, S.B. (2012) Comparing volunteer and professionally collected monitoring data from the rocky subtidal reefs of Southern California, USA. Environmental Monitoring and Assessment (2012) 184, 32393257.Google Scholar
Gladstone, W. (2007) Requirements for marine protected areas to conserve the biodiversity of rocky reef fishes. Aquatic Conservation: Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems 17, 7187.Google Scholar
Greenwood, J.J.D. (1994) Trust the wildlife volunteers. Nature 368, 490.Google Scholar
Harmelin-Vivien, M.L. (1982) Ichtyofaune des herbiers de posidonies du Parc National de Port-Cros: I. composition et variations spatio-temporelles. Travaux Scientifiques du Parc National de Port-Cros 8, 6992.Google Scholar
Harmelin-Vivien, M.L., Harmelin, J.G., Chauvet, C., Duval, C. and Galzin, R. (1985) Evaluation visuelle des peuplements et populations de poissons: méthodes et problèmes. Revue d'Ecologie (la Terre et la Vie) 40, 467539.Google Scholar
Helfman, G.S. (1993) Fish behaviour by day, night and twilight. In Pitcher, T.J. (ed.) Behaviour of teleost fishes. London: Chapman & Hall, pp. 479512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, E.S. (1965) Diurnal–nocturnal activity of some inshore fishes in the Gulf of California. Copeia 3, 291302.Google Scholar
Jadot, C., Ovidio, M. and Voss, J. (2002) Diel activity of Sarpa salpa (Sparidae) by ultrasonic telemetry in a Posidonia oceanica meadow of Corsica (Mediterranean Sea). Aquatic Living Resources 15, 343350.Google Scholar
Jadot, C., Donnay, A., Acolas, M.L., Cornet, Y. and Bégout Anras, M.L. (2006) Activity patterns, home-range size, and habitat utilization of Sarpa salpa (Teleostei: Sparidae) in the Mediterranean Sea. ICES Journal of Marine Science 63, 128139.Google Scholar
Kasai, M., Yamamoto, T. and Kiyohara, S. (2009) Circadian locomotor activity in Japanese sea catfish Plotosus lineatus . Fisheries Science 75, 8189.Google Scholar
Last, K.S., Bailhache, T., Kramer, C., Kyriacou, C.P., Rosato, E. and Olive, P.J. (2009) Tidal, daily, and lunar–day activity cycles in the marine polychaete Nereis virens . Chronobiology International 26, 167183.Google Scholar
Letourneur, Y., Darnaude, A., Salen-Picard, C. and Harmelin-Vivien, M. (2001) Spatial and temporal variations of fish assemblages in a shallow Mediterranean soft-bottom area (Gulf of Fos, France). Oceanologica Acta 24, 273285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lincoln-Smith, M.P. (1988) Effects of observer swimming speed on sample counts of temperate rocky reef fish assemblages. Marine Ecology Progress Series 43, 223231.Google Scholar
March, D., Palmer, M., Alós, J., Grau, A. and Cardona, F. (2010) Short-term residence, home range size and diel patterns of the painted comber Serranus scriba . Marine Ecology Progress Series 400, 195206.Google Scholar
Marques, M.D. and Waterhouse, J.M. (2004) Rhythms and the ecology—do chronobiologists still remember nature? Biological Rhythms Research 35, 12.Google Scholar
Morgan, E. (2004) Ecological significance of biological clocks. Biological Rhythms Research 35, 312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mrosovsky, N. and Hattar, S. (2005) Diurnal mice (Mus musculus) and other examples of temporal niche switching. Journal of Comparative Physiology 191, 10111024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naylor, E. (2005) Chronobiology: implications for marine resources exploitation and management. Scientia Marina 69, 157167.Google Scholar
Palmer, J.D. (2000) The clocks controlling the tide-associated rhythms of intertidal animals. BioEssays 22, 3237.Google Scholar
Pattengill-Semmens, C.V. and Semmens, B.X. (2003) Conservation and management applications of the reef volunteer fish monitoring program. Environmental Monitoring and Assessment 81, 4350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piet, G.J. and Guruge, W.A.H.P. (1997) Diel variation in feeding and vertical distribution of ten co-occurring fish species: consequences for resource partitioning. Environmental Biology of Fishes 50, 293307.Google Scholar
Reebs, S.G. (2002) Plasticity of diel and circadian activity rhythms in fishes. Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries 12, 349371.Google Scholar
Schimmel, M. (2001) Emphasizing difficulties in the detection of rhythms with Lomb–Scargle periodograms. Biological Rhythms Research 32, 341345.Google Scholar
Silvertown, J. (2009) A new dawn for citizen science. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 24, 467471.Google Scholar
Spyker, K.A. and Van den Berghe, E.P. (1995) Diurnal abundance patterns of Mediterranean fishes assessed on fixed transects by scuba divers. Transactions of the American Fishery Society 124, 216224.Google Scholar
Stallings, C. (2009) Fishery-independent data reveal negative effect of human population density on Caribbean predatory fish communities. PLoSOne 4, e5333.Google Scholar
Thompson, A.A. and Mapstone, B.D. (1997) Observer effects and training in underwater visual surveys of reef fishes. Marine Ecology Progress Series 154, 5363.Google Scholar
Videler, J.J. (1986) Sleep under sand cover of the labrid fish Coris julis . In Koella, W.P., Obál, F. and Schulz, H. (eds) Phylogeny of sleep. Stuttgart: Fischer Verlag, pp. 145147.Google Scholar
Ward-Paige, C.A., Mora, C., Lotze, H.K., Pattengill-Semmens, C.V., McClenachan, L. and Arias-Castro, E. (2010) Large-scale absence of sharks on reefs in the greater-Caribbean: a footprint of human pressures. PLoSOne 5, e11968.Google Scholar
Ward-Paige, C.P., Pattengill-Semmens, C., Myers, R.A. and Lotze, H.K. (2011) Spatial and temporal trends in yellow stingray abundance: evidence from diver surveys. Environmental Biology of Fishes 90, 263276.Google Scholar
Williams, I.D., Walsh, W.J., Tissot, B.N. and Hallacher, L.E. (2006) Impact of observers' experience level on counts of fishes in underwater visual surveys. Marine Ecology Progress Series 310, 185191.Google Scholar
Willis, T.J., Badalamenti, F. and Milazzo, M. (2006) Diel variability in counts of reef fishes and its implications for monitoring. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 331, 108120.Google Scholar
Yammouni, R., Bozzano, A. and Douglas, R.H. (2011) A latitudinal cline in the efficacy of endogenous signals: evidence derived from retinal cone contraction in fish. Journal of Experimental Biology 214, 501508.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Study locations (A, B) at Linosa Island (35°85′N 12°85′E) within the Central Mediterranean (Sicily Strait).

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Square root-transformed mean abundances (number of individuals in 250 m2) of target fish species registered at locations A and B (see Figure 1).

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Non-metric multi-dimensional scaling three-dimensional ordination of the site's centroids comparing the species recorded during the 8 periods. The replicate symbols indicate the 3 days for each time period.

Figure 3

Table 1. Permutational multivariate analysis of variance based on the Bray–Curtis dissimilarity measure for square root-transformed abundance data in locations A and B. The test was performed using 9999 permutations under the reduced model. Values for different levels of significance are reported as follows: *, P < 0.05; **, P ≤ 0.01; ***, P ≤ 0.001.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Time-series of visual observations (standardized for transect total area) for all selected species for the two study locations (A and B; see Figure 1). Vertical dashed lines delimit visual counts for the two locations. Grey vertical rectangles represent night.

Figure 5

Fig. 5. Waveform analysis findings for the time-series of visual observations of the selected species. The horizontal line is the daily mean, and values above that line represent significant increases in visual counts (i.e. the phase). Upward and downward arrows indicate the first and the last activity value above the daily mean, representing the timings of the waveform peak onset and offset, respectively.

Figure 6

Fig. 6. Fourier analysis findings as the mean (± SEM) power content (PC) obtained for all species by the fitting of two harmonics of submultiple periods (i.e. 24-hours, PC24 and 12-hours, PC12) on visual count time-series segments of 24-hours duration.