Introduction
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder with defining characteristics of inattention and/or hyperactivity–impulsivity, and symptom onset before the age of 12 years [Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5); American Psychiatric Association, 2013]. Persistence of ADHD symptoms into adulthood has been frequently described (Gittelman et al. Reference Gittelman, Manuzza, Shenker and Bonagura1985; Barkley et al. Reference Barkley, Fischer, Smallish and Fletcher2002; Faraone & Biederman, Reference Faraone and Biederman2006; Simon et al. Reference Simon, Czobor, Balint, Mészarós and Bitter2009) and ADHD symptoms have profound implications for academic achievement, social functioning, self-esteem and mental health across the lifespan (Manuzza et al. Reference Manuzza, Gittelman Klein and Addalli1991; Barkley et al. Reference Barkley, Fischer, Smallish and Fletcher2002; Faraone & Biederman, Reference Faraone and Biederman2006). These consequences endorse the need for effective interventions for ADHD from childhood into adulthood.
Methylphenidate (MPH) is a psychostimulant frequently prescribed in the treatment of ADHD. It increases the availability of extracellular dopamine (DA) and noradrenaline (NA) by blocking the DA transporter and the NA transporter in striatal and prefrontal areas (Koda et al. Reference Koda, Ago, Cong, Takuma and Matsuda2010; Volkow et al. Reference Volkow, Wang, Tomasi, Kollins, Wigal, Newcorn, Telang, Fowler, Logan, Wong and Swanson2012). These areas are volumetrically smaller and functionally less activated in people with ADHD (Durston et al. Reference Durston, Tottenham, Thomas, Davidson, Eigsti, Yang, Ulug and Casey2003; Nakao et al. Reference Nakao, Radua, Rubia and Mataix-Cols2011; Cortese et al. Reference Cortese, Kelly, Chabernaud, Proal, Di Martino, Milham and Castellanos2012; Frodl & Skokauskas, Reference Frodl and Skokauskas2012; Hart et al. Reference Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols and Rubia2013). In typically developing individuals, maturation of specific brain areas, particularly the prefrontal cortex and frontal–temporal connections, continues well into adulthood (Giedd, Reference Giedd2004; Shaw et al. Reference Shaw, Kabani, Lerch, Eckstrand, Lenroot, Gogtay, Greenstein, Clasen, Evans, Rapoport, Giedd and Wise2008; Westlyle et al. Reference Westlyle, Walhovd, Dale, Bjørnerud, Due-Tønnessen, Engvig, Grydeland, Tamnes, Østby and Fjell2010; Lebel et al. Reference Lebel, Gee, Camicioli, Wieler, Martin and Beaulieu2012). Although the temporal sequence of development of different brain areas in ADHD is comparable with that in typically developing children, peak thickness of the prefrontal, temporal and occipital cortices is attained at a later age in children with ADHD (Shaw et al. Reference Shaw, Eckstrand, Sharp, Blumenthal, Lerch, Greenstein, Clasen, Evans, Giedd and Rapoport2007). As neurotransmitter systems change drastically from early postnatal time to early adulthood, with a peak of synaptogenesis and pruning in the prefrontal cortex around adolescence (Blakemore & Choudhury, Reference Blakemore and Choudhury2006), it could be argued that sensitivity to MPH is age-dependent.
In line with this hypothesis, animal studies have shown different behavioral responses, reflecting cognitive processes, to stimulant administration in juvenile as compared with adult animals. Some studies have reported a reduced sensitivity in young animals following stimulant administration. For example, peri-adolescent rats exposed to a single challenge of amphetamine responded with less locomotion activity, or subsensitivity, in comparison with adult rats (Bolanos et al. Reference Bolanos, Glatt and Jackson1998), and young mice exposed to a single challenge of MPH responded with less locomotion activity than peri-adolescent and adult mice (Niculescu et al. Reference Niculescu, Ehrlich and Unterwald2005). Other studies, however, suggest a higher sensitivity for juvenile as compared with adult animals. For example, a single dose of MPH has been shown to ameliorate an inhibition deficit in juvenile, but not in adult, spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR; an animal model of ADHD) (Bizot et al. Reference Bizot, Chenault, Houzé, Herpin, David, Pothion and Trovero2007). Together, these animal studies demonstrate that treatment effects of MPH may depend on the maturational level of the brain. However, it is currently unknown whether this holds true for the human brain.
In humans, the primary measure to determine whether MPH works adequately is change in ADHD symptomatology (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2001). When focusing on this specific outcome measure there seems to be no age-dependency in the MPH effect, as similar effect sizes have been reported in a meta-analysis including pediatric studies (effect size 0.79; Faraone & Buitelaar, Reference Faraone and Buitelaar2010) and in a meta-analysis including adult studies (effect size 0.96; Faraone & Glatt, Reference Faraone and Glatt2010). However, while cognitive processes are more closely related to brain maturation, hardly any human study has focused on the age-dependency of MPH effects on cognitive responses, or has summarized MPH effects in adults with ADHD. A study focusing on attention reported enhanced positive effects of MPH on a wide range of attentional functions in pre-school children when compared with grade-school children (Hanisch et al. Reference Hanisch, Konrad, Günther and Herpertz-Dahlmann2004). Seven (meta-analytic) reviews have systematically tested or described the influence of MPH on executive functions in ADHD (Kavale, Reference Kavale1982; Solanto, Reference Solanto1984; Losier et al. Reference Losier, McGrath and Klein1996; Riccio et al. Reference Riccio, Waldrop, Reynolds and Lowe2001; Pietrzak et al. Reference Pietrzak, Mollica, Maruff and Snyder2006; Chamberlain et al. Reference Chamberlain, Robbins, Winder-Rhodes, Müller, Sahakian, Blackwell and Barnett2011; Coghill et al. Reference Coghill, Seth, Pedroso, Usala, Currie and Gagliano2013), with only one (now dated) review summarizing beneficial effects of MPH on a broader range of cognitive functions in children as compared with adults with ADHD (Solanto, Reference Solanto1984). While a recent meta-analysis studied the effects of MPH on reaction time (variability), response inhibition, and (non-) executive memory (Coghill et al. Reference Coghill, Seth, Pedroso, Usala, Currie and Gagliano2013), this study included pediatric studies only. Since the possible age-dependency of MPH effects has not recently been addressed, the current study will test whether the effect of MPH on executive functioning in humans with ADHD is different across developmental stages.
In conclusion, it is unclear whether the magnitude of MPH effects on cognition depends on the maturational level of the human brain. In the current study, we will, therefore, focus on those functions that are known to be often compromised in ADHD and have been sufficiently studied in the context of MPH effects to conduct a meta-regression analysis. While a plethora of articles on the effects of MPH on executive functions of response inhibition, working memory and sustained attention (see also Coghill et al. Reference Coghill, Seth, Pedroso, Usala, Currie and Gagliano2013) have been published, there are hardly any studies focusing on MPH effects on, for example, motivation, reward sensitivity and timing (e.g. Shiels et al. Reference Shiels, Hawk, Reynolds, Mazzullo, Rhodes, Pelham, Waxmonsky and Gangloff2009; Luman et al. Reference Luman, Papanikolau and Oosterlaan2015). Hence, we will focus on the aforementioned executive functions. Fortunately, within the field of executive functions, the same neuropsychological tests are often used in ADHD research with children and adults, creating the opportunity for quantitative evaluation of a potential age effect. Thus, we conducted a meta-regression analysis, to test the hypotheses that the effects of MPH on response inhibition, working memory and sustained attention are moderated by age. Although not previously addressed in meta-analyses regarding the effects of MPH on executive functioning, previous research has shown that medication naivety (Schwartz & Correll, Reference Schwartz and Correll2014), dosage (Tannock et al. Reference Tannock, Schachar and Logan1995; Konrad et al. Reference Konrad, Günther, Hanisch and Herpertz-Dahlmann2004, Reference Konrad, Günther, Heinzel-Gutenbrunner and Herpertz-Dahlmann2005) and MPH formulation (Punja et al. Reference Punja, Zorzela, Hartling, Urichuk and Vohra2013) are additional potential moderators of the effects of stimulants. Therefore, we also included an explorative analysis of these moderators.
Method
Identification of studies
A comprehensive search of the literature was undertaken using search engines PubMed, PsycINFO, Web of Science and MEDLINE. Search terms used were ‘ADHD’, ‘ADD’, ‘HD’ or ‘hyperkinetic disorder’ AND ‘methylphenidate’ or ‘stimulants’ AND ‘neuropsychology’, ‘neuropsychological (test/task)’, ‘cognition’, ‘(response) inhibition’, ‘(working/verbal/declarative/spatial) memory’, ‘(sustained) attention (span)’, ‘vigilance’, ‘reaction time’, ‘variability’, ‘intra-individual variability’, ‘IIV’, ‘executive functions’, ‘(verbal) learning’, ‘(processing/psychomotor) speed’, ‘reaction time’, ‘Nback’, ‘SART’, ‘Continuous Performance’, ‘Stop Signal’ or ‘Go-NoGo’. In addition, meta-analyses, reviews and references were checked in search of relevant studies.
Studies that met the following criteria were included: (a) designs were double-blind, placebo-controlled medication trials with MPH [immediate release (IR) or osmotic release oral system (OROS)] with a parallel-groups or crossover design; (b) dependent variables were measures of pre-potent response inhibition, sustained attention or working memory (see section ‘Selection of dependent variables’); (c) population under study was diagnosed with ADHD according to DSM-III, DSM-III-TR, DSM-IV, DSM-IV-R, International Classification of Diseases-10 criteria, or scored above the cut-off on clinical rating scales of ADHD and qualified for pharmacological treatment; (d) studies reported sufficient data to allow for the calculation of effect sizes, or contact information of authors could be retraced in order to request sufficient data; (e) published articles should be presented in peer-reviewed journals between 1970 and March 2015, and should be written in English, German or Dutch; (f) articles should present original data. Studies assessing cognition in an imaging setting (e.g. functional magnetic resonance imaging, electroencephalography, functional near-infrared spectroscopy) were not excluded (see also the footnotes of Table 1).
Note that a positive effect size indicates better performance in the MPH condition as compared with the placebo condition.
s.d., Standard deviation; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; TOVA, Test of Variables of Attention; WISC, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; IR, immediate release; CANTAB, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery; SST, Stop Signal Task; GDS, Gordon Diagnostic System; d.d., de die (daily); CPT, continuous performance task; WRAML, Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning; SSRT, stop signal reaction time; OROS, osmotic release oral system; TEA-Ch, Test of Everyday Attention for Children; GNG, Go/No-Go; ANT, Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks; RT, reaction time; MPH, methylphenidate; MLR, multilayer-release; PASAT, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test; ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; CD, conduct disorder; s.e.m., standard error of the mean.
a Parallel-group design: demographic data of the treatment and placebo condition combined.
b Participants with a body weight within the normal range received a fixed dose, but dose was adjusted to a fixed high or low dose for participants with high or low body weight dosage.
c Effect sizes for two separate tests of the same functions were merged to reduce population bias. Merged data are reported here.
d Some participants met criteria for ADHD in remission or subthreshold ADHD.
e Trial with multiple fixed dosages: dosage with highest effect size selected and reported here.
f Number of participants assessed with the WISC [WRAML Finger Windows n = 59 (83%)].
g Data based on a larger number of participants than completed assessment.
h Demographic data reported for two groups separately were merged for the present analysis. Merged data are reported here.
i Effect size calculated with mean and s.d. obtained from median and range, see Hozo et al. (Reference Hozo, Djulbegovic and Moore2005).
j Number of participants assessed with the GNG and CANTAB spatial working memory between search errors [CANTAB Spatial Span n = 59 (100%)].
k Described in Rhodes et al. (Reference Rhodes, Coghill and Matthews2004). l Based on a larger number of participants (n = 75).
m Described in Klorman et al. (Reference Klorman, Coons and Borgstedt1987). n Rewarded CPT. oAssessment during magnetic resonance imaging.
p Trial comparing MPH with atomoxetine. q Percentage also includes participants with CD. r Previous adverse response as exclusion criterion.
s Only parents selected from trial with parents and children.
t Number of participants in total sample (n-back n = 75, SST n = 90, GNG n = 85).
u s.d. deducted from s.e.m.; see Higgins & Green (2011).
v Part of the total sample of n = 54 was already described in another included article (Konrad et al. Reference Konrad, Günther, Hanisch and Herpertz-Dahlmann2004), data of the remaining n = 25 were included here.
w Based on a larger number of participants (n = 54).
x Study comparing MPH with bupropion.
y Assessment during functional near-infrared spectroscopy.
z Attentional dysfunction on neuropsychological tests as inclusion criterion.
aa Study included responders.
bb Trial comparing MPH with l-dopa and desipramine.
cc Trial comparing MPH with paroxetine.
dd Assessment during electroencephalography.
ee Trial comparing MPH with dex-MPH.
ff Assessment during motion-tracking.
gg Weighted combination of RT, variability, omission and commission errors.
hh Standard score.
ii Data from dex-MPH and MPH condition combined.
jj Study with a withdrawal condition being administration of placebo during usual treatment.
kk Trial comparing MPH with Adderall.
See Fig. 1 for a flow diagram of the search results. Authors not reporting sufficient data for the calculation of effect sizes were contacted and requested to provide the missing data, as well as any unpublished data on the subject. After the initial search by the first author (H.G.H.T.), the extracted data and inclusion criteria were checked independently by a research assistant.
Selection of dependent variables
The majority of collected studies presented more than one dependent variable for each task. For each task, we selected the variable that best reflected the cognitive function of interest. If this variable was not reported, we selected the next variable. We planned to reduce heterogeneity by selecting the variable most frequently reported in other collected articles, if two or more dependent variables were considered to reflect a cognitive function equally well; however, this was never the case.
Moreover, as some articles presented data from multiple designs, settings, dosages or inter-stimulus intervals, we only included the data with the largest effect size in these cases, assuming that the study design in which these largest effects were obtained was optimal for detection of MPH effects in this specific population. Some data were acquired in a paradigm with conditions with and without incentives. On the grounds of consistency over studies, we used the without-incentives condition to measure the effect of MPH alone. For a more detailed description of this selection process, please see online Supplementary Appendix S1.
Calculation of effect sizes and analysis
In the present analysis, effect sizes reflect the difference between MPH and placebo conditions. For each clinical study, standardized mean differences and variances were calculated. When only the standard error of the mean (s.e.m.) was reported, the standard deviation (s.d.) was obtained by multiplying s.e.m. by the square root of the sample size. When only the median and range were reported, we estimated the mean and s.d. (Hozo et al. Reference Hozo, Djulbegovic and Moore2005). We calculated effect sizes based on the Hedges’ g’ index; however, in order to combine results from different research designs, design-specific equations were applied (Morris & DeShon, Reference Morris and DeShon2002; see online Supplementary Appendix S2 for details).
Seven studies presented two tests of the same cognitive construct (Coghill et al. Reference Coghill, Rhodes and Matthews2007; McInnes et al. Reference McInnes, Bedard, Hogg-Johnson and Tannock2007; Bedard & Tannock, Reference Bedard and Tannock2008; Blum et al. Reference Blum, Awad, Clarke and Power2011; Epstein et al. Reference Epstein, Brinkman, Froehlich, Langberg, Narad, Antonini, Shiels, Simon and Altaye2011; Murray et al. Reference Murray, Childress, Giblin, Williamson, Armstrong and Starr2011; Wigal et al. Reference Wigal, Wigal, Schuck, Brams, Williamson, Armstrong and Starr2011; Agay et al. Reference Agay, Yechiam, Carmel and Levkovitz2014). To prevent an undesired increase of the relative weight of these studies, which is induced when including both tests, we aggregated two effects sizes within one study into one aggregated effect size (Borenstein et al. Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2009) and assumed an inter-test correlation of 0.6. To determine the overall effect of MPH on executive functioning, a random-effects meta-regression analysis was executed, weighting effect sizes with their s.d., and accounting for between-study variation. Heterogeneity between studies was determined with the Q statistic (Lipsey & Wilson, Reference Lipsey and Wilson2001). The random-effects meta-regression was performed with the metafor package (Viechtbauer, Reference Viechtbauer2010). We tested the effects of each moderator separately.
Results
Population and study characteristics
In all, 50 studies with a total number of 1611 participants were included in the analysis (see Table 1 and online Supplementary Appendix S3 for characteristics of the included studies). Mean age ranged from 4.8 to 50.1 years, with a median of 10.8 years. Of the studies, 33 were conducted with pediatric samples (mean age ⩽12 years), five† Footnote 1 with adolescents (mean age 13–18 years), and 12 with adult samples (mean age >18 years). From these 50 studies, 67 data points were obtained, of which 25 were on response inhibition (n = 787), 13 on working memory (n = 559) and 29 on sustained attention (n = 956). The number of times that we had to select the dosage yielding the largest effect size, when multiple dosages were presented in a single study, was comparable between cognitive domains (response inhibition 40%, working memory 38%, and sustained attention 45% of data points, respectively). Most studies that reported a time interval assessed the MPH effect within 60–180 min after ingestion of MPH.
Overall effect of MPH on cognition
In Fig. 2, effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) are presented for all 67 data points, i.e. for response inhibition, working memory and sustained attention. For all data points together, a mean effect size of 0.38 (95% CI 0.27–0.49) was found, which proved significant (p < 0.0001), reflecting a medium and positive overall effect of MPH on executive functioning, with non-significant heterogeneity between data points [Q = 32.51, degrees of freedom (df) = 66, p > 0.99].
Effect of MPH on response inhibition, working memory and sustained attention
The mean effect sizes of 0.40 for response inhibition (95% CI 0.22–0.58) and 0.42 for sustained attention (95% CI 0.26–0.59) were significant (both p < 0.0001). The mean effect size of 0.24 for working memory (95% CI 0.00–0.48) failed to reach significance (p = 0.053). Mean effect sizes did not differ significantly when compared with each other (sustained attention v. response inhibition β = 0.022, p = 0.86; response inhibition v. working memory β = 0.160, p = 0.230; sustained attention v. working memory β = 0.182, p = 0.23). For each function separately, no significant heterogeneity was observed (response inhibition Q = 12.87, df = 24, p = 0.98; working memory Q = 6.11, df = 12, p = 0.91; sustained attention Q = 11.96, df = 28, p > 0.99).
Age-related effects
We centered the predictor variable around the adolescent age of 14 years, the age around which total brain volume peaks in males (Giedd, Reference Giedd2004). Age-related effects are depicted in Fig. 3. Overall, we found no support for a linear (β = −0.002, p = 0.65) association between age and MPH effect; the quadratic predictor was also not significant (β = −0.0002, p = 0.55)Footnote 2 . Visual inspection of scatter plots for cognitive functions separately only suggested a relationship between age and the effect on working memory. However, for working memory, neither a model with a linear predictor (β = 0.02, p = 0.16), nor a model with a quadratic predictor (β = 0.002, p = 0.14) was significant. We also tested the age-relationship for response inhibition and sustained attention separately. No significant linear, quadratic, or combined linear and quadratic relationships were observed.
Exploratory moderator analysis: medication naivety, dosage, MPH formulation, and interactions with age
As we explored three moderators, we corrected for multiple testing with a Bonferroni correction, p values therefore are interpreted as significant if they are below 0.05/3 = 0.017. The relationship between medication naivety and MPH effects was assessed with IR single-dose studies (instead of longer treatment regimens) in which the population was either described as 100% naive (k = 7) or as 0% naive (k = 8). Naive and non-naive studies were equally represented by cognitive domains. The mean effect size of studies with a treated population (effect size = 0.47, 95% CI 0.15–0.80) was significant, whereas the effect of MPH on studies with stimulant-naive participants was not significant (effect size = 0.28, 95% CI −0.06 to 0.63). However, medication naivety was not a significant moderator (β = 0.19, p = 0.44).
As mentioned, we selected the dosage yielding the largest effect size in approximately 40% of data points. Consequently, mean dosages of studies with MPH IR ranged between 0.21 and 0.60 mg/kg, with a median dosage of 0.50 mg/kg in studies of sustained attention and working memory and of 0.30 mg/kg in studies of response inhibition. No linear effect of dosage was identified (β = 0.49, p = 0.39)Footnote 3 . When centering the predictor variable dosage around 0.6 mg/kg (see Tannock et al. Reference Tannock, Schachar and Logan1995), a quadratic model yielded no significant results (β = −0.67, p = 0.65)Footnote 4 . We inspected the dose–response relationship for each function separately. Visual inspection of the scatter plots suggested a dose–response pattern for working memory only. However, the working memory analysis comprised only six studies, and, as for response inhibition and sustained attention, both the linear and (centered) quadratic dose–response relationships were non-significant (β = −0.11, p = 0.94 and β = −0.31, p = 0.95, respectively)Footnote 5 .
All studies reported which type of MPH formulation was tested. MPH formulation was not associated with the effect on response inhibition (β = 0.18, p = 0.38), working memory (β = 0.194, p = 0.59) or sustained attention (β = 0.06, p = 0.78). Linear interactions between age and medication naivety, age and dosage, and age and MPH formulation were all non-significant (β = 0.04, p = 0.16; β = 0.05, p = 0.58; β = 0.005, p = 0.75, respectively).
Publication bias
Regression tests for funnel plot asymmetry were not significant (overall z = 0.40, p = 0.69; response inhibition z = 0.21, p = 0.83; working memory z = 1.19, p = 0.23; sustained attention z = −0.59, p = 0.55), indicating that no publication bias was present. Duval and Tweedie's trim-and-fill method (Duval & Tweedie, Reference Duval and Tweedie2000) demonstrated six hypothetically missing studies on the right side of the overall funnel plot (see Fig. 4). Inclusion of these hypothetical studies would increase the overall mean effect size from 0.38 to 0.42 (95% CI 0.32–0.53). Applying the trim-and-fill method to the sustained attention and working memory data yielded one and three hypothetically missing studies, respectively, and none for response inhibition. Inclusion of hypothetical studies would increase the mean effect size for sustained attention from 0.42 to 0.44 (95% CI 0.28–0.60) and for working memory from 0.24 to 0.32 (95% CI 0.09–0.54), which indicates a potential negative effects bias. Robustness of the significant effects was demonstrated with Rosenthal's fail-safe n calculation (Rosenthal, Reference Rosenthal1979), showing a high number of null findings needed to nullify the effects (overall n = 1091, p < 0.0001; response inhibition n = 153, p < 0.0001; sustained attention n = 235, p < 0.0001).
Conclusions
The major goal of the current study was to test whether the effects of MPH on executive functioning are age-dependent. The present meta-analysis shows moderate and consistent effects of MPH on overall test performance in individuals with ADHD, despite the wide age range of the studied population and diversity in neuropsychological tests, dependent variables and medication protocols. However, no age-dependency was observed when analysing response inhibition, working memory and sustained attention separately. Thus, MPH improves executive functioning, irrespective of age.
The first main finding, regarding the effect of MPH on executive functioning, is that the mean effect size of working memory studies was small and, although the magnitude of the MPH effect did not differ significantly between cognitive domains, failed to reach statistical significance when tested separately. This indicates that working memory is the least sensitive to MPH effects of the executive functions we studied. Interestingly, one out of two asymmetry tests suggested an underestimation of the MPH effect on working memory, although this should be interpreted with care as studies on working memory were scarce. Nonetheless, the finding that inhibition and attention, but not working memory, are enhanced by MPH is in line with a lack of MPH-induced normalization in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) during working memory tasks, and with the MPH-induced normalization of activation in the inferior frontal cortex (IFC) during inhibition and timing tasks (Rubia et al. Reference Rubia, Alegria, Cubillo, Smith, Brammer and Radua2014). It is also in line with a study revealing normalization of DLPFC underactivation by atomoxetine, a NA reuptake inhibitor, but not by MPH (Cubillo et al. Reference Cubillo, Smith, Barrett, Giampietro, Brammer, Simmons and Rubia2014 Reference Cubillo, Smith, Barrett, Giampietro, Brammer, Simmons and Rubiab ). As part of the ventral attention system, the IFC plays a crucial role in attention and cognitive control. Thus, MPH seems to improve attentional and inhibitory control by increasing IFC function, but not working memory organization by increasing DLPFC function. Furthermore, the observed effect sizes for inhibition and working memory across the lifespan are in line with the reported effect sizes in a recent meta-analysis comprising pediatric studies only (Coghill et al. Reference Coghill, Seth, Pedroso, Usala, Currie and Gagliano2013; 0.42 and 0.24, respectively), even though we included in the current meta-analysis also adult studies and a series of additional pediatric studies (an increase of nine inhibition studies and six working memory studies), and incorporated design-specific effect sizes as a methodological improvement. Moreover, our focus on omissions in sustained attention yielded a similar effect size as reported for reaction time variability in the Coghill meta-analysis. The fact that two meta-analyses with different statistical approaches and inclusion criteria reached similar conclusions increases the validity of the conclusions drawn.
The second main finding is that, when focusing on the age-dependency findings, we did not observe a linear or quadratic relationship between age and the effect of MPH on overall executive functioning, nor on the specific executive functions. Hence, in humans, the cognitive response to MPH did not seem to depend on age. While some human studies have suggested an age-dependency of MPH effects when comparing young children with older children and adolescents (Hanisch et al. Reference Hanisch, Konrad, Günther and Herpertz-Dahlmann2004; Faraone & Buitelaar, Reference Faraone and Buitelaar2010; Chamberlain et al. Reference Chamberlain, Robbins, Winder-Rhodes, Müller, Sahakian, Blackwell and Barnett2011), the age-dependency across the life span (i.e. including adulthood) is more apparent from animal studies (Andersen, Reference Andersen2005) and has been hardly studied in humans. The translation from animal research – often with equivocal conclusions – to human findings remains complex, as many differences between these types of research exist. For example, not all animal studies used an ADHD model such as the SHR, the administration method can be oral, intravenous or intraperitoneal, and drug dosages are not directly translatable to those used in humans (Kuczenski & Segal, Reference Kuczenski and Segal2002). Our hypotheses were informed by human studies, but also on animal studies assessing the locomotor response to stimulants. However, this locomotor response is considered to be more representative of reward sensitivity and addiction than of executive function. Given the scarcity of studies on the effect of MPH on reward, as well as on timing, we did not include these domains in the present analysis. Still, given the relevance of these domains in ADHD, it would be pertinent to run meta-regression analyses as soon as a sufficient number of MPH trials focusing on these cognitive domains have been conducted, to determine whether the effects of MPH on reward and timing are age-related.
With respect to the age-dependency results, it is important to note that especially the number of adult working memory studies was low. Put differently, the paucity of adult studies focusing on working memory is hampering the interpretation of the lack of an age-dependency of the effects of MPH on this specific cognitive domain. It is therefore that we also included all three cognitive domains in a single analysis, to determine a general age-dependency of cognitive effects of MPH. However, a general age-dependency was absent. Moreover, given that exposure to stimulants at a young age has been described to decrease sensitivity to stimulants, while exposure at an adult age increases sensitivity to stimulants in animals (Andersen, Reference Andersen2005), prior stimulant use may affect MPH response in humans. Therefore, one could argue that in the present study a potential age-relationship was masked by prior stimulant use. Since most studies do not report all factors potentially affecting the relationship between prior stimulant use and response to MPH (such as the onset and discontinuation of prior treatment), the exact role of prior stimulant use in our findings could not be determined. However, we did compare studies with either a fully treated or a fully naive sample in order to explore the relationship between prior stimulant use and the cognitive effects of MPH. Interestingly, the results of these analyses suggests that stimulant naivety was not a significant moderator of MPH effects and no interaction was present between age and medication naivety. Yet, the effect of MPH in fully treated samples was moderate and significant, whereas it was small and non-significant in stimulant-naive samples. Although, due to the scarcity of adult studies, these exploratory findings predominantly apply to the pediatric population, the pattern of findings does not suggest a differential effect of MPH on executive functioning across the lifespan; however, future research is warranted to determine the exact role of prior medication use.
As mentioned, the main goal of our analysis was to determine the age-dependency of the effect of MPH, if any, on executive functions. Therefore, we selected the dosage yielding the largest effect from studies reporting results of multiple dosages. This resulted in a mean dosage of 0.5 mg/kg for sustained attention and working memory and a slightly lower dosage of 0.3 mg/kg for response inhibition. While selecting the optimal effect is likely to induce a bias towards positive effects, which might result in an overestimate of effect sizes, the selected dosages are in line with the optimal effects in studies reporting linear dose–response relationships for working memory and attention, and an inverted U-shaped dose–response relationship for inhibition (Tannock et al. Reference Tannock, Schachar and Logan1995; Konrad et al. Reference Konrad, Günther, Hanisch and Herpertz-Dahlmann2004, Reference Konrad, Günther, Heinzel-Gutenbrunner and Herpertz-Dahlmann2005). Our exploratory analysis, however, did not reveal a significant association between effect size and dosage for any of the executive functions. This does not imply a general absence of a dose–response relationship, but implies that the optimal dose across studies induces comparable effect sizes.
In conclusion, while replicating the general effect of MPH on cognition, the present study shows no age-dependency of MPH effects on overall executive function, response inhibition, working memory and sustained attention. The major challenge for the future is to further unravel the relationship between the onset and duration of stimulant exposure and the cognitive sensitivity to MPH in humans, as there is a lack of knowledge on this subject. This could be done by including stimulant-naive participants in future studies. In addition, more studies with adolescent populations are needed to clarify the cognitive effects of MPH during this highly important developmental period. Moreover, it is of interest to determine how these cognitive effects relate to behavioral improvement (i.e. ADHD symptomatology), which is the primary target of MPH treatment. Some MPH studies, mostly with small samples, suggest minimal association between these two (Konrad et al. Reference Konrad, Günther, Hanisch and Herpertz-Dahlmann2004; Loo et al. Reference Loo, Hopfer, Teale and Reite2004; McInnes et al. Reference McInnes, Bedard, Hogg-Johnson and Tannock2007; Biederman et al. Reference Biederman, Mick, Fried, Wilner, Spencer and Faraone2011), which is in line with the notion that cognitive (performance-based) measures and clinical rating scales in ADHD seem to tap different aspects of daily functioning (Toplak et al. Reference Toplak, West and Stanovich2013). Since cognitive dysfunction in ADHD is apparent in many individuals with ADHD, and predicts clinical response to MPH (Scheres et al. Reference Scheres, Oosterlaan and Sergeant2006; Coghill et al. Reference Coghill, Rhodes and Matthews2007; van der Oord et al. Reference Van der Oord, Geurts, Prins, Emmelkamp and Oosterlaan2012), additional work is needed to clarify the role of cognitive dysfunction in clinical functioning in order to further determine the clinical relevance of cognitive enhancement by MPH. Hence, better insight in the neurocognitive effects of MPH will, hopefully, ultimately result in improved ADHD treatment across the lifespan.
Supplementary material
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716000350
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank research assistants Milou Jacobs and Linda Olde Dubbeling for their support with the literature search and checking of the data. We are grateful to Dr Nirit Agay, Dr Annet Bron, Dr Russel Barkley, Dr Jeff Epstein, Dr Thomas Günther, Dr Leon Kenemans, Dr Rafael Klorman and Dr Mitul Mehta for providing additional data for the study.
This project was funded by Fonds Nuts Ohra (grant number 1002-40). Work by H.M.G. and H.M.H. was, in part, supported by The Innovational Research Incentives Scheme VIDI (H.M.G., grant number 452-10-003) and VICI (H.M.H., grant number 453-12-005) grants by The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). The funding sources were not involved in the content of this article.
Declaration of Interest
None.