Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T00:35:14.647Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The cognitive-emotional brain: Opportunitvnies and challenges for understanding neuropsychiatric disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Alexander J. Shackman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology; Affective & Translational Neuroscience Laboratory; Neuroscience & Cognitive Science Program; Maryland Neuroimaging Center; University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742. shackman@umd.eduhttp://shackmanlab.org
Andrew S. Fox
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry; HealthEmotions Research Institute; Wisconsin Psychiatric Institute & Clinics; University of Wisconsin–Madison; Madison, WI 53719. asfox@wisc.eduhttp://brainimaging.waisman.wisc.edu/~fox/
David A. Seminowicz
Affiliation:
Department of Neural and Pain Sciences; School of Dentistry; University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD 21201. dseminowicz@umaryland.eduhttps://www.dental.umaryland.edu/neuralpain/clinical-and-translational-research/dr-seminowicz/

Abstract

Many of the most common neuropsychiatric disorders are marked by prominent disturbances of cognition and emotion. Characterizing the complex neural circuitry underlying the interplay of cognition and emotion is critically important, not just for clarifying the nature of the mind, but also for discovering the root causes of a broad spectrum of debilitating neuropsychiatric disorders, including anxiety, schizophrenia, and chronic pain.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Until the twentieth century, the study of cognition and emotion was largely a philosophical matter. But recent years have witnessed the emergence of powerful new tools for interrogating the brain and new areas of multidisciplinary research focused on identifying the neurobiological mechanisms underlying cognition, emotion, and their role in mental health and disease. In The Cognitive-Emotional Brain, Luiz Pessoa (Reference Pessoa2013) provides an authoritative perspective on this recent work and its implications for our understanding of the basic building blocks of the mind. Here, we highlight four of the book's most important implications for understanding neuropsychiatric disorders, including anxiety, schizophrenia, substance abuse, chronic pain, and autism. These disorders cause enormous suffering for millions of patients and their families, outstripping the global burden of cancer or cardiovascular disease (Collins et al. Reference Collins, Patel, Joestl, March, Insel, Daar, Anderson, Dhansay, Phillips, Shurin, Walport, Ewart, Savill, Bordin, Costello, Durkin, Fairburn, Glass, Hall, Huang, Hyman, Jamison, Kaaya, Kapur, Kleinman, Ogunniyi, Otero-Ojeda, Poo, Ravindranath, Sahakian, Saxena, Singer and Stein2011; Goldberg & McGee Reference Goldberg and McGee2011; Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Wittchen2012; Whiteford et al. Reference Whiteford, Degenhardt, Rehm, Baxter, Ferrari, Erskine, Charlson, Norman, Flaxman, Johns, Burstein, Murray and Vos2013). Notably, these disorders involve prominent alterations in both cognition and emotion (Millan et al. Reference Millan, Agid, Brune, Bullmore, Carter, Clayton, Connor, Davis, Deakin, DeRubeis, Dubois, Geyer, Goodwin, Gorwood, Jay, Joëls, Mansuy, Meyer-Lindenberg, Murphy, Rolls, Saletu, Spedding, Sweeney, Whittington and Young2012), pointing to the need for a deeper understanding of the cognitive-emotional brain.

First, The Cognitive-Emotional Brain reminds us that mental faculties emerge from the coordinated interactions of large-scale brain networks. Put simply, fear, reward, attention, and other psychological processes cannot be mapped to isolated brain regions because no one region is both necessary and sufficient. Conversely, similar symptoms can emerge from damage to different regions in the same functional network (Karnath & Smith Reference Karnath and Smith2014). Pain, which is among the most prevalent clinical disorders (Institute of Medicine 2011), nicely illustrates this point. Pain is a multidimensional experience, involving systematic changes in both cognition and emotion: painful stimuli elicit anxiety, capture attention, and motivate action. Neurobiologically, pain is associated with a complex pattern of regional activation, often termed the “pain matrix” (Iannetti et al. Reference Iannetti, Salomons, Moayedi, Mouraux and Davis2013). Stimulation of individual components of the pain matrix does not consistently elicit pain, suggesting that pain and its disorders are emergent properties of regional interactions. This is not a new or contentious idea; pioneers like Mesulam, Goldman-Rakic, and LeDoux highlighted the importance of distributed neural circuits more than two decades ago, and there is widespread agreement among basic and translational researchers (Bullmore & Sporns Reference Bullmore and Sporns2012; Fornito et al. Reference Fornito, Zalesky and Breakspear2015; Goldman-Rakic Reference Goldman-Rakic1988; LeDoux Reference LeDoux1995; Reference LeDoux2012; Mesulam Reference Mesulam1998; Turk-Browne, Reference Turk-Browne2013; Uhlhaas & Singer Reference Uhlhaas and Singer2012). The Cognitive-Emotional Brain is a bracing call for accelerating the transition from localization strategies (i.e., mapping brain structures to function; sometimes termed “neophrenology”) to a network-centered approach. From a clinical neuroscience perspective, this suggests that understanding neuropsychiatric disorders will require embracing the kinds of analytic tools (e.g., functional connectivity fingerprinting, graph theoretic and machine learning approaches) that are necessary for elucidating how psychological constructs and mental disorders are realized in brain circuits (Turk-Browne Reference Turk-Browne2013; Woo et al. Reference Woo, Koban, Kross, Lindquist, Banich, Ruzic, Andrews-Hanna and Wager2014).

Pessoa's second key conclusion is that the identity of brain functional networks, including the circuitry that underlies clinically relevant phenotypes, cannot be inferred from neuroanatomy alone. Pessoa makes it clear that the networks identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and other neurophysiological techniques do not necessarily recapitulate the pattern of direct connections revealed by invasive anatomical tracing techniques. Indeed, there is ample evidence of robust functional connectivity between brain regions that lack direct structural connections (Adachi et al. Reference Adachi, Osada, Sporns, Watanabe, Matsui, Miyamoto and Miyashita2012; Birn et al. Reference Birn, Shackman, Oler, Williams, McFarlin, Rogers, Shelton, Alexander, Pine, Slattery, Davidson, Fox and Kalin2014; Honey et al. Reference Honey, Sporns, Cammoun, Gigandet, Thiran, Meuli and Hagmann2009; Vincent et al. Reference Vincent, Patel, Fox, Snyder, Baker, Van Essen, Zempel, Snyder, Corbetta and Raichle2007) and increasing evidence that regulatory signals can propagate across complex, indirect pathways (Ekstrom et al. Reference Ekstrom, Roelfsema, Arsenault, Bonmassar and Vanduffel2008). From a clinical perspective, this indicates that fMRI-derived measures of functional connectivity are particularly useful because they can be used to assay dysfunctional networks that encompass polysynaptically connected nodes (Birn et al. Reference Birn, Shackman, Oler, Williams, McFarlin, Rogers, Shelton, Alexander, Pine, Slattery, Davidson, Fox and Kalin2014), just as viral tracers can be used to delineate polysynaptic anatomical pathways in the nervous system (Dum et al. Reference Dum, Levinthal and Strick2009). More broadly, The Cognitive-Emotional Brain implies that many of the signs and symptoms of mental disorders – anhedonia, hypervigilance for threat, working memory impairments, drug seeking, and so on – will reflect complex brain circuits (Okon-Singer et al. Reference Okon-Singer, Hendler, Pessoa and Shackman2015; Seminowicz et al. Reference Seminowicz, Mayberg, McIntosh, Goldapple, Kennedy, Segal and Rafi-Tari2004; Shackman et al. Reference Shackman, Fox, Oler, Shelton, Davidson and Kalin2013; Stout et al. Reference Stout, Shackman and Larson2013).

The third key conclusion is that emotion and cognition are not different in kind but are instead deeply interwoven in the fabric of the brain. Subjectively, we often experience cognition and emotion as fundamentally different. Emotion is saturated with feelings of pleasure or pain and manifests in readily discerned changes in the body, whereas cognition often appears devoid of substantial hedonic, motivational, or somatic features. These apparent differences in phenomenological experience and peripheral physiology have led many scholars to treat emotion and cognition as categorically distinct, even oppositional, mental forces that presumably reflect the operation of segregated brain circuits (de Sousa Reference de Sousa and Zalta2014; Schmitter Reference Schmitter and Zalta2014). A similar dichotomy pervades psychiatric nosology. But careful scrutiny reveals contrary evidence; cognition can arouse the face and body; conversely, emotion can profoundly alter attention, working memory, and cognitive control (Grupe & Nitschke Reference Grupe and Nitschke2013; Okon-Singer et al. Reference Okon-Singer, Hendler, Pessoa and Shackman2015; Shackman et al. Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011). The Cognitive-Emotional Brain provides a useful survey of recent brain imaging research demonstrating the integration of emotional and cognitive processes in the brain (Shackman et al. Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011). Largely on the basis of brain imaging data, Pessoa joins with other theorists in rejecting claims that emotion and cognition are categorically different (Barrett & Satpute Reference Barrett and Satpute2013; Damasio 2005; Duncan & Barrett Reference Duncan and Barrett2007; Lindquist & Barrett Reference Lindquist and Barrett2012). Elucidating the contribution of the cognitive-emotional brain to psychopathology mandates the joint efforts of cognitive, affective, computational, and clinical neuroscientists. This kind of multidisciplinary research would refine our understanding of the mechanisms that give rise to “mixed” cognitive-emotional symptoms, such as hypervigilance or aberrant reinforcement learning (Cavanagh & Shackman Reference Cavanagh and Shackman2014), and provide novel targets for intervention.

Pessoa's fourth and most original conclusion is a powerful synthesis of the first three. Pessoa argues that widely held beliefs about the constituents of “the emotional brain” and “the cognitive brain” are fundamentally flawed. Regions such as the amygdala are not “emotional,” and regions such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) are not “cognitive” (Birn et al. Reference Birn, Shackman, Oler, Williams, McFarlin, Rogers, Shelton, Alexander, Pine, Slattery, Davidson, Fox and Kalin2014; Buhle et al. Reference Buhle, Silvers, Wager, Lopez, Onyemekwu, Kober, Weber and Ochsner2014; Fox et al. Reference Fox, Oakes, Shelton, Converse, Davidson and Kalin2005; Shackman et al. Reference Shackman, McMenamin, Maxwell, Greischar and Davidson2009). Both regions play a central role in the regulation of adaptive behavior. This should not be surprising – the human brain did not evolve to optimize performance on artificial laboratory probes of “pure” cognition or emotion. Pessoa also makes it clear that brain regions can dynamically assume different roles. Just as an individual can perform psychologically distinct roles in different social networks (e.g., executive, mother, sister, daughter), brain regions are poised to perform a range of functions (a property termed functional “superimposition”) in different neural “contexts” corresponding to their level of participation in particular functional networks. To paraphrase Pearson and colleagues (Pearson et al. Reference Pearson, Watson and Platt2014), key brain regions, such as the orbitofrontal cortex, are functionally heterogeneous, with individual neurons dynamically multiplexed into different functional roles. As such, they will “evade a single, modular, functional role assignment” (p. 954). Our brain reflects evolutionary pressures that demanded distributed neural systems capable of using information about pleasure and pain, derived from stimuli saturated with hedonic and motivational significance, to adaptively regulate attention, learning, somatic mobilization, and action in the service of maximizing reproductive fitness. From this perspective, it is easy to imagine how dysfunction of circumscribed territories of the brain can have a deep impact on distal regions and circuits, as recent work by our group and others demonstrates (Fox & Kalin Reference Fox and Kalin2014; Fox et al. Reference Fox, Shelton, Oakes, Converse, Davidson and Kalin2010; Gratton et al. Reference Gratton, Nomura, Perez and D'Esposito2012). This may help to explain the co-occurrence of cognitive and emotional symptoms, as well as frequent comorbidities, among psychiatric and neurological disorders. Clarifying the nature of the cognitive-emotional brain is likely to have substantial benefits for our understanding of disorders marked by symptoms that blend elements of cognition and emotion (e.g., hypervigilance to potential threat or overgeneralization of threat, in the case of the anxiety disorders [Grupe & Nitschke Reference Grupe and Nitschke2013]).

Although many challenges remain, The Cognitive-Emotional Brain provides a road map to the most fruitful avenues for future research. One of the most important unresolved questions concerns the functional significance of regions activated by both cognitive and emotional challenges. For example, Pessoa highlights a recent meta-analysis from our group demonstrating that the elicitation of negative affect, pain, and cognitive control are all associated with activation in an overlapping region of the MCC (Shackman et al. Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011). A key unresolved question is whether the MCC and other regions implicated in both cognitive and emotional processes, such as the anterior insula, perform a single general function (e.g., adaptive control [Cavanagh & Shackman, in press; Shackman et al. Reference Shackman, Salomons, Slagter, Fox, Winter and Davidson2011]) or salience detection (Iannetti et al. Reference Iannetti, Salomons, Moayedi, Mouraux and Davis2013) or multiple specific functions.

On a broader note, much of the evidence surveyed by Pessoa comes from the human brain imaging literature. Accordingly, his conclusions are ultimately tempered by questions about the origins and significance of the fMRI signal and the measures of functional connectivity that underlie network-centered approaches to understanding the cognitive-emotional brain (Akam & Kullmann Reference Akam and Kullmann2014; Cabral et al. Reference Cabral, Kringelbach and Deco2014; Logothetis Reference Logothetis2008). An important challenge for future studies will be to combine mechanistic techniques in animal models (e.g., optogenetics) with the same whole-brain imaging strategies routinely applied in humans (Birn et al. Reference Birn, Shackman, Oler, Williams, McFarlin, Rogers, Shelton, Alexander, Pine, Slattery, Davidson, Fox and Kalin2014; Borsook et al. Reference Borsook, Becerra and Hargreaves2006; Casey et al. Reference Casey, Craddock, Cuthbert, Hyman, Lee and Ressler2013; Narayanan et al. Reference Narayanan, Cavanagh, Frank and Laubach2013; Oler et al. Reference Oler, Birn, Patriat, Fox, Shelton, Burghy, Stodola, Essex, Davidson and Kalin2012; Roseboom et al. Reference Roseboom, Nanda, Fox, Oler, Shackman, Shelton, Davidson and Kalin2014). Combining noninvasive mechanistic techniques (e.g., transcranial magnetic stimulation or transcranial direct current stimulation) or pharmacological manipulations with fMRI provides another opportunity for understanding how circumscribed perturbations can produce distributed dysfunction (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Oathes, Chang, Bradley, Zhou, Williams, Glover, Deisseroth and Etkin2013; Guller et al. Reference Guller, Ferrarelli, Shackman, Sarasso, Peterson, Langheim, Meyerand, Tononi and Postle2012; Paulus et al. Reference Paulus, Feinstein, Castillo, Simmons and Stein2005; Reinhart & Woodman Reference Reinhart and Woodman2014).

For many disorders marked by cognitive and emotional disturbances, extant treatments are inconsistently effective or associated with significant adverse effects (e.g., Bystritsky Reference Bystritsky2006). The Cognitive-Emotional Brain provides an insightful survey of state of the science and a useful stimulus for the next generation of basic and clinical research, reminding us that we have a remarkable opportunity to use new tools for understanding brain function to discover the origins of neuropsychiatric disease.

References

Adachi, Y., Osada, T., Sporns, O., Watanabe, T., Matsui, T., Miyamoto, K. & Miyashita, Y. (2012) Functional connectivity between anatomically unconnected areas is shaped by collective network-level effects in the macaque cortex. Cerebral Cortex 22(7):1586–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akam, T. & Kullmann, D. M. (2014) Oscillatory multiplexing of population codes for selective communication in the mammalian brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 15:111–22. doi: 10.1038/nrn3668.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. & Satpute, A. B. (2013) Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: Towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 23(3):361–72. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birn, R. M., Shackman, A. J., Oler, J. A., Williams, L. E., McFarlin, D. R., Rogers, G. M., Shelton, S. E., Alexander, A. L., Pine, D. S., Slattery, M. J., Davidson, R. J., Fox, A. S. & Kalin, N. H. (2014) Evolutionarily-conserved dysfunction of prefrontal-amygdalar connectivity in early-life anxiety. Molecular Psychiatry 19:915–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borsook, D., Becerra, L. & Hargreaves, R. (2006) A role for fMRI in optimizing CNS drug development. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 5(5):411–24. doi: 10.1038/nrd2027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buhle, J.T., Silvers, J. A., Wager, T. D., Lopez, R., Onyemekwu, C., Kober, H., Weber, J., Ochsner, K. N. (2014) Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex 24: 2981–90. doi: bht154 [pii] 10.1093/cercor/bht154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullmore, E. & Sporns, O. (2012) The economy of brain network organization. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13(5):336–49. doi: 10.1038/nrn3214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bystritsky, A. (2006) Treatment-resistant anxiety disorders. Molecular Psychiatry 11:805–14. doi: 4001852 [pii]10.1038/sj.mp.4001852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabral, J., Kringelbach, M. L. & Deco, G. (2014) Exploring the network dynamics underlying brain activity during rest. Progress in Neurobiology 114C:102–31. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.12.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casey, B. J., Craddock, N., Cuthbert, B. N., Hyman, S. E., Lee, F. S. & Ressler, K. J. (2013) DSM-5 and RDoC: Progress in psychiatry research? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14(11):810–14. doi: 10.1038/nrn3621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, J. F., & Shackman, A. J. (2014) Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: Meta-analytic evidence. Journal of Physiology, Paris. pii: S0928-4257(14)00014-X. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003. [Epub ahead of print].Google ScholarPubMed
Chen, A. C., Oathes, D. J., Chang, C., Bradley, T., Zhou, Z. W., Williams, L. M., Glover, G. H., Deisseroth, K. & Etkin, A. (2013) Causal interactions between fronto-parietal central executive and default-mode networks in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110(49):19944–49. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1311772110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, P. Y., Patel, V., Joestl, S. S., March, D., Insel, T. R., Daar, A. S., Anderson, W., Dhansay, M. A., Phillips, A., Shurin, S., Walport, M., Ewart, W., Savill, S. J., Bordin, I. A., Costello, E. J., Durkin, M., Fairburn, C., Glass, R. I., Hall, W., Huang, Y., Hyman, S. E., Jamison, K., Kaaya, S., Kapur, S., Kleinman, A., Ogunniyi, A., Otero-Ojeda, A., Poo, M. M., Ravindranath, V., Sahakian, B. J., Saxena, S., Singer, P. A. & Stein, D. J. (2011) Grand challenges in global mental health. Nature 475:2730. doi: 475027a [pii]10.1038/475027a.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. (1994/2005) Descartes' error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. Penguin. (Original work published by Putnam in 1994).Google Scholar
de Sousa, R. (2014) Emotion. In: Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, ed. Zalta, E. N.. Available at: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/emotion/.Google Scholar
Dum, R. P., Levinthal, D. J. & Strick, P. L. (2009) The spinothalamic system targets motor and sensory areas in the cerebral cortex of monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience 29:14223–35. doi: 29/45/14223 [pii] 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3398-09.2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, S. & Barrett, L. F. (2007) Affect is a form of cognition: A neurobiological analysis. Cognition & Emotion 21:1184–211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekstrom, L. B., Roelfsema, P. R., Arsenault, J. T., Bonmassar, G. & Vanduffel, W. (2008) Bottom-up dependent gating of frontal signals in early visual cortex. Science 321:414–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fornito, A., Zalesky, A. & Breakspear, M. (2015) The connectomics of brain disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 16:159–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, A. S. & Kalin, N. H. (2014) A translational neuroscience approach to understanding the development of social anxiety disorder and its pathophysiology. American Journal of Psychiatry 171:1162–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, A. S., Oakes, T. R., Shelton, S. E., Converse, A. K., Davidson, R. J. & Kalin, N. H. (2005) Calling for help is independently modulated by brain systems underlying goal-directed behavior and threat perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102:4176–79. doi: 0409470102 [pii] 10.1073/pnas.0409470102 [doi].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, A. S., Shelton, S. E., Oakes, T. R., Converse, A. K., Davidson, R. J. & Kalin, N. H. (2010) Orbitofrontal cortex lesions alter anxiety-related activity in the primate bed nucleus of stria terminalis. Journal of Neuroscience 30:7023–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, D. S. & McGee, S. J. (2011) Pain as a global public health priority. BMC Public Health 11:770. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1988) Topography of cognition: Parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience 11:137–56. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratton, C., Nomura, E. M., Perez, F. & D'Esposito, M. (2012) Focal brain lesions to critical locations cause widespread disruption of the modular organization of the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24(6):1275–85. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grupe, D. W. & Nitschke, J. B. (2013) Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: An integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 14:488501. doi: nrn3524 [pii]10.1038/nrn3524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guller, Y., Ferrarelli, F., Shackman, A. J., Sarasso, S., Peterson, M. J., Langheim, F. J., Meyerand, M. E., Tononi, G. & Postle, B. R. (2012) Probing thalamic integrity in schizophrenia using concurrent transcranial magnetic stimulation and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of General Psychiatry 69(7):662–71. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honey, C. J., Sporns, O., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Thiran, J. P., Meuli, R. & Hagmann, P. (2009) Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(6):2035–40. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811168106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iannetti, G. D., Salomons, T. V., Moayedi, M., Mouraux, A. & Davis, K. D. (2013) Beyond metaphor: Contrasting mechanisms of social and physical pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17:371–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IOM (Institute of Medicine) (2011) Relieving pain in America: A blueprint for transforming prevention, care, education, and research. National Academies Press. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22553896 Google Scholar
Karnath, H.-O. & Smith, D. V. (2014) The next step in modern brain lesion analysis: Multivariate pattern analysis. Brain 137:2405–406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M. & Wittchen, H. U. (2012) Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 21:169–84. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (1995) Emotion: Clues from the brain. Annual Review of Psychology 46:209–35. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (2012) Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73(4):653–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindquist, K. A. & Barrett, L. F. (2012) A functional architecture of the human brain: Emerging insights from the science of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16(11):533–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logothetis, N. K. (2008) What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature 453:869–78. doi: nature06976 [pii]10.1038/nature06976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesulam, M. M. (1998) From sensation to cognition. Brain 121(Pt 6):1013–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millan, M. J., Agid, Y., Brune, M., Bullmore, E. T., Carter, C. S., Clayton, N. S., Connor, R., Davis, S., Deakin, B., DeRubeis, R. J., Dubois, B., Geyer, M. A., Goodwin, G. M., Gorwood, P., Jay, T. M., Joëls, M., Mansuy, I. M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Murphy, D., Rolls, E., Saletu, B., Spedding, M., Sweeney, J., Whittington, M. & Young, L. J. (2012) Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 11(2):141–68. doi: 10.1038/nrd3628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narayanan, N. S., Cavanagh, J. F., Frank, M. J. & Laubach, M. (2013) Common medial frontal mechanisms of adaptive control in humans and rodents. Nature Neuroscience 16(12):1888–95. doi: 10.1038/nn.3549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okon-Singer, H., Hendler, T., Pessoa, L. & Shackman, A. J. (2015) The neurobiology of emotion-cognition interactions: Fundamental questions and strategies for future research. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9: 58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oler, J. A., Birn, R. M., Patriat, R., Fox, A. S., Shelton, S. E., Burghy, C. A., Stodola, D. E., Essex, M. J., Davidson, R. J. & Kalin, N. H. (2012) Evidence for coordinated functional activity within the extended amygdala of non-human and human primates. NeuroImage 61:1059–66. doi: S1053-8119(12)00326-6 [pii]10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, M. P., Feinstein, J. S., Castillo, G., Simmons, A. N. & Stein, M. B. (2005) Dose-dependent decrease of activation in bilateral amygdala and insula by lorazepam during emotion processing. Archives of General Psychiatry 62(3):282–88. doi: 62/3/282 [pii]10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, J. M., Watson, K. K. & Platt, M. L. (2014) Decision making: The neuroethological turn. Neuron 82(5):950–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pessoa, L. (2013) The cognitive-emotional brain. From interactions to integration. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reinhart, R. M. & Woodman, G. F. (2014) Causal control of medial-frontal cortex governs electrophysiological and behavioral indices of performance monitoring and learning. Journal of Neuroscience 34(12):4214–27. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5421-13.2014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roseboom, P. H., Nanda, S. A., Fox, A. S., Oler, J. A., Shackman, A. J., Shelton, S. E., Davidson, R. J. & Kalin, N. H. (2014) Neuropeptide Y receptor gene expression in the primate amygdala predicts anxious temperament and brain metabolism. Biological Psychiatry 76: 850–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitter, A. M. (2014) 17th and 18th century theories of emotions. In: Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, ed. Zalta, E. N.. Available at: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/emotions-17th18th/.Google Scholar
Seminowicz, D. A., Mayberg, H. S., McIntosh, A. R., Goldapple, K., Kennedy, S., Segal, Z. & Rafi-Tari, S. (2004) Limbic-frontal circuitry in major depression: A path modeling metanalysis. NeuroImage 22:409–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.015 S1053811904000497 [pii].CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shackman, A. J., Fox, A. S., Oler, J. A., Shelton, S. E., Davidson, R. J. & Kalin, N. H. (2013) Neural mechanisms underlying heterogeneity in the presentation of anxious temperament. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110:6145–50. doi: 1214364110 [pii] 10.1073/pnas.1214364110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shackman, A. J., McMenamin, B. W., Maxwell, J. S., Greischar, L. L. & Davidson, R. J. (2009) Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and behavioral inhibition. Psychological Science 20:1500–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J. & Davidson, R. J. (2011) The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 12(3):154–67. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21331082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, D. M., Shackman, A. J. & Larson, C. L. (2013) Failure to filter: Anxious individuals show inefficient gating of threat from working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7:58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turk-Browne, N. B. (2013) Functional interactions as big data in the human brain. Science 342:580–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uhlhaas, P. J. & Singer, W. (2012) Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric disorders: Toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks. Neuron 75(6):963–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vincent, J. L., Patel, G. H., Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Baker, J. T., Van Essen, D. C., Zempel, J. M., Snyder, L. H., Corbetta, M. & Raichle, M. E. (2007) Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 447:8386. doi: nature05758 [pii]10.1038/nature05758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiteford, H. A., Degenhardt, L., Rehm, J., Baxter, A. J., Ferrari, A. J., Erskine, H. E., Charlson, F. J., Norman, R. E., Flaxman, A. D., Johns, N., Burstein, R., Murray, C. J. & Vos, T. (2013) Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet 382(9904):1575–86. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61611-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woo, C. W., Koban, L., Kross, E., Lindquist, M. A., Banich, M. T., Ruzic, L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R. & Wager, T. D. (2014) Separate neural representations for physical pain and social rejection. Nature Communications 5:5380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed