Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T02:20:41.476Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neural code: Another breach in the wall?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2019

Chloé Huetz
Affiliation:
Institute of Neuroscience, NeuroPSI, UMR CNRS 9197, Université Paris-Sud, 91405Orsay, France. chloe.huetz@u-psud.frsamira.souffi@u-psud.frvictor.adenis@u-psud.frjean-marc.edeline@u-psud.frhttp://neuro-psi.cnrs.fr/
Samira Souffi
Affiliation:
Institute of Neuroscience, NeuroPSI, UMR CNRS 9197, Université Paris-Sud, 91405Orsay, France. chloe.huetz@u-psud.frsamira.souffi@u-psud.frvictor.adenis@u-psud.frjean-marc.edeline@u-psud.frhttp://neuro-psi.cnrs.fr/
Victor Adenis
Affiliation:
Institute of Neuroscience, NeuroPSI, UMR CNRS 9197, Université Paris-Sud, 91405Orsay, France. chloe.huetz@u-psud.frsamira.souffi@u-psud.frvictor.adenis@u-psud.frjean-marc.edeline@u-psud.frhttp://neuro-psi.cnrs.fr/
Jean-Marc Edeline
Affiliation:
Institute of Neuroscience, NeuroPSI, UMR CNRS 9197, Université Paris-Sud, 91405Orsay, France. chloe.huetz@u-psud.frsamira.souffi@u-psud.frvictor.adenis@u-psud.frjean-marc.edeline@u-psud.frhttp://neuro-psi.cnrs.fr/

Abstract

Brette presents arguments that query the existence of the neural code. However, he has neglected certain evidence that could be viewed as proof that a neural code operates in the brain. Albeit these proofs show a link between neural activity and cognition, we discuss why they fail to demonstrate the existence of an invariant neural code.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

By questioning the existence of the neural code, Romain Brette opens again a strong debate between representational views of the brain (cognitivism and computationalism) and sensorimotor/enaction theories (O'Regan and Noë Reference O'Regan and Noë2001; Varela et al. Reference Varela, Thompson and Rosch1991), his preference being the latter. According to his view, all cognitive functions, particularly action and perception, are viewed as means to interact with the world, without the need to build internal representations of it. Neural activity during perception should be viewed as the result of the organism's interaction with the world, taking into account all possible influences, such as its internal state and its actions resulting in a given percept. Therefore, as the brain does not manipulate representations, it is senseless to try to decipher any code supposed to encrypt representations in neural activity. The results of three research fields focusing on proving that a particular neural code is at play should be addressed by Brette's review to strengthen his point.

First, in sensory physiology, research on tuning curves has been extended to naturalistic stimuli and is divided into two complementary approaches: encoding and decoding. Based on models of the stimulus-response function, these approaches rely on the idea that neural activity encodes some features of the external world. Successful reconstructions of complex stimuli based on neural responses (decoding), or successful predictions of responses to new stimuli (encoding) are viewed as proofs that the neural code has been cracked. Interpreting these results in the light of Brette's arguments seems necessary. Initially, the stimulus reconstruction method (decoding) was performed either with simple artificial stimuli (Bialek et al. Reference Bialek, Rieke, de Ruyter van Steveninck and Warland1991) or in peripheral sensory systems (Rieke et al. Reference Rieke, Bodnar and Bialek1995; Warland et al. Reference Warland, Reinagel and Meister1997). More recently, studies have reconstructed natural stimuli from cortical responses (Akbari et al. Reference Akbari, Khalighinejad, Herrero, Mehta and Mesgarani2019; Miyawaki et al. Reference Miyawaki, Uchida, Yamashita, Sato, Morito, Tanabe, Sadato and Kamitani2008; Naselaris et al. Reference Naselaris, Prenger, Kay, Oliver and Gallant2009), opening the spectacular expectation to read subjects’ percepts. In the auditory modality, encoding models were used to investigate neural selectivity to a variety of acoustic properties such as phonetic features (Mesgarani et al. Reference Mesgarani, Cheung, Johnson and Chang2014), pitch (Oxenham Reference Oxenham2018), and timbre and rhythm (Woolley et al. Reference Woolley, Gill, Fremouw and Theunissen2009). To achieve good performance, the stimulus/response models used in decoding/encoding approaches rely on features such as trial averaging, statistics of natural stimuli, and starting time of the stimulus. Thus, the right interpretation should be that an “ideal observer” with a priori knowledge of the experimental design can infer the stimulus (in the decoding approach) or the neural response (in the encoding approach). Noteworthy, this field has led to an interesting drift from the idea of a fixed relationship between stimulus and neural responses to a more dynamic model, and is now tackling the mechanisms by which sensory responses are modulated by learning, context, and history (Fritz et al. Reference Fritz, Elhilali and Shamma2005; Holdgraf et al. Reference Holdgraf, de Heer, Pasley, Rieger, Crone, Lin, Knight and Theunissen2016; Williamson et al. Reference Williamson, Ahrens, Linden and Sahani2016).

Second, the field of neuroprosthetic devices offers demonstrations of causal links between neural code and brain functions. The most successful of these devices, the cochlear implant (CI), operates with blunt stimulations of auditory nerve terminals. Despite a large current spread in the tympanic ramp, the CI allows implanted subjects to have percepts and recover speech understanding. Even though there are huge differences between the normal cochlea and the CI, the fact that CIs restore hearing can be viewed as a proof that the neural code at play in the periphery has been deciphered and is successfully implemented in a prosthetic device. However, the CI settings that lead to speech comprehension differ considerably from one subject to another, as do the strategies leading to the largest evoked responses in auditory cortex (Adenis et al. Reference Adenis, Gourévitch, Mamelle, Recugnat, Stahl, Gnansia, Nguyen and Edeline2018). Thus, in contrast to the genetic code that is invariant across cells and species, the neural code (understood as changes in neural activity in adaption to a CI) is probably specific for each individual and/or each type of neuron. In line with sensorimotor theories, the success of CIs shows that the brain is using a new input in a way it can interact again with the environment, which might be the basis of hearing restoration.

A third important field investigates the effect of disrupting a particular feature of neural activity on a cognitive skill. In the visual system, disruption of physiological activity in the primate middle temporal area during presentation of moving stimuli biases the perceptive judgment of a behaving animal (Salzman & Newsome Reference Salzman and Newsome1994; Salzman et al. Reference Salzman, Britten and Newsome1990), thus making the first link between neural code (understood as a pattern of activity of specific neurons) and behavioral performance. More recently, studies performed in the hippocampus have found that disrupting the replay of spiking patterns occurring across neuronal ensembles during the sharp wave ripples profoundly alters the memory of previously acquired information (Ego-Stengel & Wilson Reference Ego-Stengel and Wilson2010; Girardeau et al. Reference Girardeau, Benchenane, Wiener, Buzsáki and Zugaro2009). These data reinforce the notion that neuronal activity patterns do correlate with the acquired information. More importantly, associating a rewarding stimulation of the medial forebrain bundle with a hippocampal place cell activity induced a place preference at the place cell location (de Lavilléon et al. Reference de Lavilléon, Lacroix, Rondi-Reig and Benchenane2015), demonstrating causal links between a particular place cell's firing rate and a specific location memory. In all these examples, the exact neural activity feature (its firing rate or its temporal spike patterns) correlated with the animal's location is unknown, but causal relationships do exist. Yet, causality is not enough to define a neural code.

Clearly, more caution is necessary when discussing the neural code as overstatements made (Ferster & Spruston Reference Ferster and Spruston1995; Panzeri et al. Reference Panzeri, Harvey, Piasini, Latham and Fellin2017) tend to generate the illusions that (1) the same code operates in any sensory and motor system, which is obviously not the case; and (2) the brain's cognitive functions consist of manipulating encoded representations of the world, a theory that is controversial. Does this mean that the concept of neural code should be abandoned or should be used to describe studies linking neural activity to brain function? We believe that the neural code definition should be freed from the notion of representation, and we should clarify what we refer to when investigating the neural mechanism of brain functions.

References

Adenis, V., Gourévitch, B., Mamelle, E., Recugnat, M., Stahl, P., Gnansia, D., Nguyen, Y. & Edeline, J. M. (2018) ECAP growth function to increasing pulse amplitude or pulse duration demonstrates large inter-animal variability that is reflected in auditory cortex of the guinea pig. PLoS One. 13(8):e0201771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akbari, H., Khalighinejad, B., Herrero, J. L., Mehta, A. D. & Mesgarani, N. (2019) Towards reconstructing intelligible speech from the human auditory cortex. Scientific Reports 29;9(1):874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialek, W., Rieke, F., de Ruyter van Steveninck, R. R. & Warland, D. (1991) Reading a neural code. Science 252:1854–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Lavilléon, G., Lacroix, M. M., Rondi-Reig, L. & Benchenane, K. (2015) Explicit memory creation during sleep demonstrates a causal role of place cells in navigation. Nature Neuroscience 18(4):493–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ego-Stengel, V. & Wilson, M. A. (2010) Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat. Hippocampus 20(1):110.Google ScholarPubMed
Ferster, D., & Spruston, N. (1995) Cracking the neuronal code. Science 270(5237):756–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fritz, J, Elhilali, M, Shamma, S. (2005) Active listening: Task-dependent plasticity of spectrotemporal receptive fields in primary auditory cortex. Hearing Research 206(1/2):159–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girardeau, G., Benchenane, K., Wiener, S. I., Buzsáki, G. & Zugaro, M. B. (2009) Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nature Neuroscience 12(10):1222–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holdgraf, C. R., de Heer, W., Pasley, B., Rieger, J., Crone, N., Lin, J. J., Knight, R.T., & Theunissen, F. E. (2016) Rapid tuning shifts in human auditory cortex enhance speech intelligibility. Nature Communications 7:13654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesgarani, N., Cheung, C., Johnson, K. & Chang, E. F. (2014) Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science 343(6174):1006–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyawaki, Y., Uchida, H., Yamashita, O., Sato, M. A., Morito, Y., Tanabe, H. C., Sadato, N. & Kamitani, Y. (2008) Visual image reconstruction from human brain activity using a combination of multiscale local image decoders. Neuron 60(5):915–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naselaris, T., Prenger, R. J., Kay, K. N., Oliver, M. & Gallant, J. L. (2009) Bayesian reconstruction of natural images from human brain activity. Neuron 63(6):902–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Regan, J. K. & Noë, A. (2001) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(5):939–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oxenham, A.J. (2018) How we hear: The perception and neural coding of sound. Annual Review of Psychology 69:2750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panzeri, S., Harvey, C. D., Piasini, E., Latham, P. E., Fellin, T. (2017) Cracking the neural code for sensory perception by combining statistics, intervention, and behavior. Neuron 93(3):491507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rieke, F., Bodnar, D. A. & Bialek, W. (1995) Naturalistic stimuli increase the rate and efficiency of information transmission by primary auditory afferents. Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 262(1365):259–65.Google ScholarPubMed
Salzman, C. D., Britten, K. H. & Newsome, W. T. (1990) Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction. Nature 346(6280):174–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salzman, C. D. & Newsome, W. T. (1994) Neural mechanisms for forming a perceptual decision. Science 264(5156):231–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E. & Rosch, E. (1991) The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warland, D. K., Reinagel, P. & Meister, M. (1997) Decoding visual information from a population of retinal ganglion cells. Journal of Neurophysiology 78(5):2336–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamson, R. S., Ahrens, M. B., Linden, J. F. & Sahani, M. (2016) Input-specific gain modulation by local sensory context shapes cortical and thalamic responses to complex sounds. Neuron 91(2):467–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woolley, S. M., Gill, P. R., Fremouw, T. & Theunissen, F. E. (2009) Functional groups in the avian auditory system. Journal of Neuroscience 29(9):2780–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed