Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T13:26:58.892Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Fluency: A trigger of familiarity for relational representations?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 January 2020

Talya Sadeh*
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Zlotowski Center for Neuroscience & Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, POB 653, Beer Sheva8410501, Israel. tsadeh@bgu.ac.ilhttp://bgu.ac.il/~tsadeh/index.php

Abstract

According to Bastin et al.’s integrative memory model, familiarity may be attributed to both entity representations and relational representations. However, the model does not specify what triggers familiarity for relational representations. I argue that fluency is a key player in the attribution of familiarity regardless of the type of representation. Two lines of evidence are reviewed in support of my claim.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Bastin et al. make a valiant attempt to rejuvenate the dual-process theory of recognition – a theory formulated more than 40 years ago, and which has triggered intense debate and research ever since (Dunn Reference Dunn2004; Wixted Reference Wixted2007; Wixted & Mickes Reference Wixted and Mickes2010; Yonelinas Reference Yonelinas2002). Their attempt yields the integrative memory model. The most novel and significant aspect of this model is the clear distinction between mnemonic representations – entity and relational – and the subjective experiences – familiarity and recollection – that are attributed to reactivation of the corresponding representations. This distinction has been misleadingly blurred in previous dual-process model of recognition. According to the integrative memory model, familiarity is typically associated with entity representations and recollection is typically associated with relational representations. However, recollection and familiarity are not necessarily triggered by relational and entity representations, respectively. Thus, the model makes the novel prediction that the subjective experience of familiarity may also be triggered by relational representations and, likewise, the subjective experience of recollection may also be triggered by entity representations (though this latter point is only implied, and not explicitly mentioned in the target article). This is an important prediction – perhaps the most important prediction that the model makes. However, its implications are not fully explored by the authors and are not sufficiently elaborated on. In this commentary, I focus on one specific aspect of this prediction – namely, that reactivation of a trace in the relational core system may trigger familiarity.

Bastin et al. claim that “even if the relational representation core system reactivates specific item-context details, one may experience a feeling of familiarity” (sect. 3, para. 2). This claim begs the question: What gives rise to this feeling of familiarity? However, the model remains mute regarding the source for the experience of familiarity for relational representations. For entity representations, the source triggering familiarity is the fluency heuristic, defined as “the speed and ease with which a stimulus is processed” (sect. 2, para. 2). Importantly, while mentioning other factors that are potential sources of familiarity (e.g., proprioceptive and affective information), the authors acknowledge that these may also be intricately linked to fluency – either being a by-product of fluency, or by triggering fluency (see sect. 4.2.1). Thus, familiarity emerges predominantly from the fluency with which a stimulus is processed. In line with the vast majority of the relevant literature (e.g., Kleider & Goldinger Reference Kleider and Goldinger2004; Whittlesea et al. Reference Whittlesea, Jacoby and Girard1990; Whittlesea & Leboe Reference Whittlesea and Leboe2000), Bastin et al. describe fluency only as a property of processing entity representations (e.g., single words, pictures). However, I maintain that there is no reason to preclude fluency as a relevant property of relational representations as well. Hence, a feasible source for the subjective experience of familiarity for relational representations is the fluency with which these representations are processed. Two lines of evidence support this notion.

First, though fluency is typically examined in the context of single entities, effects of fluency have also been demonstrated for information which can be construed as relational. Such relational information may include a variety of content types which, critically, involve the binding of two or more items – namely, forming a link between the items while preserving the meaning of each individual item (Eichenbaum et al. Reference Eichenbaum, Otto and Cohen1994). For instance, processing of arithmetic problems, which are comprised of several numbers and the relations between them, is affected by fluency (manipulated as the number of exposures to each problem; Paynter et al. Reference Paynter, Reder and Kieffaber2009; Reder & Ritter Reference Reder and Ritter1992). Sentences are an additional instance of relational information whose representations include both their constituent elements (namely, the words) and the relationships between them. Fluency for sentences has been manipulated both visually, by comparing sentences written in a degraded font to those written in a clear font (Alter et al. Reference Alter, Oppenheimer, Epley and Eyre2007; Laham et al. Reference Laham, Alter and Goodwin2009; Song & Schwarz Reference Song and Schwarz2008a), and aurally, by comparing sentences pronounced with a non-native accent versus a native accent (Lev-Ari & Keysar Reference Lev-Ari and Keysar2010). Interestingly, the effects of the fluency with which sentences got processed were demonstrated on various dependent measures, including syllogistic reasoning, speakers’ credibility, and moral judgments. Going beyond sentences, fluency has been shown to affect processing of whole paragraphs (Diemand-Yauman et al. Reference Diemand-Yauman, Oppenheimer and Vaughan2011; Song & Schwarz Reference Song and Schwarz2008b). Finally, fluency has also been shown to exert its effects on processing of ambiguous paintings which, as in previous examples of relational information, are composed of several elements and the relationships between them (Jakesch et al. Reference Jakesch, Leder and Forster2013).

A second line of evidence supporting the relevance of fluency to relational representations concerns repetition suppression – reduction of neural activity to repeated presentations of stimuli. Repetition suppression is regarded by many (apparently, Bastin et al. included) as a neural marker for fluency (e.g., Ward et al. Reference Ward, Chun and Kuhl2013). The target article describes several pieces of evidence for repetition suppression in the perirhinal cortex, a key structure in the entity representation core system. However, repetition suppression is not limited to neural structures within the entity representation system, and has also been demonstrated for relational representations in the hippocampus (Duncan et al. Reference Duncan, Ketz, Inati and Davachi2012; Düzel et al. Reference Düzel, Habib, Rotte, Guderian, Tulving and Heinze2003; Kumaran & Maguire Reference Kumaran and Maguire2006; Reference Kumaran and Maguire2007; Reference Kumaran and Maguire2009). For instance, in one study relational representations were operationalized as face–object and face–location associations (Düzel et al. Reference Düzel, Habib, Rotte, Guderian, Tulving and Heinze2003). Decreased activity in the hippocampus was found for repeated associations (intact pairs) versus novel associations (recombined pairs). Thus, the hippocampus – a key structure in the relational representation core system – also exhibits repetition suppression, the neural correlate of fluency.

To conclude, the integrative memory model makes the novel prediction that familiarity can be attributed to relational representations. However, the model does not specify what would lead to this attribution. Therefore, an exciting avenue for future research is to elucidate the sources of information or heuristics that may give rise to familiarity for relational representations. Based on the two lines of evidence reviewed above, I suggest considering fluency as a major candidate.

References

Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N. & Eyre, R. N. (2007) Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136(4):569–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M. & Vaughan, E. B. (2011) Fortune favors the Bold (and the Italicized): Effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition 118(1):111–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, K., Ketz, N., Inati, S. J. & Davachi, L. (2012) Evidence for area CA1 as a match/mismatch detector: A high-resolution fMRI study of the human hippocampus. Hippocampus 22(3):389–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, J. C. (2004) Remember-know: A matter of confidence. Psychological Review 111(2):524–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Düzel, E., Habib, R., Rotte, M., Guderian, S., Tulving, E. & Heinze, H.-J. (2003) Human hippocampal and parahippocampal activity during visual associative recognition memory for spatial and nonspatial stimulus configurations. Journal of Neuroscience 23(28):9439–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eichenbaum, H., Otto, T. & Cohen, N. J. (1994) Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17(3):449–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakesch, M., Leder, H. & Forster, M. (2013) Image ambiguity and fluency. PLOS ONE 8(9):e74084. Available at: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0074084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleider, H. M. & Goldinger, S. D. (2004) Illusions of face memory: Clarity breeds familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language 50(2):196211. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2003.09.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumaran, D. & Maguire, E. A. (2006) An unexpected sequence of events: Mismatch detection in the human hippocampus. PLOS Biology 4(12):e424. Available at: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/article?id=10.1371/journal.pbio.0040424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumaran, D. & Maguire, E. A. (2007) Match–mismatch processes underlie human hippocampal responses to associative novelty. Journal of Neuroscience 27(32):8517–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumaran, D. & Maguire, E. A. (2009) Novelty signals: A window into hippocampal information processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(2):4754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laham, S. M., Alter, A. L. & Goodwin, G. P. (2009) Easy on the mind, easy on the wrongdoer: Discrepantly fluent violations are deemed less morally wrong. Cognition 112(3):462–66. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lev-Ari, S. & Keysar, B. (2010) Why don't we believe non-native speakers? The influence of accent on credibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46(6):1093–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paynter, C. A., Reder, L. M. & Kieffaber, P. D. (2009) Knowing we know before we know: ERP correlates of initial feeling-of-knowing. Neuropsychologia 47(3):796803. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reder, L. M. & Ritter, F. E. (1992) What determines initial feeling of knowing? Familiarity with question terms, not with the answer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18(3):435–51. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.435.Google Scholar
Song, H. & Schwarz, N. (2008a) Fluency and the detection of misleading questions: Low processing fluency attenuates the Moses illusion. Social Cognition 26(6):791–99. doi:10.1521/soco.2008.26.6.791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, H. & Schwarz, N. (2008b) If it's hard to read, it's hard to do: Processing fluency affects effort prediction and motivation. Psychological Science 19(10):986–88. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02189.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, E. J., Chun, M. M. & Kuhl, B. A. (2013) Repetition suppression and multi-voxel pattern similarity differentially track implicit and explicit visual memory. Journal of Neuroscience 33(37):14749–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittlesea, B. W. A., Jacoby, L. L. & Girard, K. (1990) Illusions of immediate memory: Evidence of an attributional basis for feelings of familiarity and perceptual quality. Journal of Memory and Language 29(6):716–32. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90045-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whittlesea, B. W. A. & Leboe, J. P. (2000) The heuristic basis of remembering and classification: Fluency, generation, and resemblance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129(1):84106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wixted, J. T. (2007) Dual-process theory and signal-detection theory of recognition memory. Psychological Review 114(1):152–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wixted, J. T. & Mickes, L. (2010) A continuous dual-process model of remember/know judgments. Psychological Review 117(4):1025–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonelinas, A. P. (2002) The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language 46(3):441517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar