Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T19:06:19.547Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2012

Corey L. Fincher
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131. fincher@unm.eduhttp://biology.unm.edu/fincher
Randy Thornhill
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131rthorn@unm.eduhttp://biology.unm.edu/Thornhill/rthorn.htm
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Throughout the world people differ in the magnitude with which they value strong family ties or heightened religiosity. We propose that this cross-cultural variation is a result of a contingent psychological adaptation that facilitates in-group assortative sociality in the face of high levels of parasite-stress while devaluing in-group assortative sociality in areas with low levels of parasite-stress. This is because in-group assortative sociality is more important for the avoidance of infection from novel parasites and for the management of infection in regions with high levels of parasite-stress compared with regions of low infectious disease stress. We examined this hypothesis by testing the predictions that there would be a positive association between parasite-stress and strength of family ties or religiosity. We conducted this study by comparing among nations and among states in the United States of America. We found for both the international and the interstate analyses that in-group assortative sociality was positively associated with parasite-stress. This was true when controlling for potentially confounding factors such as human freedom and economic development. The findings support the parasite-stress theory of sociality, that is, the proposal that parasite-stress is central to the evolution of social life in humans and other animals.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

1. Introduction

Across the world people vary in the magnitude with which they value strong family ties and extended families (Alesina & Giuliano Reference Alesina and Giuliano2007; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Bhawuk, Nishii, Bechtold, House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004); and people adopt religion and exhibit religious commitment to different degrees across the world in patterns strikingly similar to those of family ties (McCleary & Barro Reference McCleary and Barro2006; Norris & Inglehart Reference Norris and Inglehart2004). In this target article, we argue that the reason some people devalue family ties or eschew religion while others prioritize and embed themselves in family relationships and religion, rests on a central phenomenon of social life called in-group assortative sociality. Such sociality refers to the preferential association among similar individuals who compose an in-group versus out-group or dissimilar others. Phenotypic features such as dress and formal costumes, tattooing and scarification, culinary preference, language and dialect, religion and other belief systems, normative behavior, social displays, rituals, and body-scent mark in-group similarity. Assortative sociality's three general social components are (1) limited dispersal for reproduction from the natal locale, (2) in-group favoritism, and (3) out-group dislike and avoidance – in humans, the three are referred to as philopatry, ethnocentrism, and xenophobia, respectively (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008a; Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008b). Recent theory – the parasite-stress theory of sociality – and its empirical testing tie the patterns of these three phenomena fundamentally to varying levels of parasite-stress experienced by people, both within a region and across geographic space. We first review this research linking in-group assortative sociality to parasite-stress and then expand it, conceptually and empirically, to include family ties and religious affiliation and commitment. We conduct our analyses both cross-nationally and within a single polity, the United States of America.

2. The parasite-stress theory of sociality

2.1. Foundations

Established knowledge of the ecology and evolution of parasitic disease (=infectious disease=pathogenic disease) provides a foundation for the parasite-stress theory of sociality. Infectious diseases were a major source of morbidity and mortality, and hence of natural selection, in human evolutionary history (Anderson & May Reference Anderson and May1991; Dobson & Carper Reference Dobson and Carper1996; Ewald Reference Ewald1994; McNeill Reference McNeill1998; Wolfe et al. Reference Wolfe, Dunavan and Diamond2007). Human adaptations that defend against parasites comprise the biochemical, cellular, and tissue-based classical immune system, as well as the behavioral immune system that includes (a) anti-parasite psychology and behavior (Schaller & Duncan Reference Schaller, Duncan, Forges, Haselton and Von Hippel2007), and (b) psychology and behavior that manages infectious diseases when they occur. The behavioral immune system is comprised of ancestrally adaptive feelings, attitudes, and values about and behaviors toward out-group and in-group members, caution about or unwillingness to interact with out-group people, and prejudice against people perceived as unhealthy, contaminated, or unclean (Curtis Reference Curtis2007; Curtis et al. Reference Curtis, Aunger and Rabie2004; Faulkner et al. Reference Faulkner, Schaller, Park and Duncan2004; Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Navarrete & Fessler Reference Navarrete and Fessler2006; Oaten et al. Reference Oaten, Stevenson and Case2009; Park et al. Reference Park, Faulkner and Schaller2003; Reference Park, Schaller and Crandall2007; Schaller & Duncan Reference Schaller, Duncan, Forges, Haselton and Von Hippel2007; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010). The behavioral immune system also includes the same types of bias against contact with nonhuman animals that pose human infectious disease threats (Prokop et al. Reference Prokop, Usak and Fancovicová2010a; Reference Prokop, Usak and Fancovicová2010b).

Hosts and their parasites coevolve in antagonistic and perpetual races with adaptation, counter-adaptation, and counter-counter-adaptation for both hosts and parasites (Ewald Reference Ewald1994; Haldane Reference Haldane1949; Ridley Reference Ridley1993; Thompson Reference Thompson2005; Tooby Reference Tooby1982; Van Valen Reference Van Valen1973). In the human case, this dynamic, enduring, antagonistic interaction is illustrated by the observation that, despite the huge somatic allocation made to the immune system, people still get sick and even small reductions in immunocompetence increase vulnerability to infectious disease.

Furthermore, host-parasite races are geographically localized across the range of a host species, creating a coevolutionary mosaic that involves genetic and phenotypic differences in host immune adaptation and corresponding parasite counter-adaptation across a host's range (Thompson Reference Thompson2005). An important outcome of this is that host defense works most effectively against the local parasite species, strains, or genotypes, and not against those evolving in nearby host groups. Hence, out-groups may often harbor novel parasites that cannot be defended against by an individual or his or her immunologically similar in-group members (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008a; Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008b). Out-group individuals pose the additional infectious-disease threat of lacking knowledge of and therefore violating local customs or norms, many of which, like hygiene and methods of food preparation, may prevent infection from local parasites (Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Schaller & Neuberg Reference Schaller, Neuberg, Crawford and Krebs2008). Norms of many types – culinary, linguistic, moral, sexual, nepotistic, religious, dress-related, and so on – are used by people both to portray in-group affiliation and associated values and to distinguish in-group from out-group members. Norm differences between groups are often the basis of intergroup prejudice and hostility (i.e., xenophobia). Likewise, norm similarity is the basis of positive valuation and altruism among people (Norenzayan & Shariff Reference Norenzayan and Shariff2008; Park & Schaller Reference Park and Schaller2005).

Evidence for geographically localized host–parasite coevolutionary races is convincing. On the parasite side of the race, parasite geographical mosaics were found, for example, in recent research on the important human parasite Leishmania braziliensis. Rougeron et al. (Reference Rougeron, De Meeus, Hide, Waleckx, Bermudez, Arevalo, Llanos-Cuentas, Dujardin, De Doncker, Le Ray, Ayala and Banuls2009) described the high genetic diversity and subdivided population structure of this parasite across both Peru and Bolivia. They found high levels of microgeographic variation identifiable by at least 124 highly localized, physiologically and genetically distinct strains. The strains showed strong evidence of high degrees of close inbreeding and thus resembled genetic clones. This extremely fine-grained geographic mosaic in L. braziliensis implies a similar microgeographic population and genetic mosaic in human hosts. This type of spatial variation in host adaptation against local parasites, or, said differently, in host immune maladaptation against out-group-typical parasites, is a general pattern in the animal and plant infectious disease literature (e.g., Corby-Harris & Promislow Reference Corby-Harris and Promislow2008; Dionne et al. Reference Dionne, Miller, Dodson, Caron and Bernatchez2007; Kaltz et al. Reference Kaltz, Gandon, Michalakis and Shykoff1999; Thompson Reference Thompson2005; Tinsley et al. Reference Tinsley, Blanford and Jiggins2006). Human cases showing this include the caste-specific infectious diseases and corresponding specific immunity among sympatric Indian castes (Pitchappan Reference Pitchappan2002). Indeed, McNeill (Reference McNeill1998) and Mattausch (in press) have suggested that the castes of India formed in part from differential cultural responses to parasite-stress. Another case is found in the village-specific immune defenses against leishmania parasites in adjacent Sudanese villages (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Fadl, Mohamed, Elzein, Jamieson, Cordell, Peacock, Fakiola, Madhuri, Eltahir, Elhassan, Musa, Muntaser and Blackwell2007). In particular cases the localization is so fine-grained that human hosts inbreed, risking the potential costs of inbreeding depression, in order to maintain coadapted gene complexes important for their offsprings' defense against local parasite infection, as Denic and colleagues have found for malaria (Denic & Nicholls Reference Denic and Nicholls2007; Denic et al. Reference Denic, Nagelkerke and Agarwal2008a; Reference Denic, Nagelkerke and Agarwal2008b; also Hoben et al. Reference Hoben, Buunk, Fincher, Thornhill and Schaller2010).

Further evidence of localized immunity derives from events where humans from isolated groups interact with novel groups by conquest or trade and infectious disease transmission ensues, sometimes with drastic effects. This has occurred after intra- and inter-continental movement of individuals brought about inter-group contact (Diamond Reference Diamond1998; Dubos Reference Dubos and Dubos1980; Good Reference Good1972; Jenkins et al. Reference Jenkins, Dimitrakakis, Cook, Sanders and Stallman1989; McNeill Reference McNeill1998). Other human examples of localized immunity are reviewed in Fincher and Thornhill (Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008a) and Tibayrenc (Reference Tibayrenc and Tibayrenc2007). Final evidence for local host adaptation to parasites is found in the literature showing that the hybridization between adjacent, closely related conspecific populations results in hybrid offspring with reduced immunocompetence (e.g., house mice: Sage et al. Reference Sage, Heyneman, Lim and Wilson1986; cottonwood trees: Floate et al. Reference Floate, Kearsley and Whitham1993; also see Thompson Reference Thompson2005 for other examples).

Because of localized host immune adaptation in an ecological setting of high disease stress, xenophobia, ethnocentrism, and reduced dispersal are adaptive preferences/values and behaviors for avoidance of novel parasites contained in out-groups and the management of local infectious disease. Philopatry – the absence or low level of dispersal away from a natal area for reproduction – reduces contact with out-groups and their habitats that may contain new parasites. Likewise, xenophobia – the avoidance and dislike of out-group members – discourages contact with out-groups and their likely different parasites. Ethnocentrism – in-group favoritism and embeddedness entailing nepotism toward both nuclear and extended family, as well as altruism toward unrelated, yet immunologically similar, in-group members – focuses prosociality among in-group members, and fosters the supportive social networks for coping with present infections in members of the in-group. Thus, philopatry, xenophobia, and ethnocentrism – elements of in-group assortative sociality – are predicted to be strongly held values in areas of high parasite-stress (also see Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009).

Parasite-stress is not the same across the globe nor has it been the same across time. Humans have experienced parasite gradients throughout history and continue to do so today (Crawford Reference Crawford2007; Dobson & Carper Reference Dobson and Carper1996; Guernier et al. Reference Guernier, Hochberg and Guégan2004; Lopez et al. Reference Lopez, Mathers, Ezzati, Jamison and Murray2006; Low Reference Low1990; McNeill Reference McNeill, Stanley and Joske1980; Reference McNeill1998; Smith et al. Reference Smith, Sax, Gaines, Guernier and Guégan2007; Wolfe et al. Reference Wolfe, Dunavan and Diamond2007). Hence, we expect that the benefits and costs of in-group assortative sociality will shift along the parasite-stress gradient such that in some areas (of high parasite-stress) high levels of in-group assortative sociality will be more beneficial than in other areas (of low parasite-stress). As parasite-stress declines, the infectious-disease contagion risks to individuals from interaction with out-groups decrease. Consequently, for individuals in areas that are relatively low in parasite-stress, out-group contacts and alliances may provide greater benefits than costs. The benefits of out-group interactions include gains through intergroup trade, new and better ideas and technology, and diversified and larger social networks for marriage and other social alliances (Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009).

McElreath et al. (Reference McElreath, Boyd and Richerson2003) and Nettle (Reference Nettle1999) argued that assortative sociality could cause cultural isolation and, hence, cultural divergence and emergent new cultures in the absence of geographic barriers (e.g., unfavorable habitats or unsurpassable mountain ranges) that fractionate a culture's distribution. Building on this, we argued that given the ecological localization of host defenses against parasites, the three components of assortative sociality – limited dispersal, ethnocentrism, and xenophobia – fractionate populations and thereby cause their cultural and evolutionary independence (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008a; Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008b). Therefore, the parasite-stress model includes a theory about the genesis of cultural or ethnic diversity, and some of the predictions related to this aspect of the model have been empirically supported. We have shown that endemic religion diversity (both major religions and ethnoreligions), as well as indigenous language diversity, across contemporary countries worldwide are related strongly and positively to parasite-stress (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008a; Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008b). Also consistent with the ethnogenesis aspect of the parasite-stress model was Cashdan's (Reference Cashdan2001a) finding for traditional peoples in the ethnographic record that high parasite-stress regions have more ethnic groups than low parasite-stress regions.

The parasite-stress theory of sociality posits an adaptive (ancestrally) condition-dependent adoption of in-group and out-group social tactics by individuals dependent on local parasite-stress. This condition-dependent adaptation requires local variation in morbidity and mortality from parasite severity, as the selection that acted historically in favoring contingent assortative-sociality behavioral and psychological adaptations. The evolution of conditional response as an important feature of assortative sociality's design, rather than exclusive fixity of localized genetically distinct adaptations, is consistent with knowledge about infectious diseases. The dynamics of an infectious disease can generate high variation in incidence, prevalence, transmissibility, and pathogenicity of the disease agent across the range of its host species, as well as on a very fine-grained, local scale. Factors affecting this variability at a single locale are temporal changes in host group size, climate and weather, disease-vector abundance and behavior, and the number and dynamics of the different infectious diseases infecting a host (Anderson & May Reference Anderson and May1991; Corby-Harris & Promislow Reference Corby-Harris and Promislow2008; Ewald Reference Ewald1994; Guernier et al. Reference Guernier, Hochberg and Guégan2004; Prugnolle et al. Reference Prugnolle, Manica, Charpentier, Guégan, Guernier and Balloux2005). The dynamic nature of host-parasite coevolution itself creates localized variation across generations in parasite-stress (Hamilton & Zuk Reference Hamilton and Zuk1982).

In-group assortative sociality is an example, we argue, of adaptive phenotypic plasticity, that is, of a conditional strategy with multiple contingent tactics (Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Schaller & Murray Reference Schaller and Murray2008; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009). Such plasticity in traits is favored when phenotypic change allows the individual to modify phenotypic expression in directions that give higher inclusive fitness than that achieved by a single phenotype. Conditional strategies in behavior, psychology, development, and physiology are very common across animal taxa (West-Eberhard Reference West-Eberhard2003). Socially learned or cultural behavior in humans is a category of behavioral and psychological plasticity that evolved, at least in part, as a solution to the evolutionary historical fitness problem of local social complexity and change (Alexander Reference Alexander1979; Flinn Reference Flinn1997; Flinn & Coe Reference Flinn, Coe, Gangestad and Simpson2007). We have argued that a significant part of this complexity and change likely arose from local people's adjustments in in-group and out-group oriented behavior to deal adaptively with temporally varying parasite problems (Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009).

The characterization of human in-group assortative sociality as a contingent, “plastic” phenotype includes cultural transmission via social learning that is conditional on local optima in values. Accordingly, culture is not evoked and transmitted passively, but instead is actively evoked and transmitted by psychological adaptations for enculturation that discriminate cultural elements incorporating the elements that have local utility and discarding elements that do not. We hypothesize that the ontogeny of people includes design by Darwinian selection to choose ideas, ways of thinking, and attitudes – that is, choose values – that correspond to routes of ancestrally adaptive social navigation in their community/local culture. The recognition that individuals choose their values is quite old in the sociological and psychological literature (see Jost et al. Reference Jost, Federico and Napier2009). We add to this traditional sociological view with our proposal that the choices are by evolved design and, specifically in regard to values of assortative sociality, they are guided by psychological adaptations dedicated to the function of value acquisition to meet ecological adversity and demands pertaining to infectious-disease stress (for views compatible with this hypothesis for cultural acquisition, see Alexander Reference Alexander1979; Billing & Sherman Reference Billing and Sherman1998; Boyd & Richerson Reference Boyd and Richerson1985; Gangestad et al. Reference Gangestad, Haselton and Buss2006b; Henrich & Henrich Reference Henrich and Henrich2010; Schaller Reference Schaller2006).

However, we do not restrict application of the parasite-stress theory of sociality only to humans and other cultural animals. Therefore, we include in our article aspects of the behavioral immune system that can be considered even in acultural species.

Furthermore, our emphasis on adaptive contingency in the expression and adoption of human assortative sociality does not imply that we expect no region-specific variation across human societies in genetic adaptation for assortative sociality. Culture-gene coevolution may produce genetically differentiated cross-cultural variation in the values and behaviors of assortative sociality. For example, in areas of high parasite prevalence, cultural practices of philopatry, ethnocentrism, and xenophobia may effectively select for alleles affecting psychological features that promote the learning and use of these practices (Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008). Our argument is that infectious disease problems are locally variable, and hence, significant conditionality will be favored and maintained by selection even in the presence of region-specific genetic adaptation functioning in local adoption and use of values and behaviors. Boyd and Richerson (Reference Boyd and Richerson1985), Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman (Reference Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman1981), and Lumsden and Wilson (Reference Lumsden and Wilson1981) treat culture-gene coevolution in detail. There is evidence that it may play a role in cross-national variation in the value dimension collectivism-individualism (Chiao & Blizinsky Reference Chiao and Blizinsky2010), a dimension related importantly to topics in this article. That genetically distinct adaptations for coping with an ecological problem and condition-dependent adaptation for the same problem domain can co-occur, is well established in the literature of alternative reproductive tactics (see recent review in Oliveira et al. Reference Oliveira, Taborsky and Brockmann2008).

A considerable body of research supports the hypothesis of an evolved contingent assortative sociality in people that functions against infectious disease. For example, Faulkner et al. (Reference Faulkner, Schaller, Park and Duncan2004) and Navarrete and Fessler (Reference Navarrete and Fessler2006) provide evidence, based on numerous and diverse Western samples, that scores among individuals on scales that measure the degree of xenophobia and ethnocentrism correspond to chronic individual differences in perceived vulnerability to infectious disease; those who perceive high disease risk are more xenophobic and ethnocentric than those who perceive low disease risk. This research also shows that xenophobia and ethnocentrism within individuals increases under experimental primes of greater disease salience in the current environment. A related recent study (Schaller et al. Reference Schaller, Miller, Gervais, Yager and Chen2010) reports that research subjects who observed slides of people with disease symptoms (e.g., pox, skin lesions, sneezing) immediately mounted a classical immune response. Their white blood cells produced elevated amounts of inflammatory cytokine-interleukin-6 when exposed to bacterial antigens. This immune response was not seen in research subjects who viewed control slides, including slides depicting a person pointing a gun directly at the viewer. Hence, the immune response was not a general reaction to danger or threat, but was specific to cues of other people with symptoms of parasitic infection (Schaller et al. Reference Schaller, Miller, Gervais, Yager and Chen2010).

Moreover, a recent study by Mortensen et al. (Reference Mortensen, Becker, Ackerman, Neuberg and Kenrick2010) reports that subjects viewing slides with disease-salient cues immediately exhibited greater feelings promoting between-person avoidance (reduced extraversion and openness to experiences) in comparison to these subjects' feelings upon viewing control slides. These researchers also found that subjects with high scores on perceived vulnerability to disease showed greater feelings of interpersonal avoidance than did subjects with low scores on the same scale. Finally, this same paper reports that viewing parasite-salient slides resulted in increased avoidant arm movements when subjects viewed facial photos of strangers, especially for subjects high in perceived vulnerability to disease. In sum, the studies by Schaller et al. (Reference Schaller, Miller, Gervais, Yager and Chen2010) and Mortensen et al. (Reference Mortensen, Becker, Ackerman, Neuberg and Kenrick2010) reveal that visually perceiving cues pertinent to risk of parasitic infection generates an immediate immune response and a change in perceptions of one's own personality and behavioral actions that defend against or avoid infectious people. Hence, such cues markedly activate the classical immune system as well as the behavioral immune system.

In sum, there is considerable evidence of both inter-individual chronic differences as well as within-individual conditionality in xenophobic and ethnocentric values and related personality features and behaviors, and that both the inter-individual consistency and within-individual contingency are caused by infectious-disease problems in the local environment.

Proximate mechanisms by which individuals assess local parasite-stress – and thereby ontogenetically and contingently express the locally adaptive degree of defensive assortative sociality – may include immune system activation (such as, the frequency of infection; Stevenson et al. Reference Stevenson, Case and Oaten2009) and social learning of local disease risks, as well as direct observation of parasite threat (as evidenced in the studies mentioned just above). All of these mechanisms may act in combination and account for both the inter-individual and within-individual variation in values affecting in- and out-group behavioral preferences comprising assortative sociality. The recent research by Stevenson et al. (Reference Stevenson, Case and Oaten2009) reports that people with high contamination sensitivity and disgust sensitivity, which are thought to be emotional defenses against parasitic infection, had fewer recent infectious diseases than people with low sensitivities, providing evidence of a protective function of these emotions against these diseases. These researchers also found that high contamination sensitivity, in particular, was associated positively with a person's history of contracting infectious diseases (but not with recency of infections), implying that an ontogeny and conditionality of repeated activation of the classical immune system may underlie the adoption of the values associated with assortative sociality.

2.2. Cross-cultural evidence for the parasite-stress theory of sociality

We mentioned in section 2.1 that the parasite-stress theory of sociality led to the discovery of global patterns in the diversity of religions and languages. The theory has been tested and supported by additional, recent cross-cultural studies, which we describe next.

The parasite-stress model predicts that philopatry should be positively associated with infectious disease stress. We tested this prediction by examining human societal range size (a measure of dispersal) in relation to parasite-stress for a large sample of traditional societies in the anthropological record (339 societies), and found that range size was smaller where pathogen stress was higher, indicative of adaptive philopatry as defense against parasites (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008b). It is possible that the reduced range size in high pathogen-stress regions is due to the malaise and inactivity of parasitized individuals. However, this apparently is not the case. According to our analysis in Fincher and Thornhill (Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008b), people in traditional societies move more often but over shorter distances in high parasite-stress areas than do people in low parasite-stress areas.

High xenophobia and ethnocentrism correspond to the Western value system referred to as conservatism, whereas low xenophobia (i.e., high xenophilia toward out-groups) and restricted ethnocentrism (i.e., focus on nuclear family) correspond to more liberal values (see Navarrete & Fessler Reference Navarrete and Fessler2006; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009). Furthermore, conservatism–liberalism overlaps considerably with the well-studied cross-cultural value system referred to as “collectivism–individualism” by cross-cultural sociologists and psychologists. Conservatism and collectivism are similar in their heightened xenophobia and ethnocentrism inclusive of the extended family and other in-group members with similar conformist and traditional values, whereas liberalism and individualism are similar in nuclear-family-focused nepotism and relatively high xenophilia (Barnea & Schwartz Reference Barnea and Schwartz1998; Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Bhawuk, Nishii, Bechtold, House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004; Georgas et al. Reference Georgas, Mylonas, Bafiti, Poortinga, Christakopoulou, Kagitcibasi, Kwak, Ataca, Berry, Orung, Sunar, Charalambous, Goodwin, Wang, Angleitner, Stepanikova, Pick, Givaudan, Zhuravliova-Gionis, Konantambigi, Gelfand, Marinova, Mcbride-Chang and Kodic2001; Oishi et al. Reference Oishi, Schimmack, Diener and Suh1998; Sagiv & Schwartz Reference Sagiv and Schwartz1995; Schwartz Reference Schwartz, Vinkin, Soeters and Ester2004; Triandis Reference Triandis1995). In contemporary societies, collectivists and individualists differ significantly in their view of the social structure of the society in which they reside. Collectivists emphasize the boundary between in-group and out-group and are distrusting of and unwilling to contact out-group members; individualists make less distinction between in- and out-groups, and are more trusting of and show more willingness to contact out-groups (Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Bhawuk, Nishii, Bechtold, House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004; Oishi et al. Reference Oishi, Schimmack, Diener and Suh1998; Sagiv & Schwartz Reference Sagiv and Schwartz1995).

Fincher et al. (Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008) showed that the unidimension of collectivism–individualism across many countries of the world is predicted strongly by infectious disease prevalence. High parasite-stress is associated with high collectivism (low individualism), and low infectious disease risk with low collectivism (high individualism). Murray et al. (Reference Murray, Trudeau and Schaller2011) showed that cultural emphasis on conformity was positively related to pathogen prevalence across many countries. Other recent cross-national studies showed that collectivism, autocracy, traditional gender roles (women's subordination relative to men's higher status), and women's traditional sexual restrictiveness and continence are values that positively covary with one another, and occur in nations with high prevalence of infectious disease. The assortative sociality adaptations of xenophobia and ethnocentrism link these values to avoidance and management of parasites. Also, the antipoles of each of the values – individualism (hence, liberalism), democracy, and women's political rights, freedom, and increased participation in casual sex – are a positively covarying set of values and are found in countries with relatively low parasite-stress (Gangestad et al. Reference Gangestad, Haselton and Buss2006a; Murray & Schaller Reference Murray and Schaller2010; Schaller & Murray Reference Schaller and Murray2008; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009).

Moreover, Schaller and Murray (Reference Schaller and Murray2008) found that important components of personality seem to be part of assortative sociality and associated behavioral immunity. They reported that extraversion versus introversion and openness versus closedness to new experiences and ideas correlated with variation in parasite prevalence across many countries of the world. People in high-parasite-stress nations showed cautious personalities conducive to avoiding exposure to contagion from conspecifics – high introversion scores and low scores on interest in new ideas and experiences – whereas people in low-parasite-stress nations showed high extraversion and openness to novelty.

In addition, Thornhill et al. (Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010) have shown that, across countries, the relationships between parasite-stress and democratization, gender relations, sexual restrictiveness, and collectivism-individualism are much more strongly correlated with human infectious diseases that are transmissible between humans (called nonzoonotics) than with those that are not transmissible between humans (zoonotics). This is a strong test of the parasite-stress theory of sociality because only nonzoonotic human diseases can be contracted from conspecifics; and hence, assortative sociality and related values should be designed to respond primarily to these diseases.

The cross-national patterns we mentioned that support the parasite-stress theory of sociality, depending on the particular analysis, statistically controlled for potential confounders, including Murdock's (Reference Murdock1949) six world regions (see sect. 4.6), a particular region's history of colonization and conquest, and the respective countries' latitude, Gross Domestic Product (GDP) per capita, wealth disparity, population size, land area, and extent of democratization. The cross-cultural study of philopatry in traditional societies controlled for population size, big-game hunting, world region, and other variables. Relatedly, we reported that across a large sample of contemporary countries, collectivism is positively correlated with philopatry measured as adults remaining throughout life in their natal region (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008b).

In sum, we and others have argued that parasite-stress generated past selection that crafted the assortative-sociality psychological adaptation of humans. Accordingly, this adaptation has a condition-dependent functional design, resulting in the contingent expression by the individual of ancestrally adaptive degrees of in-group assortative sociality – arising from its three basic components, philopatry, xenophobia, and ethnocentrism – along a gradient of experienced parasite-stress. Individuals who experience relatively high levels of parasite-stress show greater in-group assortative sociality than those who experience relatively lower levels of parasite-stress (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008a; Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008b; Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Murray & Schaller Reference Murray and Schaller2010; Schaller & Duncan Reference Schaller, Duncan, Forges, Haselton and Von Hippel2007; Schaller & Murray Reference Schaller and Murray2008; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009; see also Freeland Reference Freeland1976; Reference Freeland1979; Loehle Reference Loehle1995 for earlier discussion of related ideas).

3. Extending the theory to family ties and religiosity

Thus far, research indicates that parasite-stress is an important correlate of the values of in-group assortative sociality as reflected in the cross-cultural elements of democracy, collectivism-individualism, conformity, dispersal behavior, and the personality components, openness and extraversion. Two other important elements of social life that appear to represent in-group assortative sociality are strong family ties and religiosity. Neither of these two topics has been placed, we feel, in the appropriate context of their causation by parasite-stress generating adaptive in-group assortative sociality. In the next sections, we develop hypotheses to explain the global patterns for these two phenomena based on relative variation in parasite-stress and then demonstrate, through empirical analyses involving most of the countries of the world as well as interstate comparisons within the United States, the centrality of parasite-stress for explaining the cross-cultural patterns of family ties and religiosity.

3.1. Strength of family ties

No one is without parents; but individuals do differ in the amount that they rely on or invest in their parents and other members of their family. Some people invest only in themselves or maybe at most in their nuclear family (spouse and children), while others consider themselves completely interdependent on a much larger extended family that includes not only spouses and children but also their parents, siblings, grandparents, cousins, uncles, aunts, nieces and nephews, and so on. Why this variation exists has been frequently discussed by scholars and it is generally attributed to differences in relative economic prosperity. For example, Inglehart and Baker (Reference Inglehart and Baker2000) provided evidence suggesting that countries that have relatively low wealth are also characterized by people who generally rely on their extended families; whereas people who live in countries with high levels of wealth are more able to strike out on their own because of the greater opportunities associated with greater wealth (see also Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Bhawuk, Nishii, Bechtold, House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004).

Intense loyalty and interdependence on the family (i.e., strong family ties) are generally considered important components of the cross-cultural dimension of collectivism-individualism, with collectivism positively associated with strong family ties in multiple cross-cultural studies (e.g., Alesina & Giuliano Reference Alesina and Giuliano2007; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Bhawuk, Nishii, Bechtold, House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004). Often, collectivism is indexed by measuring the relative importance of the family. For example, Vandello and Cohen (Reference Vandello and Cohen1999) developed a measure of collectivism for each of the states in the USA that includes scores for family loyalty and interdependence. The distribution of collectivism-individualism across the globe has been explained by the distribution of wealth across the world in the form of GDP with the highest levels of collectivism associated most strongly with lowest levels of societal wealth (Hofstede Reference Hofstede2001; Kashima & Kashima Reference Kashima and Kashima2003; Triandis Reference Triandis1995). This is consistent with the argument that prosperity can explain patterns of family interdependence.

There is an alternative view, however (Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008). Along with an emphasis on interdependence, collectivistic attitudes are generally associated with an unwillingness to contact or otherwise interact with out-group members (Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Bhawuk, Nishii, Bechtold, House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004; Oishi et al. Reference Oishi, Schimmack, Diener and Suh1998; Sagiv & Schwartz Reference Sagiv and Schwartz1995). As described in section 2.2, Fincher et al. (Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008) argue that this reflects the importance of avoiding out-group members, who may carry infectious diseases that an individual is not able to cope with. Relatedly, collectivism is adaptive under high levels of parasite-stress by providing the benefits of disease avoidance and management, while widespread individualism under low levels of parasite-stress provides benefits in terms of increased out-group interaction (e.g., increased sharing of goods and innovations). Our studies of multiple, separate but conceptually related measures of collectivism-individualism support this view (Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010). We make a similar argument for the importance of strong family ties – in areas with high levels of parasite-stress people will value strong family ties more than in regions with low parasite-stress. This reflects the importance of xenophobia for the avoidance of out-group members and of ethnocentrism for the development and maintenance of supportive in-group networks in the face of parasite-stress.

Current evidence that supports this comes from studies of traditional societies. To paraphrase Navarrete and Fessler (Reference Navarrete and Fessler2006), in human evolutionary history, when under parasite attack, in-group members were the only health insurance one had, and it was adaptive to have always paid your premiums – in terms of social investment and loyalty toward in-group allies that buffer an individual and his or her family against the morbidity and mortality of infectious disease. The support and loyalty toward in-group members was an individual's defense against the morbidity and mortality effects of parasites (Navarrete & Fessler Reference Navarrete and Fessler2006; Sugiyama Reference Sugiyama2004; Sugiyama & Sugiyama Reference Sugiyama and Sugiyama2003). Sugiyama (Reference Sugiyama2004) reported that among the Shiwiar, an Amazonian society without ready access to modern medicine, health care in the forms of food and other assistance from in-group members to persons suffering from infectious diseases was a major factor in lowering the mortality rate. This pattern, in general, seems to characterize numerous traditional human societies in the ethnographic record (Gurven et al. Reference Gurven, Allen-Arave, Hill and Hurtado2000; Hill & Hurtado Reference Hill and Hurtado2009; Sugiyama Reference Sugiyama2004; Sugiyama & Sugiyama Reference Sugiyama and Sugiyama2003).

Thornhill et al. (Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010) show as well that parasite-stress was positively associated with a measure of family ties across modern countries. In this target article we explore a new measure of the strength of family ties at the cross-national level, using updated World Values Survey files from a recently produced, public dataset which was unavailable at the time of Thornhill et al. (Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010). We also provide a novel empirical examination of parasite-stress in relation to a measure of collectivism across the states of the United States of America (Vandello & Cohen Reference Vandello and Cohen1999) and to a component of this measure that taps family-ties specifically.

3.2. Religiosity

Participation in a religion has certain costs for the participant, which include the time and effort involved in learning a religion and practicing it, the loss of opportunity to engage in other beneficial activities (opportunity costs), and risks such as the avoidance of modern medical care or extended fasting (Sosis et al. Reference Sosis, Kress and Boster2007). To learn the emotionality and associated language of a religion requires a long developmental (ontogenetic) exposure to the belief system. Opportunity costs include the inability to associate with other groups because one's specific beliefs may be considered irrational or contra-evidentiary to out-group members. (On irrationality as a functional component of religiosity, see Irons Reference Irons, Bulbulia, Sosis, Genet, Genet and Wyman2008).

This premise – that religious participation has costs – is a basis for studying religiosity from both the economic and the evolutionary science perspectives. From the economics viewpoint, Azzi and Ehrenberg (Reference Azzi and Ehrenberg1975) presented an analysis of individual religiosity as a function of the maximization of household allocation of time and found that people were attempting to rationally engage in religious behaviors such that they maximized the return on their time investments. Iannaccone has done much to formalize this economic investigation of religiosity (e.g., Iannaccone Reference Iannaccone1990; Reference Iannaccone1994, among others). Iannaccone (Reference Iannaccone1990) used economic theory to show that people employ sophisticated cost-benefit analyses often maximizing their investments when engaging in religious behaviors. Iannaccone (Reference Iannaccone1994) used rational choice theory to examine the relationship between a church's religious strictness and its strength or permanence, and concluded that, “Strictness reduces free riding. It screens out members who lack commitment and stimulates participation among those who remain” (p. 1204). In other words, paying-in indicates commitment but it also precludes desertion to other churches, because it is too costly to desert and then develop the same level of embeddedness in a new church. Therefore, individuals in strict churches exhibit higher rates of participation because they are assured, in comparison to individuals in less strict churches, a higher level of return on their investment through the reduction of free-riders and a higher level of investment by other individuals in the church. Iannaccone (Reference Iannaccone1994) observed that there is variation among churches in strictness and hence in the average religiosity among its members. That same research established that the strictest churches, those that require the highest costs for continued membership, have the tightest and most permanent collectives.

Many researchers have applied evolutionary costly-signaling theory to the understanding of religion and religious behavior (Bulbulia Reference Bulbulia2004a; Reference Bulbulia2004b; Cronk Reference Cronk1994; Henrich Reference Henrich2009; Irons Reference Irons1996; Reference Irons and Nesse2001; Johnson Reference Johnson, Bulbulia, Sosis, Genet, Genet, Harris and Wyman2008; Sosis Reference Sosis2000; Reference Sosis2003; Reference Sosis2005; Sosis & Alcorta Reference Sosis and Alcorta2003; Sosis & Bressler Reference Sosis and Bressler2003; Sosis & Ruffle Reference Sosis and Ruffle2003; Steadman & Palmer Reference Steadman and Palmer2008; Wilson Reference Wilson2002). This approach builds on the same foundation as the economic study of religious behavior – that religious participation has costs. Evolutionary science deepens our knowledge by providing methods that can pinpoint not only how benefits are maximized currently (also called the proximate-level of understanding), but also how benefits were maximized historically (also called ultimate-level of understanding). Through adaptationist study, evolutionary science has the ability to discover the historical setting in which a phenotypic feature that is an evolved adaptation, yielded net reproductive benefits (inclusive fitness) to its bearers (Andrews et al. Reference Andrews, Gangestad and Matthews2002; Thornhill Reference Thornhill, Bekoff and Jamieson1990; Williams Reference Williams1966). The researchers using costly-signaling theory propose that membership in a religious group is necessary for individuals to accrue certain social benefits not accessible independently, and that engaging in religious behavior is a signal of in-group allegiance to other individuals (both in-group and out-group individuals). The greater the costs of religious participation, the more honestly the participation signals allegiance to the religious in-group. The high costs of religiosity mentioned above are ideal for honestly signaling embeddedness in and commitment to an in-group with a particular spiritual belief system. Religion is often defined as a value system that is based on supernatural phenomena (Boyer Reference Boyer2001). This defining feature of religiosity has the high cost of displaying belief in the power of supernatural phenomena – phenomena that are generally considered antithetical to the empirical data humans gather from sensory experience (Irons Reference Irons, Bulbulia, Sosis, Genet, Genet and Wyman2008). Religious groups adopt their own distinct costly versions of supernatural beliefs in order to heighten costs of participation and distance themselves from out-groups.

Sosis (Reference Sosis2000) and Sosis and Bressler (Reference Sosis and Bressler2003) provided supportive evidence for the costly-signaling theory of religion by studying the longevity of 19th century United States communes. Sosis (Reference Sosis2000) found that religious communes in comparison to secular ones had longer life spans, and Sosis and Bressler (Reference Sosis and Bressler2003) found that longevity for religious communes was positively related to the magnitude of the costly acts required for membership within a commune (e.g., restriction from alcohol and sex). A logical prediction from the costly-signaling perspective, put in evolutionary theoretical terms, is that the adaptive value of religious signaling to signalers, and hence the magnitude and associated costs of the signal, will vary from place to place based on the underlying ecological necessity of in-group assortativeness for inclusive fitness maximization (also see, Sosis et al. Reference Sosis, Kress and Boster2007). According to the parasite-stress model, this ecological necessity is determined by parasite-stress variation across regions.

We argue that the maintenance of in-group assortativeness by practiced and signaled religious allegiance provides two benefits: (a) the protective barrier provided by separation from out-group individuals who may harbor novel infectious diseases and/or perform non-normative behavior; and (b) in-group embeddedness that reduces the morbidity and mortality caused when infectious disease invades the in-group. Hence, measures of the importance of religion for people in an area (religiosity) should be predictable based on the area's position along the parasite gradient, reflecting the average infectious disease stress experienced by people in the region. Therefore, we hypothesized that religious participation and commitment, indicating the importance of in-group assortative sociality, would be positively related to parasite-stress across regions.

One of the assumptions of our hypothesis is that there is a positive relationship between religiosity and out-group dislike or in-group preference. Evidence supporting this is found in a few studies. For example, Jackson and Hunsberger (Reference Jackson and Hunsberger1999) conducted a study of the relationships between individuals' religiosity and their prejudicial attitudes toward religious and non-religious others. They found that the religious participants showed significant positive attitudes towards in-group-religious others but negative attitudes towards non-religious others. As well, the magnitude of the prejudice was correspondent to the individual's own level of religiosity. That is, a participant who scored highly on religious fundamentalism also scored highly on out-group prejudice. In a separate study, Bulbulia and Mahoney (Reference Bulbulia and Mahoney2008) demonstrated that New Zealand Christians were more altruistic toward Canadian Christians than were New Zealand citizens to other New Zealand citizens. Similarly, Widman et al. (Reference Widman, Corcoran and Nagy2009) showed that individuals with strong Christian beliefs were more likely to rate others displaying a symbol of Christianity (a cross) as more kind and moral than others not displaying such a symbol. These studies support our assumption. They also suggest, on the one hand, the importance of religiosity as a marker of in-group membership, and, on the other hand, an underlying mental mechanism within individuals to measure religious similarity. Such a mechanism was indicated by Park and Schaller (Reference Park and Schaller2005), who found that when people experienced attitudinal similarity with others, they considered them more like family than when attitudes were dissimilar. Furthermore, there is convincing evidence that religious prosociality is primarily in-group altruism (Norenzayan & Shariff Reference Norenzayan and Shariff2008).

In a study supportive of the proposal we present (albeit this study was not designed to test the parasite-stress model), Saroglou et al. (Reference Saroglou, Delpierre and Dernelle2004) conducted a meta-analysis of the relationships between Schwartz's model of 10 cross-culturally stable, core values (Schwartz Reference Schwartz1992) and religiosity. Saroglou et al.'s (2004) meta-analysis focused on 21 samples from 15 countries (total n=8551 people). They discovered that religious people favored values that promoted social order (mainly the values Tradition and Conformity) but disliked values that promoted openness to change and autonomy (Stimulation and Self-Direction). This was true across a variety of religions (i.e., Christians, Jews, and Muslims) and countries from Europe, North America, and the Middle East. Interestingly, the positive correlation between religiosity and “conservation” (=conservative) values (Conformity, Tradition, and Security) and the negative relationship between religiosity and openness to change and autonomy (Stimulation and Self-Direction) showed greater effect sizes in a sample of Mediterranean countries in contrast to a sample of Western European countries. Mediterranean countries have greater levels of parasite-stress than Western European countries (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008b; Guernier et al. Reference Guernier, Hochberg and Guégan2004).

The question may be raised: If a signal such as religious identity is strong enough to function as a social boundary, then why are multiple signaling modalities (e.g., language and religion) used to indicate in-group allegiance? Signaling systems across species typically show redundancy across components or modalities (Searcy & Nowicki Reference Searcy and Nowicki2005). This is thought to enhance communication, given that each signal is imperfect in information content but, combined, they provide greater accuracy. Redundancy is seen in human signaling of in-group affiliation and boundary. A combination of signals involving religiosity, language or dialect, word use, dress, music, smell, and so on comprise a redundant suite of honest signals about one's group membership and embeddedness.

3.2.1. Other models to explain cross-cultural differences in religiosity.Footnote 1

McCleary and Barro (Reference McCleary and Barro2006) have explored the validity of the secularization hypothesis that economic development causes lower levels of religiosity among individuals. Inglehart and Baker (Reference Inglehart and Baker2000) and Norris and Inglehart (Reference Norris and Inglehart2004) have suggested that individuals reduce religiosity when conditions of living are benign but become religiously embedded under dire conditions of hardship and high mortality salience (i.e., the existential security hypothesis). In both models, people are less religious in areas where they have less “need” of a religion and the benefits that it offers. We refer collectively to the secularization hypothesis and the existential security hypothesis as the “conditions-of-living” model. The conditions-of-living model has been tested and supported, in part, by examining the relationships between religiosity and economic conditions across countries. McCleary and Barro (Reference McCleary and Barro2006) focused on GDP per capita as the most indicative marker of economic development, demonstrating significant negative relationships between economic development and religiosity. Norris and Inglehart (Reference Norris and Inglehart2004) showed large differences in religiosity between wealthy and poor nations, providing positive support for their hypothesis that people living in poor conditions also show greater religiosity. More recent tests found support for the conditions-of-living model: Rees (Reference Rees2009) discovered a positive relationship between income inequality (used as a proxy for personal insecurity) and religiosity across many nations, and Delamontagne (Reference Delamontagne2010) found that social inequality (measured by inequalities in education, income and race) was highly, positively predictive of religiosity in the United States.

There is clearly overlap between the conditions-of-living model and our proposed framework because high levels of infectious diseases are a component of “dire conditions” and low economic development. Indeed, both Inglehart and Baker (Reference Inglehart and Baker2000) and McCleary and Barro (Reference McCleary and Barro2006) mention disease differences across countries and explicitly try to treat disease in their analyses by including a country's latitude (latitude is negatively correlated with infectious disease stress; e.g., Guernier et al. Reference Guernier, Hochberg and Guégan2004). Our approach is different in that it incorporates the evolutionary history of Homo sapiens into the research framework for generating hypotheses and predictions. Our model relies on specific aspects of the biology of infectious diseases and incorporates these processes into the hypothesized design of the human mind and human behavior by Darwinian selection acting in the context of parasite-stress. For example, we make predictions based on the fact that out-group conspecifics that carry novel infectious diseases can be potentially dangerous to an individual's reproductive success. This leads to predictions about the evolution of human psychology and its manifestations in values or ideology that are not generated from the models presented by Inglehart and Baker (Reference Inglehart and Baker2000), Norris and Inglehart (Reference Norris and Inglehart2004) or McCleary and Barro (Reference McCleary and Barro2006). Furthermore, the conditions-of-living model, as currently formulated, assumes that individuals will turn to an in-group under conditions of stress. However, this assumption isn't framed to consider the costs and benefits of seeking support from an out-group under ecological stress. Contact with an out-group can provide benefits unattainable from an in-group. We attempt to erect this framework by providing a fundamental explanation for the relative costs of interacting with in-groups versus out-groups under different ecological settings of parasite-stress.

4. Methods for establishing an empirical link between family ties, religiosity, and parasite-stress

4.1. Strength of family ties

We propose that individuals who value strong family ties will be found predominantly in areas with greater parasite-stress because of the benefits of in-group assortativeness promoted by family embeddedness. We predict, then, a positive relationship between the strength of family ties and parasite-stress across nations and across the states of the United States of America.

4.1.1. Cross-nationalFootnote 2 : Strength of Family Ties

We compiled a new measure of the strength of family ties which assesses the importance of family loyalty and interdependence. It is similar to that used by the GLOBE project (House et al. Reference House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004), and by Alesina and Giuliano (Reference Alesina and Giuliano2007) and Thornhill et al. (Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010), but is more encompassing and updated. Data for the five items comprising our index came from the 1981–2007 pooled dataset of the World Values Survey across 72 countries (see the Electronic Supplement 1.A [ES 1.A], which can be viewed at http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012001). All five components were summed to become our measure, Strength of Family Ties. Larger values indicate stronger family ties while smaller values indicate weaker family ties. The data are provided in the Electronic Supplement 2 (ES 2) which can be viewed at http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012002.

4.1.2. United StatesFootnote 3 : Collectivism and Strength of Family Ties USA

In order to investigate family ties in the United States we used a measure of state-level collectivism compiled and validated by Vandello and Cohen (Reference Vandello and Cohen1999) because collectivism includes strong family ties; it also includes preferential assortment with in-group members outside the extended family (Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Bhawuk, Nishii, Bechtold, House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004; Hofstede Reference Hofstede2001; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009; Triandis Reference Triandis1995). Vandello and Cohen (Reference Vandello and Cohen1999) measured collectivism (referred to here as Collectivism) across the U.S. states by standardizing and summing eight items obtained from state data archives (ES 1.B). Larger values indicate greater collectivism (or less individualism) while smaller values indicate lower levels of collectivism (or more individualism). We extracted from the same sources data for the three components that specifically address family ties as described by Vandello and Cohen (ES 1.B). The three items were combined to become the variable Strength of Family Ties USA. Larger values indicate stronger family ties while smaller values indicate weaker family ties. The data are provided in the Electronic Supplement 3 (ES 3) which can be viewed at http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012003.

4.2. Religiosity

We predicted a positive association between religiosity and parasite-stress cross-nationally and across the states of the United States of America. To test this we indexed religiosity with two measures: (a) religious affiliation, and (b) religious participation and value. In the next sections, we describe how these variables were constructed for the cross-national and interstate analyses.

4.2.1. Religious affiliation

According to the parasite-stress model, people in areas with more parasite-stress will adhere to local religious systems to a greater extent than individuals in areas with low parasite-stress. This is because the values of people in areas with low parasite-stress provide them with greater flexibility in whether they adhere to a religion or not, or they may make up their own system of secular beliefs. The benefits of heightened in-group assortative sociality are predicted to be higher in high parasite-stress areas than in low parasite-stress areas. Hence, we predict that the proportion of religionists in an area would be positively correlated with parasite-stress because higher levels of parasite-stress can potentially increase the costs of nonconformity to in-group values and norms. We describe next our measures of religious affiliation for both the cross-national and the United States analyses.

4.2.1.1. Cross-national: Proportion of Religionists

To construct this variable, we extracted the proportion of non-religionists for the year 2000 from the World Christian Encyclopedia (Barrett et al. Reference Barrett, Kurian and Johnson2001), an oft-used and highly regarded resource in religious scholarship (Grim & Finke Reference Grim and Finke2006). Non-religionists include the two forms of nonbelievers: agnostics and atheists. The proportion of non-religionists within nations ranged from 0 (e.g., Afghanistan) to 55.6% (Democratic People's Republic of Korea) (n=230 countries). The proportion of non-religionists was subtracted from 1 to yield our analytical variable, Proportion of Religionists, which was arcsine-square-root transformed.

4.2.1.2. Cross-national: Proportion of Believers

For the Proportion of Believers we used the inverse of the “proportion of nonbelievers in God” as presented in Lynn et al. (Reference Lynn, Harvey and Nyborg2009; this is a tabulation of data described in Zuckerman Reference Zuckerman and Martin2007). This measure relies in part on values from the World Christian Encyclopedia (Barrett et al. Reference Barrett, Kurian and Johnson2001) but incorporates many other survey sources and likely provides more reliable estimates. The proportion of nonbelievers ranged from .5% (e.g., Cameroon) to 81% (Vietnam) (n=137 countries). The values were subtracted from 1 to represent the Proportion of Believers, which was arcsine-square-root transformed. The Proportion of Religionists and the Proportion of Believers were positively correlated (r=.67, n=137, p < .0001).

4.2.1.3. United States: Proportion of Religionists USA

The 2001 American Religious Identification Survey (ARIS; Kosmin et al. Reference Kosmin, Mayer and Keysar2001) was a telephone survey of 50,281 households. The survey asked, “What is your religion, if any?” From this, we obtained the proportion of respondents that indicated “no religion” for each state (Hawaii and Alaska were not included in the ARIS 2001). The “no religion” proportion/state value was subtracted from 1 to represent the Proportion of Religionists USA, and then arcsine-square-root transformed.

4.2.1.4. United States: Proportion of Religious Adherents

The Association of Statisticians of American Religious Bodies conducted a study of 149 religious bodies in the United States over the years 1999–2001 to assess the number of congregations in each state within the USA. The study produced a measure of the total adherents of each congregation providing a comprehensive measure of the total religious adherents in each state. These data comprised our interstate variable, Proportion of Religious Adherents (ES 1.C), which was arcsine-square-root transformed. The Proportion of Religious Adherents was correlated positively with the Proportion of Religionists USA (r=.66, n=48, p < .0001).

4.2.2. Religious participation and value

We predicted that the magnitude of time and effort dedicated to religious practice and the value placed on religious practice and ideals would be positively correlated with parasite-stress. We describe next our measures of religious participation and value cross-nationally and within the United States.

4.2.2.1. Cross-national: Religious Participation and Value

We created an index of Religious Participation and Value based on items contained in the World Values Survey collected in 1981–2007 from about 344,000 individuals in 95 countries (ES 1.D). The data are in the ES 2. Also, we created a variable, Proportion that Prayed Every Day, from the same survey (ES 1.D). The Proportion that Prayed Every Day was correlated positively with Religious Participation and Value, r=.93 (n=59, p < .0001). And, Religious Participation and Value was correlated positively with the Proportion of Religionists (r=.74, n=90, p < .0001) and the Proportion of Believers (r=.83, n=82, p < .0001). Furthermore, the Proportion that Prayed Every Day was correlated positively with the Proportion of Religionists (r=.64, n=57, p < .0001) and the Proportion of Believers (r=.85, n=51, p < .0001).

4.2.2.2. United States: Religious Participation and Value USA

The Pew Forum on Religion and Public Life produced the report “U.S. Religious Landscape Survey, Religious Affiliation: diverse and dynamic (February 2008).” We collected data for eight items from this survey and generated the variable Religious Participation and Value USA (ES 1.E). The data are in the ES 3. Religious Participation and Value USA was positively correlated with the Proportion of Religionists USA and the Proportion of Religious Adherents (religionists: r=.56, n=44, p < .0001; adherents: r=.42, n=45, p=.0041).

4.3. In-group assortative sociality

There is considerable conceptual overlap between religiosity and family ties that, we argue, reflects the importance of in-group assortative sociality within societies (e.g., the cross-national Religious Participation and Value was positively correlated with Strength of Family Ties, r=.79, n=72 countries, p < .0001). Because of this conceptual overlap, we generated synthetic in-group assortative sociality variables, one cross-national, which we called In-Group Assortativeness, and one for the states of the USA, which we called In-Group Assortativeness USA, to capture the common variation among our multiple dependent variables tapping in-group assortative sociality (ES 1.F). The cross-national data are in ES 2 and the interstate data are in ES 3.

4.4. Parasite-stress

4.4.1. Cross-national: Infectious Disease DALY

We used the World Health Organization (WHO) variable Infectious Disease DALY, a cross-national measure of morbidity and mortality (Disability Adjusted Life Years; DALY) attributed to 28 different “infectious and parasitic diseases” for the year 2002 (e.g., tuberculosis, measles, leprosy, dengue; WHO 2004). The DALY measure combines the time lived with disability and the time lost due to premature mortality. One “Infectious Disease DALY” is equivalent to one lost year of healthy life, with the burden of infectious disease as a measurement of the gap between current health status and an ideal situation where everyone lives into old age free of disease and disability (ES 1.G).

4.4.2. Cross-national: Nonzoonotic versus Zoonotic Parasite Prevalence

An important element of the parasite-stress theory of sociality is the costs associated with acquiring diseases from out-group humans. Thus, infectious diseases that are transmissible between humans are predicted to be more important for assortative sociality than human infectious diseases that are not transmitted between humans (Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010). Human-to-human transmitted infectious diseases are of two types, referred to as human-specific and multi-host diseases, respectively. Human-specific diseases are ones that humans are only able to acquire from other humans (e.g., measles, cholera, hookworm), whereas multi-host diseases are those that humans contract from other humans but in which the parasites can use either human or other animals as hosts to carry out their reproductive life (e.g., leishmaniasis, leprosy, dengue fever). These two types of infectious diseases contrast with zoonotic diseases (e.g., lyme disease, rabies, tularemia) that humans are only able to acquire from species other than humans (livestock and wildlife). Using Smith et al.'s (2007) classification of these disease types, we determined the prevalence (number of cases) of human-specific and multi-host infectious diseases per country (called “nonzoonotic”) and of zoonotic diseases, based on data from the GIDEON database (Global Infectious Disease & Epidemiology Network; www.gideononline.com). The earlier cross-national study of cultural variation by Thornhill et al. (Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010) used a different measure of these diseases: the number of diseases of each type, not the prevalence (Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010). Prevalence measures are likely better assays of the impact of parasitic diseases than the number of such diseases (Dunn et al. Reference Dunn, Davies, Harris and Gavin2010). Nonzoonotic Parasite Prevalence was correlated positively with Zoonotic Parasite Prevalence (r=.61, n=226, p < .0001). Nonzoonotic Parasite Prevalence was correlated positively with Infectious Disease DALY (r=.76, n=192, p < .0001), as was Zoonotic Parasite Prevalence (r=.16, n=192, p=.03). See ES 1.H for further details on the construction of this measure. The Electronic Supplement 4 (ES 4) which can be viewed at http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012004, contains the list of infectious diseases and their classification. The Electronic Supplement 2 contains the national values for the nonzoonotic and zoonotic parasite prevalence variables.

4.4.3. Cross-national: Combined Parasite-Stress

Because there is overlap and covariation in our infectious disease measures, we standardized Infectious Disease DALY, and Nonzoonotic Parasite Prevalence, and then summed these scores for each country to become Combined Parasite-Stress (Cronbach's α=.76, n=192). Zoonotic Parasite Prevalence was not included because of its minimal relationship with the dependent variables (see sect. 5.1.1). Combined Parasite-Stress was the focal variable used in the cross-national multivariate analyses (see sect. 4.5.1). These scores are in ES 2.

4.4.4. United States: Parasite-Stress USA

We obtained the annual Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report's “Summary of Notifiable Diseases, United States” from the Centers for Disease Control (CDC) for the years 1993 to 2007 (available at: www.cdc.gov). For each year, we adjusted the number of cases of all infectious diseases tracked by the CDC for which there was information for all states for that year with the CDC-reported population size for each state (i.e., for some diseases – not all states reported whether cases occurred [termed “non-notifiable” by CDC]; these unreported diseases were not included in the tally). For each state, we determined the average z-score of this population-adjusted disease incidence score for the 15-year time-span. This approach was necessary because the infectious diseases tracked by the CDC can vary between years, though there was often great similarity between years. The standardization allowed us to pinpoint a state's position along a parasite-stress gradient relative to the other states. See ES 1.I for validation of this index. The Electronic Supplement 5 (ES 5) which can be viewed at http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012005, contains the list of diseases included in our index for each year and the data are in ES 3.

4.5. Potentially confounding influences

As described in the introduction, there are features other than parasite-stress and assortative sociality (e.g., economic development) that have been proposed as explanations of the strength of family ties and religiosity. We next outline our approach used to explore alternative causal conjectures.

4.5.1. Cross-national approach

We examined zero-order correlations between the potentially confounding factors (described below) and the dependent variables. Potentially confounding variables that were significantly correlated (p≤.05) were then entered into multiple regressions with Combined Parasite-Stress and the dependent variables to determine whether the associations predicted by the parasite-stress theory remained after removing the effect of the potentially confounding variables. For the cross-national analysis, we examined the effects of national wealth (Gross domestic product per capita in US dollars purchasing power parity averaged over the years 1960–2008 [GDP per capita]; raw data obtained from data.worldbank.org) and the equitability of resource distribution within a nation. For the equitability of resource distribution, we used the measure produced by Vanhanen (Reference Vanhanen2003), called resource distribution (and referred to here as Resource Distribution), that incorporates GDP per capita, percentages of university students and literates, the degree to which land ownership is widespread, and the degree of decentralization of non-agricultural economic resources. We also examined the effects of human freedom (e.g., the freedoms of expression and belief), using the average of cross-national scores of civil liberties from Freedom House for the years 1972–2008, Civil Liberty (www.freedomhouse.org). In our regression analyses, we used two model specifications. The most general model contained Combined Parasite-Stress, Civil Liberty, and Resource Distribution as the predictor variables of each of the dependent variables. Resource Distribution includes GDP per capita; however, because of the large amount of research that focuses on GDP per capita we tested a second model that used GDP per capita and Combined Parasite-Stress as the predictor variables.

While we have identified some potentially confounding factors there are likely others that we have not identified. Because we propose that parasite-stress is an encompassing causal factor, we regressed the average life expectancy at birth (for the year 2008) for both sexes combined (data from data.worldbank.org) on Nonzoonotic Parasite Prevalence (r 2=.51, n=190, p < .0001). Infectious Disease DALY was not included, because its calculation by WHO incorporates life expectancy. The residuals from this regression represent the variation in life span expectancy that cannot be explained by parasite-stress (i.e., potentially, this variation represents other causal factors besides parasite-stress). We then used these residuals in correlations with the strength of family ties and religiosity variables to address the potential of causal factors besides parasite-stress to account for international variation in strength of family ties and religiosity.

4.5.2. United States approach

For addressing potentially confounding variables in the USA analysis, we followed a similar approach as in the cross-national analysis. We examined zero-order correlations among the dependent variables and the potentially confounding factors described below. Variables that were significantly correlated (p≤.05) were then entered into multiple regressions with Parasite-Stress USA to examine whether the predicted associations between parasite-stress and the dependent variables remained after controlling the potentially confounding factors. The factors across states that we considered were GDP per capita and the Gini index (a measure of wealth inequality). GDP per capita was an average of the values for years 1999 to 2007 obtained from the Bureau of Economic Analysis (data from www.bea.gov). Gini was measured at the family level for 1999, the last year available for the variable from the Census Bureau (data from www.census.gov).

As with the cross-national analysis, we regressed average life expectancy at birth for both sexes combined for the year 2000 (obtained from www.census.gov) on Parasite-Stress USA. This regression was significant for the larger USA data set (r 2=.45, n=50, p < .0001) as well as for the restricted Pew Forum dataset (r 2=.46, n=46, p < .0001). The residuals of these regressions represent the variation in life expectancy that is not explained by our measure of parasite-stress. The finding of statistically significant covariation between these residuals and any one of the dependent variables would imply causation other than parasite-stress.

4.6. The problem of nonindependence

Geographically adjacent countries or U.S. states may be similar to each other due to common influences such as experiencing similar levels of infectious disease. Because of this, statistical independence among analytical units in the cross-national and the USA analyses may be questioned. To account for this potential problem, we used the following approach: We divided the countries into six world regions according to the method devised by Murdock (Reference Murdock1949), which is based on geographical proximity and cultural historical contact. Murdock's division of world cultures reduces the interdependence between societies among the six regions. The country assignments to the world regions are indicated in ES 2. Then, we conducted correlations using the mean values for each of the variables for each world region. This approach allowed us to characterize a region composed of multiple countries (or states) into a single value (thus deflating sample size). The small sample size makes the p-values suspect, but it does allow us to examine whether the correlations remain in the direction predicted by the parasite-stress theory after reducing the sample size. We also conducted a nested-effect linear regression that accounts for the nested design of our analysis. In the cross-national case, Combined Parasite-Stress was nested within each world region as the independent variable and used to predict the different dependent variables. Similarly, for the USA, we divided the states into the nine geographic regions used by the Census Bureau and used both approaches as we did for the cross-national analysis.

5. Results

5.1. Cross-national analyses

5.1.1. Are nonzoonotic infectious diseases more important for explaining assortative sociality than are zoonotics?

The answer is, Yes. Each of the dependent variables was correlated positively and significantly with Nonzoonotic Parasite Prevalence (correlation coefficients ranged from .40 to .65) while Zoonotic Parasite Prevalence was insignificantly correlated with all but one of the dependent variables (correlation coefficients ranged from −.17 to .17) (ES 6.A, The Electronic Supplement 6, which can be viewed at http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012006, contains tabulations of the results presented throughout sect. 5.) The only significant relationship between a dependent variable, Proportion of Religionists, and Zoonotic Parasite Prevalence showed a negative sign (−.17) and hence was in the direction opposite that seen with nonzoonotics. Zoonotic diseases were not generally predictive of the strength of family ties and religiosity cross-nationally. Therefore, Zoonotic Parasite Prevalence was not included in further analyses.

5.1.2. Is the strength of family ties predicted by parasite-stress?

Again, the answer is, Yes. The Strength of Family Ties was correlated positively with the parasite-stress variables measured singly or in combination; correlation coefficients ranged from .57 to .64 (ES 6.A).

5.1.3. Is religious affiliation positively correlated with parasite-stress?

Yes. Each of the infectious-disease-stress variables was correlated positively with each of the two religious affiliation variables, the Proportion of Religionists and the Proportion of Believers; correlation coefficients ranged from .40 to .64 (ES 6.A).

5.1.4. Is religious participation and value positively correlated with parasite-stress?

Yes. Each of the two variables measuring religious participation and value, Religious Participation and Value and the Proportion That Prayed Every Day, were correlated positively with each of the parasite-stress variables; correlation coefficients ranged from .50 to .73 (ES 6.A).

5.1.5. Is in-group assortative sociality predicted by parasite-stress?

Yes. The synthetic measure, In-Group Assortativeness, was correlated positively with the infectious-disease-stress variables; correlation coefficients ranged from .65 to .72 (ES 6.A) (see Fig. 1).

Figure 1. The correlation between Combined Parasite-Stress and In-Group Assortativeness for the 65 nations with correspondent data for all 11 items that make up the 2 variables (r=.71, p<.0001). The line is the regression line.

5.1.6. Are these findings repeated in world regions?

Yes. When considering the correlation between the dependent variables and Combined Parasite-Stress at the world regional level, all correlations were positive and thus in the direction predicted by the parasite-stress theory (Strength of Family Ties: r=.94; Proportion of Religionists: r=.70; Proportion of Believers: r=.82; Religious Participation and Value: r=.76; Proportion That Prayed Every Day: r=.46; In-group Assortativeness: r=.89; n=6 world regions for all).

When nested within world regions, Combined Parasite-Stress predicted significantly the Strength of Family Ties (r 2=.47, n=69); the Proportion of Religionists (r 2=.25, n=191); the Proportion of Believers (r 2=.44, n=136); Religious Participation and Value (r 2=.55, n=89); the Proportion That Prayed Every Day (r 2=.47, n=57); and, In-Group Assortativeness (r 2=.57, n=65). All regressions were significant (p < .0001).

5.1.7. Are these findings confounded by variation in other causal variables such as human freedom, resource distribution, or unidentified variables?

In this case the answer is, No. Amongst the three focal, potentially confounding variables, only Civil Liberty scores were nonsignificantly correlated with the Proportion of Religionists; the other two potentially confounding variables had significant zero-order correlations with the Strength of Family Ties, the two religious affiliation variables, and the two religious participation and value variables, and In-Group Assortativeness (ES 6.A). Therefore, each relevant, potentially confounding variable was checked to see if it accounted for the correlation between parasite-stress and family ties or religiosity variables. None did (ES 6.B). In a series of multiple regressions, Combined Parasite-Stress remained a positive predictor of the Strength of Family Ties, the four religion variables, and In-Group Assortativeness after controlling the effects of the potential confounders (standardized beta coefficients for parasite-stress ranged from .28 to .59). Thus, the positive association between parasite-stress and family ties or religiosity was robust to the effects of freedom, resource distribution, or economic development, as captured by Civil Liberty, Resource Distribution, and GDP per capita.

The majority of the correlations between the residuals of the regression of life span on Combined Parasite-Stress and Strength of Family Ties, the religiosity variables, and In-group Assortativeness were statistically insignificant (ES 6.C). The Proportion of Believers showed a significant correlation, but the effect size was much reduced and in the opposite direction compared to the correlation between Combined Parasite-Stress and the same variable (−.18 versus .63). In general, the relationship between life expectancy independent of parasite-stress and the dependent variables was trivial.

5.2. United States analyses

5.2.1. Is collectivism and family ties predicted positively by parasite-stress?

Yes. Parasite-Stress USA was correlated positively and significantly with Collectivism and the Strength of Family Ties USA (ES 6.D).

5.2.2. Is religious affiliation positively correlated with parasite-stress?

Yes. Parasite-Stress USA was correlated positively and significantly with each of the two religious affiliation variables, the Proportion of Religious Adherents and the Proportion of Religionists USA (ES 6.D).

5.2.3. Is religious participation and value positively correlated with parasite-stress?

Yes. Parasite-Stress USA was correlated positively and significantly with Religious Participation and Value USA (ES 6.D).

5.2.4. Is in-group assortative sociality predicted positively by parasite-stress?

Yes. Parasite-Stress USA was correlated positively and significantly with the synthetic measure of In-Group Assortativeness USA (ES 6.D) (see Fig. 2).

Figure 2. The correlation between Parasite-Stress USA and In-Group Assortativeness USA for the 43 states/state combinations with correspondent data for all 14 items that make up the 2 variables (r=.66, p<.0001). The line is the regression line.

5.2.5. Are these findings repeated in regional analyses?

Yes. When considering the correlation between the dependent variables and Parasite-Stress USA at the regional level, all correlations were in the direction predicted by the parasite-stress theory (Collectivism: r=.83; Strength of Family Ties USA: r=.51; Proportion of Religionists USA: r=.60; Proportion of Religious Adherents: r=.40; Religious Participation and Value USA: r=.85; In-Group Assortativeness USA: r=.89; n=9 for all).

When nested within USA regions, Parasite-Stress USA predicted significantly the Strength of Family Ties USA (r 2=.34, n=50, p=.0326); Collectivism (r 2=.45, n=50, p=.0021); the Proportion of Religionists USA (r 2=.61, n=48, p < .0001); the Proportion of Religious Adherents (r 2=.39, n=50, p=.0106); Religious Participation and Value USA (r 2=.54, n=46, p=.0004); and In-Group Assortativeness USA (r 2=.66, n=43, p < .0001).

5.2.6. Are these findings confounded by other causal variables such as wealth, wealth disparity, or perhaps unidentified variables?

No. Of the potentially confounding variables, Gini was significantly correlated with Collectivism, Strength of Family Ties USA, and In-Group Assortativeness USA but not with the Proportion of Religionists USA, Proportion of Religious Adherents, or Religious Participation and Value USA; GDP per capita was significantly correlated with Religious Participation and Value USA and In-Group Assortativeness USA (ES 6.D). Given these significant zero-order correlations, Gini was entered in a multiple regression with Parasite-Stress USA as predictors of Collectivism and Strength of Family Ties USA. And GDP per capita was entered in a multiple regression with Parasite-Stress USA as predictors of Religious Participation and Value USA. Both Gini and GDP per capita were included with Parasite-Stress USA as predictors of In-Group Assortativeness USA. The results of these regressions are shown in ES 6.E. In all cases, Parasite-Stress USA remained a significant, positive predictor of the dependent variables. Thus, the correlation between parasite-stress and family ties or religiosity was not confounded with the effects of economic inequality and development as captured by the Gini index and GDP per capita.

The residuals from regressing state-level life expectancy on Parasite-Stress USA were not significantly correlated with Collectivism (r=.11, n=50, p=.4367), Strength of Family Ties USA (r=.22, n=50, p=.1330), the Proportion of Religionists USA (r=−.10, n=48, p=.4839), or the Proportion of Religious Adherents (r=.09, n=50, p=.5180). The residuals were correlated with Religious Participation and Value USA (r=−.38, n=46, p=.0083) and In-group Assortativeness USA (r=−.35, n=43, p=.0214). Therefore, the relationship between life expectancy independent of parasite-stress and collectivism, strength of family ties, and religious affiliation was trivial, while there was a significant negative relationship between life expectancy independent of parasite-stress and religious participation and value and in-group assortativeness. The significance of the parasite-stress-independent variation may reflect the greater prevalence of non-infectious diseases such as forms of diabetes, heart disease, and cancer in the United States as compared to other countries that have lower income levels (see Lopez et al. Reference Lopez, Mathers, Ezzati, Jamison and Murray2006).

6. Discussion

Mainly, we show that when comparing countries or states within the United States, in-group assortative sociality, as reflected in strong family ties and heightened religiosity, is positively associated with parasite-stress. These findings were predicted from the parasite-stress theory of sociality. Also, paramount was the finding that the prevalence of nonzoonotic infectious diseases was more important for explaining these patterns, in the cross-national analysis, than were zoonotic infectious diseases. The findings remain when statistically removing the effects of other potential causal factors such as societal wealth and freedom. We presented the hypothesis in section 2.1 that people have psychological adaptations for enculturation that select cultural elements based on local utility in navigation of the social environment pertaining to infectious disease threats. Such adaptation is consistent with our findings on cross-national and interstate variation in values of family ties and religiosity.

6.1. Family ties

Our cross-national analysis showed that the strength of family ties was positively correlated with all measures of parasite-stress. And, as predicted, the strength of family ties was correlated more strongly with nonzoonotic infectious diseases than with zoonotic infectious diseases. Within the United States the strength of family ties and collectivism were both strongly, positively correlated with parasite-stress. Our findings are cross-cultural extensions to the ethnographic work that demonstrated the important role of nepotistic and other in-group altruistic support for surviving parasitic infections (Gurven et al. Reference Gurven, Allen-Arave, Hill and Hurtado2000; Hill & Hurtado Reference Hill and Hurtado2009; Sugiyama Reference Sugiyama2004; Sugiyama & Sugiyama Reference Sugiyama and Sugiyama2003).

The findings cross-nationally and across the USA that strong family ties was correlated with parasite-stress complement our earlier work that linked collectivism-individualism with parasite-stress (Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010). Our finding of the strong positive correlation between Vandello and Cohen's measure of collectivism and parasite-stress within the United States is also an important complement to the cross-national findings of the same relationship reported in Fincher et al. (Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008) and Thornhill et al. (Reference Thornhill, Fincher, Murray and Schaller2010).

It would be relevant to explore regional analysis within other countries that contain significant parasite gradients. For example, Japan's northern island of Hokkaido rivals the high individualism in the United States (Kitayama et al. Reference Kitayama, Ishii, Imada, Takemura and Ramaswamy2006). Probably, Hokkaido has a much lower level of parasite-stress than does southern Japan, given the negative covariation of parasite-stress and latitude (e.g., Guernier et al. Reference Guernier, Hochberg and Guégan2004). Also, in China, historically much of the innovation originating in China derived from the northern region, which was much lower in parasite-stress than the southern portion, below the Yellow River (McNeill Reference McNeill1998). Innovation – both its generation and the willingness of people to adopt it – corresponds to individualistic values (Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009). The regional development of innovation in China and elsewhere could be studied more thoroughly in its relation to historical pathogen stress. In the United States we found significant regional variation in values in spite of generally low parasite-stress relative to many other areas of the world. We expect this pattern to be repeated across the world in nations that have a parasite gradient.

One aspect of human family life that has been studied often is the demographic transition from large families to smaller families. One of the more convincing explanations for this phenomenon comes from Newson et al. (Reference Newson, Postmes, Lea and Webley2005). They argue that the demographic transition arose from an increase over time in the ratio of non-kin to kin in individuals' social networks. We agree but offer a reinterpretation of the meaning of the demographic transition. Based on our earlier studies on collectivism (cited already) and the present article on collectivism and family ties, it is reasonable to assume that this increase in the non-kin to kin ratio is related to a decrease in parasite-stress over time and the corresponding increase in individualism. Moreover, the countries in which the demographic transition has occurred are the same ones that have experienced a relative emancipation from infectious disease (Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009).

In-group assortative sociality is not restricted to humans; indeed, it appears to be widespread across animal taxa (Fincher & Thornhill Reference Fincher and Thornhill2008a). We hypothesize that parasite-stress was one of the main forces of selection responsible for adaptations that function in family life (see also Lewis [Reference Lewis1998] with respect to the evolution of kin altruism as a response to parasite-stress). Hence, we propose that variation in parasite prevalence accounts for the large variation, across animal species, in the degree of extended nepotism exhibited outside the social unit of parent(s) and offspring.

Andersson (Reference Andersson1984), Brockmann (Reference Brockmann, Krebs and Davies1984), and Emlen (Reference Emlen1994; Reference Emlen1995; Reference Emlen1997) provide important reviews of family evolution. The study of family life first became based in evolutionary science with Hamilton's (Reference Hamilton1964) realization that an individual's fitness can be more than the individual's phenotypic design for production of direct descendant relatives – that is, more than the individual's classical or Darwinian fitness. Hence, one's fitness can include nepotistic design for increasing the offspring production of non-descendant kin, such as siblings, cousins, nieces, and nephews. However, this inclusive fitness theory of fitness striving, a major part of the modern theory of social life, does not account for why nepotism is variable across social systems. Why is nepotism limited to the nuclear family in many systems but extended beyond the nuclear family in others? We provide in this article evidence that parasite-stress accounts for this variation across human social systems. The parasite-stress theory of sociality suggests a general theory of family life across animal taxa.

The social organization of animal species varies along a cooperative breeding continuum, or, said differently, a continuum of eusociality (Andersson Reference Andersson1984; Sherman et al. Reference Sherman, Lacey, Reeve and Keller1995). A mother alone investing in her offspring, or much less commonly across species, a father alone investing in offspring, are on the highest asociality end of the continuum. Species in which both mother and father nepotistically assist the offspring (so-called biparental species) are more eusocial in degree on the continuum. This is followed by varying degrees of extended nepotism outside the nuclear family. Sterility – shown by adult members of the group who serve as helpers and thereby assist relatives, or in some cases, non-relatives, instead of producing their own offspring – occurs in certain taxa of vertebrates, including the human species, as well as in invertebrates. The sterility ranges from temporary to permanent, depending on the species. The temporary case is that of delayed striving to produce descendant kin while being engaged, instead, in in-group altruism (e.g., certain human groups [Hill & Hurtado 2009] and certain species of wasps, birds, and carnivores). The permanent case is one of life-long, exclusive, extended nepotism (as is characteristic in ants and termites). Both temporary and permanent cases constitute in-group assortative sociality. Also, the relatively eusocial species on the continuum – that is, the more cooperative in terms of group breeding – in general exhibit marked sedentism, delayed or no dispersal from the natal location, and territorial defense by the family group or, in some cases, by a larger in-group (e.g., Arnold & Owens Reference Arnold and Owens1998). According to the parasite-stress model, the sedentism and limited dispersal are analogs (similarity by independent evolution by selection, i.e., convergent evolution), or, in some cases, homologs (similarity resulting from common ancestry) of human philopatry. The territoriality is the analog or homolog, depending on the comparison, of human xenophobia.

At a minimum, the parasite-stress theory applied to family evolution is supported by the fact that cooperative breeding in birds and eusocial systems in insects are more common, or, in the case of eusociality, more eusocial, in tropical regions than in temperate regions for many different taxa (e.g., birds [Brown Reference Brown1987; Ekman Reference Ekman2006; Blumstein & Møller Reference Blumstein and Møller2008], wasps [Wilson Reference Wilson1971]).

6.2. Religiosity

As predicted, our cross-national analysis showed that religiosity, as measured by religious affiliation and religious participation and value, was positively correlated with all measures of parasite-stress; and religiosity was correlated more strongly with the prevalence of nonzoonotic infectious diseases than with zoonotic infectious diseases. Within the United States, too, religiosity was also correlated positively with parasite-stress. Our results are consistent with the hypothesis that religiosity functions as an honest signal (see sect. 3.2). Costly signaling theory emphasizes the necessity of high-cost signals in order to reliably communicate information. Religiosity's costs allow honest communication of in-group allegiance, as well as provide a social boundary that protects adherents from out-groups that may possess new infectious agents and different values and norms. As expected from costly-signaling theory, the degree of religiosity – and, thereby, the extent of its costs – covaries positively with parasite-stress across regions. Hence, the parasite-stress theory of sociality in conjunction with costly-signaling theory can potentially explain all forms of religiosity, from the irreligious to the ultra-religious, as arising from the relative position of cultures along a parasite-stress gradient and corresponding collectivism-individualism gradient.

Although we found that the multiple religiosity variables were correlated with societal wealth variables, as predicted by the conditions-of-living model in Inglehart and Baker (Reference Inglehart and Baker2000), Norris and Inglehart (Reference Norris and Inglehart2004), and McCleary and Barro (Reference McCleary and Barro2006), the effect of parasite-stress for explaining variation in religiosity was still significant when the effects of societal wealth and resource inequality were removed. Furthermore, parasite-stress was significant after removing the effects of differences in freedoms, as measured by the Freedom House Civil Liberty scores. This was as predicted. However, societal wealth, resource inequality, and freedom are hardly separable from parasite-stress. This is because the values that lead to democracies versus autocracies or wealth versus dearth appear to causally arise from different infectious diseases ecologies (Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009). The long-standing tradition in economics, historical scholarship, political science, and sociology is to view economic measures, such as GDP as an encompassing causal factor. However, according to the parasite-stress theory of sociality, variation in parasite-stress is a proximate cause of variation in GDP and in resource inequality. Parasite-stress not only strongly and negatively influences human labor capacity (e.g., Price-Smith Reference Price-Smith2002), but also, the undemocratic values parasite-stress generates cause widespread economic dearth and inequality. High parasite-stress yields philopatry and localized/ethnocentric economic priorities and investment, devaluation and divestment outside the dominant in-group, and limitations of innovation, willingness to adopt new ideas and technologies, and private property rights. Low parasite-stress has the opposite effects. Hence, these effects of parasite-stress level manifest cross-nationally as economic variables (see Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009; also Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008; Schaller & Murray Reference Schaller and Murray2008).

Norris and Inglehart (Reference Norris and Inglehart2004) describe patterns of secularization (the declining religiosity in many countries of the world). They note that religiosity has declined most in wealthy nations but very little if any in poor nations. They attribute this to the importance of the lack of wealth (poor living conditions) for promoting heightened religiosity. We note that the poorest nations are also those that have the highest parasite-stress. This is evident in the negative correlation between GDP per capita and Combined Parasite-Stress (r=−.77, n=184, p < .0001). We hypothesize that the reason that the poorest nations have maintained similar levels of high religiosity over time is because of the salience placed on tradition, conformity, and other in-group values, which in turn are caused by high parasite-stress. We tested this by cross-national comparison of the religiosity of people born before 1945 versus during or after 1945. This temporal break is very meaningful in terms of the marked reduction in parasite-stress in regions with medical interventions such as widespread antibiotic availability, child vaccination programs, and disease-vector control (Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009). We used one question from the World Values Survey that is a component of our cross-national Religious Participation and Value measure (rating the importance of God from 1 to 10). We then subtracted the proportion of those born in 1945 and later who rated their response a 10 from the proportion of those born before 1945 who rated their response a 10 within each country, and then correlated this difference with Combined Parasite-Stress. The correlation was significantly negative (r=−.32, n=91, p=.0022), indicating that in the countries with high pathogen-stress there was very little difference between those born before or after 1945 in how they felt about God's importance; whereas, in countries with low parasite-stress there was a significantly larger difference between people born before 1945 and those born more recently in how they perceived God's importance.

It is clear that religiousness, religious identities, and beliefs form at early ages (Finkel et al. Reference Finkel, Swartwout and Sosis2009). It is also clear that people develop an awareness of pathogens at relatively early ages (Siegal Reference Siegal1988). Their correspondence suggests an important aspect of the developmental ecology of values and ideology. It is unknown at this point what cues are relevant, but they may include infection frequency experienced by individuals (Stevenson et al. Reference Stevenson, Case and Oaten2009). We discussed various potential ontogenetic causes of values earlier in the article (see sect. 2.1).

6.3. In-group assortative sociality

Taken together, the findings for strong family ties and heightened religiosity in the face of parasite threat provide further support for the parasite-stress theory of sociality. We argue that in-group assortative sociality represents an adaptive response to heightened parasite-stress. Furthermore, we can add variation in the strength of family ties and religiosity to the list of features of human sociality requiring an explanation couched in the parasite-stress model of sociality. The predictability and apparent universality of this adaptive response suggests that the adaptive response of in-group assortative sociality in the face of parasite-stress is an ancient feature in Homo sapiens. Furthermore, the indications that similar types of adaptive responses are found in other animals (Freeland Reference Freeland1976; Loehle Reference Loehle1995) suggest that parasites had important impacts upon social life in deep-time evolutionary history.

The social isolation of groups under high parasite-stress is not without costs to individuals in the groups. For example, under conditions of social isolation significant inbreeding can take place, possibly generating inbreeding depression. However, adaptive inbreeding is possible (Shields Reference Shields1982; Kokko & Ots Reference Kokko and Ots2006). This seems especially likely under the ecology of high infectious disease stress, as mentioned in section 2.1 regarding the effects of malaria (Denic & Nicholls Reference Denic and Nicholls2007; Denic et al. Reference Denic, Nagelkerke and Agarwal2008a; Reference Denic, Nagelkerke and Agarwal2008b), and perhaps infectious disease stress considered more broadly (Hoben et al. Reference Hoben, Buunk, Fincher, Thornhill and Schaller2010). Social isolation can also limit access to trade with out-groups, innovations generated by out-groups, and out-group social alliances. Out-group interaction and affiliation, as we have explained, is a benefit of individualism/liberalism but one that is only widely optimal under relatively low parasite-stress.

Our findings that infectious disease stress promotes in-group assortative sociality can potentially inform the study of epidemiology or spread of infectious diseases. For example, it is the case that the prevalence of many types of parasites is greater in large, or more dense, populations (Altizer et al. Reference Altizer, Nunn, Thrall, Gittleman, Antonovics, Cunningham, Dobson, Ezenwa, Jones, Pedersen, Poss and Pulliam2003; Guégan & Constantin de Magny Reference Guégan, Constantin De Magny and Tibayrenc2007). At the same time, under conditions of high parasite-stress, groups are comparatively more isolated (via in-group assortative sociality) than groups in conditions of low parasite-stress. This suggests that an important consideration for understanding parasite transmission is to recognize the difference between out-group versus in-group contact. Contact rates between individuals may be high within a group that is socially isolated from other groups. This is indeed an implication from the research presented here. Thus, high rates of contact in low pathogen areas are different from high contact rates in high pathogen areas. Based on our research, in low pathogen areas (individualistic locales), a high contact rate implies high rates of contact between genetically different, and differently infected individuals, whereas high contact rates in high pathogen areas (collectivistic locales) occur between individuals that are genetically close and likely carry similar infectious diseases.

6.3.1. In-group assortative sociality and life-history

Gladden et al. (Reference Gladden, Welch, Figueredo and Jacobs2009) explored the interactions of religiosity, moral intuitions, and life-history patterns. They showed that both the strength of moral intuitions (automatic emotional reactions brought on by norm and other rule violations) and religiosity result from a slow life-history strategy. That is, both are signs of a life-history strategy focused on somatic investment or investment in survival, in contrast to a focus on reproductive effort. Gladden et al. suggested their findings were consistent with the fact that pathogen-stress and collectivism were positively related (Fincher et al. Reference Fincher, Thornhill, Murray and Schaller2008), presumably because much of moral intuitions taps into cognition about pathogens (Oaten et al. Reference Oaten, Stevenson and Case2009). In other work, Figueredo and Wolf (Reference Figueredo and Wolf2009) showed that slow life-history people assortatively pair, sexually and socially, more strongly than fast life-history strategists.

Both sets of findings are consistent with what we have presented here, that in-group assortative sociality is strongly and positively associated with pathogen stress.

Nevertheless, whenever parasite-stress is extremely high, collectivistic, nepotistic investment may not be optimal because the extreme parasite-stress yields extrinsic mortality (see Quinlan Reference Quinlan2007 and references therein). Because extrinsic mortality, by definition, cannot be reduced by nepotistic investment, comparatively low investment per offspring is predicted from life-history theory when extrinsic mortality is high. Therefore, we hypothesize that there will be reduced nepotistic investment in offspring and other kin in the face of extremely high parasite-stress because of the inability of nepotistic investment to reduce the morbidity and mortality associated with this high level of ecological stress. In this situation, early reproduction with minimum nepotistic investment per family member (e.g., offspring) is optimal according to life-history theory (Charnov Reference Charnov1993; Kaplan & Gangestad Reference Kaplan, Gangestad and Buss2005) and predicts a curvilinear relationship between parasite-stress and nepotistic investment. Consistent with this, Quinlan (Reference Quinlan2007) found when he examined a sample of traditional societies, that maternal investment in the form of nursing duration increased along with pathogen stress but then began to decrease after pathogen stress became extreme (i.e., he found a curvilinear relationship).

We hypothesize that the same pattern will be seen in human value systems as well. The current study provides some support for this hypothesis. When focusing on the world regions, the correlation between In-Group Assortativeness and Combined Parasite-Stress in Africa was negative (−.31), instead of positive as in the other five world regions (full results available from Fincher). This means that the people in the African countries expressed less in-group assortativeness as parasite-stress increased, rather than more in-group assortativeness as did the people in other world regions. We explain this unusual result by the fact that parasite-stress is exceptionally high in Africa – and therefore generally yields extrinsic mortality – as compared to the other world regions. (A post-hoc Tukey HSD means-test showed Africa to be distinctly high in pathogen-stress: Africa, mean Combined Parasite-Stress=3.36, A [world areas not followed by the same letter are significantly different]; South America, M=.85, B; East Eurasia, M=.53, BC; North America, M=−.51, BC; Insular Pacific, M=−.65, C; West Eurasia, M=−2.28, D.) Therefore, the people in Africa are unable to ameliorate the impact of parasite-stress to their fitness through nepotistic investment, and, instead, evoke a fast life-history strategy. Presumably, the level and nature of the parasite-stress in other world regions is such that individuals are able to mediate it through investment in maintaining strong family-ties and other forms of in-group assortative sociality (it is intrinsic rather than extrinsic mortality).

6.4. Future directions

One limitation of our research reported herein is that the empirical tests of the parasite-stress theory's application to family values and religiosity were at the macro-scale across countries of the world, or on a finer but still large scale within a single polity, the USA. It would be useful to conduct additional tests of this application within more localized regions. One such method of testing the hypotheses is to record people's changes in religiosity and family values after infectious disease levels are reduced locally (e.g. by greater access to modern medicine and safe water) or increased locally (e.g. by the emergence of a new infectious disease). Evidence we have discussed herein indicates that changes in people's values can occur immediately (sect. 2.1.) and may change and stabilize across one or a few generations (see Thornhill et al. Reference Thornhill, Fincher and Aran2009). Easily administered, brief, valid questionnaires that could measure the relevant value changes are available (this study; Faulkner et al. Reference Faulkner, Schaller, Park and Duncan2004; Gelfand et al. Reference Gelfand, Bhawuk, Nishii, Bechtold, House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta2004; Thornhill & Fincher Reference Thornhill and Fincher2007). At another, micro-scale, we predict that a questionnaire study would find that religious commitment within churches and between churches in a restricted region such as a United States county or city will correlate positively with individual differences in perceived vulnerability to disease, philopatry, involvement with extended family, and collectivism, and will correlate negatively with individuals' recent history of infectious disease and the two factors of personality, openness and extraversion. We predict, too, that disgust and contamination sensitivity will covary positively with religious commitment and its covariates, and that these sensitivities will covary negatively with a recent history of infectious disease.

The parasite-stress theory of sociality seems to offer many other avenues for exploration. For example, we compiled a cross-national measure we call Strength of National Ties. This measure taps into the value placed on an individual for adopting the customs of, being born in, and/or having ancestors from, a particular country in order to make a claim of citizenship (ES 1.J and data are in ES 2). The Strength of National Ties was correlated positively with Combined Parasite-Stress (r=.71, n=40 countries, p < .0001). This relationship could be studied more thoroughly to explore the foundations of nationalism and other similar cultural features. Also, xenophobic attitudes cross-nationally seem to be related positively to pathogen-stress (as expected from the parasite-stress theory of sociality). For example, participants in the World Values Survey were asked about different types of people that they would not want as a neighbor. The proportion of those that said they did not want to live next to someone of a different race was positively associated with Combined Parasite-Stress (r=.35, n=88 countries, p=.0009; see also Schaller & Murray 2010). Other similar questions are posed in the World Values Survey with similar relationships to Combined Parasite-Stress (e.g., proportion not wanting to live next to someone that speaks a different language: r=.42, n=44 countries, p=.0044).

Colonialism, imperialism, large-scale intergroup conquest, and related forms of societal expansion have large benefits (primarily reaped by elites) in the acquisition of land and other resources, and the enslavement of conquered people. During such events, expansionists often coercively force their value systems on the original inhabitants of the acquired region. Typically, this involves committed and encompassing efforts by the conquerors, with religious beliefs being central to ideological reformation. This colonialist effort in part, we hypothesize, is to spread and enforce the conquerors' behavioral norms that reduce the cost of the conquest to the imperialists. If the conquered have the same value system as the conquerors, then the cost of the conquest, in terms of contagion risk, is reduced, allowing sustained intergroup contact. Accordingly, the coercive spread of values (notably religious ideology) is a means of reducing the costs of conquest (costs of encountering new infectious diseases) to the point that the benefits of conquest exceed these costs. Additional research could examine our hypothesis' application to conquest events in the historical record.

A related issue is the geographical pattern of large-scale historical conquests by Eurasian imperialism, described by Diamond (Reference Diamond1998), whose thesis focused on unique aspects of geography, such as the east–west orientation of the Eurasian continent and the distribution of domesticable animals and plants. We (with Kenneth Letendre) have suggested a complementary, and in part alternative, model for this history (Letendre et al. Reference Letendre, Fincher and Thornhill2010). First, conservative and collectivist values are correlated positively with severity of infectious disease. Second, such values include parochialism and associated closedness toward innovations. Third, collectivism is concentrated at low latitudes. Fourth, collectivism is related negatively with societal wealth and associated technology. Hence, we have argued that, as humans migrated from Africa to higher latitudes in Eurasia, they moved into climates less hospitable to human infectious diseases, which, in turn, generated relatively individualistic cultures that have an increased openness to and value of innovation and which place a positive value on long-range dispersal. Thus, the accumulation of wealth and technology, the domestication of plants and animals, and the large population sizes that enabled the imperial domination of otherwise impoverished and less innovative cultures resulted not from aspects of Eurasian geography, but from the relative emancipation from parasites, which allowed and promoted the rise of cultures that were more individualistic than their forebearers'. The technological dominance and individualism of these cultures motivated their expansion to obtain the benefits of conquest of other peoples, and the enforcement of the conquerors' cultural values reduced the contagion-related costs of contact with out-groups.

Vigil (Reference Vigil2009) presented a model (the socio-relational framework of expressive behaviors) for the evolution of the expression of emotion. Based on Vigil's model, many aspects of the behaviors we discuss here need also be considered in the light of their evolved function as expressed emotions. For example, in high-parasite-stress regions where maintaining strong family-ties is paramount, perhaps in-group submissive behaviors would be emphasized strategically. Or, maybe, the within-individual variation (e.g., across the life span) in emotional expression of in-group assortative sociality could be explained by changes in capacity-traits across the life span (capacity-traits include features such as the ability to provide material or social resources). Perhaps individuals express in-group biases at points in their life when they are less capable but express more out-group bias during stages when they are more capable. Similarly, this thinking may apply to individual differences in phenotypic and genetic quality. Certainly, Vigil's socio-relational framework offers an avenue for further exploration of in-group assortative sociality on an individual level.

Throughout this target article we have treated ethnocentrism and xenophobia as though they are always positively associated. However, xenophobia and ethnocentrism can arise from separate causes leading to cases where they may be uncorrelated (Brewer Reference Brewer1999; Cashdan Reference Cashdan2001b). Cashdan (Reference Cashdan2001b) demonstrated that ethnocentrism was high in traditional societies that experienced catastrophic food shortage, while xenophobia was high where the threat of intergroup violence was high. Further extension of the parasite-stress model of sociality can provide a basis for making more refined predictions about the patterns of xenophobia and ethnocentrism. For example, in a given area zoonotic diseases may generate high mortality. In this setting, ethnocentrism is predicted to be high but xenophobia low because zoonotic infections are not transmitted between human hosts.

A large literature indicates that the relationships between religiosity and mental health and freedom from coronary disease and certain cancers typically are positive (George et al. Reference George, Ellison and Larson2002; Koenig Reference Koenig1997). Future research could focus on the covariation of religiosity and infectious diseases per se. According to the parasite-stress model, religiosity will reduce recent infectious disease problems via its associated ethnocentrism and xenophobia. As George et al. (Reference George, Ellison and Larson2002) emphasize, despite a great deal of research, little is known about the mediators of the positive relationship between religiosity and health. Our approach suggests that the relationship between infectious diseases and religion will be mediated by collectivism/conservatism and related values, and by disgust and contamination sensitivity. Although high disease severity in childhood, according to the parasite-stress model, is expected to produce high in-group assortative sociality and emotionality, once those values are acquired ontogenetically, they will reduce the incidence of recent infectious diseases. Such research would add a new empirical approach to the study of the relationship between health and religiosity.

Lastly, we acknowledge that our treatment of religiosity has ignored many important aspects of religion, such as beliefs in the afterlife and attribution to supernatural causation. We have deliberately focused on features such as religious affiliation and commitment which have been measured comparably across all kinds of people, including the areligious and irreligious. The predictive power of the parasite-stress theory does not end with these features. We predict that certain unique elements of religion may be disentangled with an eye towards the human history of contending with parasites. For example, ancestor worship is a widespread component of many religions but variation in its extent and nature does exist (Rossano Reference Rossano2007). We predict that the strength of family ties of the living, which is caused by the degree of parasite-stress, could provide a foundation for the strength of worship of ancestors.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS

Electronic Supplement 1: http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012001

Electronic Supplement 2: http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012002

Electronic Supplement 3: http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012003

Electronic Supplement 4: http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012004

Electronic Supplement 5: http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012005

Electronic Supplement 6: http://www.journals.cambridge.org/bbs2012006

Parasite stress is not so critical to the history of religions or major modern group formations Atran, Scott UMR 8129, CNRS / Institut Jean Nicod – Ecole Normale Supérieure, 75005 Paris, France. http://sitemaker.umich.edu/satran/home Connecting biological concepts and religious behavior Beit-Hallahmi, Benjamin Department of Psychology, University of Haifa, Haifa 31905, Israel. Condition-dependent adaptive phenotypic plasticity and interspecific gene-culture coevolution Blute, Marion Department of Sociology, University of Toronto at Mississauga, Mississauga, ON L5L 1C6, Canada. http://individual.utoronto.ca/marionblute/ In-group loyalty or out-group avoidance? Isolating the links between pathogens and in-group assortative sociality Cashdan, Elizabeth Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84112-0060. http://www.anthro.utah.edu/faculty/elizabeth-cashdan.html Pathogens promote matrilocal family ties and the copying of foreign religions Chang, Lei, Lu, Hui Jing and Wu, Bao Pei Department of Educational Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, N.T., Hong Kong, Special Administrative Region, People's Republic of China. http://www.fed.cuhk.edu.hk/eps/people/changl.html Analyses do not support the parasite-stress theory of human sociality Currie, Thomas E. and Mace, Ruth Human Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Anthropology, University College London, London WC1H 0BW, United Kingdom. http://www.ucl.ac.uk/~ucsatec http://www.ucl.ac.uk/anthropology/staff/r_mace http://www.ucl.ac.uk/heeg Are the pathogens of out-groups really more dangerous? de Barra, Mícheál and Curtis, Val The Hygiene Centre, Department of Disease Control, Faculty of Infectious and Tropical Diseases, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London WC1E 7HT, United Kingdom. http://www.lshtm.ac.uk/people/debarra.micheal http://www.lshtm.ac.uk/people/curtis.val Extending parasite-stress theory to variation in human mate preferences DeBruine, Lisa M., Little, Anthony C. and Jones, Benedict C. School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, AB25 3FX, Scotland, United Kingdom. http://facelab.org/debruine http://facelab.org/bcjones; School of Natural Sciences, University of Stirling, Stirling, FK9 4LA Scotland, United Kingdom. http://alittlelab.com Parasite stress, ethnocentrism, and life history strategy Figueredo, Aurelio José, Gladden, Paul Robert and Black, Candace Jasmine Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, and School of Mind, Brain, and Behavior, College of Science, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721-0068. http://www.u.arizona.edu/~ajf http://www.u.arizona.edu/~cjblack; Department of Psychology and Sociology, Macon State College, Macon, GA 31206. http://vista.maconstate.edu/directory/detailsprint.aspx?id=612 The evolution and development of human social systems requires more than parasite-stress avoidance explanation Grotuss, Jason Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL 33431. Coping with germs and people: Investigating the link between pathogen threat and human social cognition Navarrete, Carlos David Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824. www.cdnresearch.net High illness loads (physical and social) do not always force high levels of mass religiosity Paul, Gregory S. 3109 N. Calvert Street, Baltimore, MD 21218. An ethical and prudential argument for prioritizing the reduction of parasite-stress in the allocation of health care resources Powell, Russell, Clarke, Steve and Savulescu, Julian Science and Religious Conflict Project, Oxford Uehiro Centre for Practical Ethics and Institute for Science and Ethics, Oxford Martin School and Faculty of Philosophy, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1-1PT, United Kingdom. http://www.practicalethics.ox.ac.uk/staff http://www.src.ox.ac.uk/staff.htm Mechanisms by which parasites influence cultures, and why they matter Schaller, Mark and Murray, Damian R. Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada. http://neuron4.psych.ubc.ca/~schallerlab/ Form and function in religious signaling under pathogen stress Swartwout, Paul, Purzycki, Benjamin Grant and Sosis, Richard Department of Anthropology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269-2176. http://www.anth.uconn.edu/faculty/sosis/ Rethinking innovative designs to further test parasite-stress theory Uskul, Ayse K. Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester CO4 3SQ, United Kingdom. http://www.essex.ac.uk/psychology/department/people/uskul.html Climato-economic livability predicts societal collectivism and political autocracy better than parasitic stress does Van de Vliert, Evert and Postmes, Tom Department of Psychology, University of Groningen, 9712 TS Groningen, The Netherlands http://www.rug.nl/staff/e.van.de.vliert/index; Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, NO-5015 Bergen, Norway. http://www.rug.nl/staff/t.postmes/index Parasite-stress, cultures of honor, and the emergence of gender bias in purity norms Vandello, Joseph A. and Hettinger, Vanessa E. Department of Psychology, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL 33620. http://psychology.usf.edu/faculty/vandello/ Intra-regional assortative sociality may be better explained by social network dynamics rather than pathogen risk avoidance Vigil, Jacob M. and Coulombe, Patrick Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131-1161. http://www.unm.edu/~psych/faculty/sm_vigil.html Immigration, parasitic infection, and United States religiosity Wall, Jaimie N. and Shackelford, Todd K. Department of Psychology, Oakland University, Pryale Hall, Rochester, MI 48309. http://www.ToddKShackelford.com Time allocation, religious observance, and illness in Mayan horticulturalists Waynforth, David The Norwich Medical School, University of East Anglia, Norwich NR4 7TJ, United Kingdom. http://www.uea.ac.uk/med/People/Academic/David+Waynforth#info The parasite-stress theory may be a general theory of culture and sociality Fincher, Corey L. and Thornhill, Randy Department of Biology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131. http://biology.unm.edu/fincher http://biology.unm.edu/Thornhill/rthorn.htm

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Thanks to Candace Black, Paul Bloom, Tomas Cabeza de Baca, Christopher Eppig, Aurelio José Figueredo, Paul Gladden, Kenneth Letendre, Jacob Vigil, Paul Watson, and five anonymous referees for their criticism. Thanks to Devaraj Aran, John Branch, Keith Davis, Hannah Doran, Phuong-Dung Le, Lindsay Livingstone, Brian Malott, Pooneh Soltani, Brandon Rice, Megan Thienel, and Mary Walker for their assistance with data processing and library tasking. Thanks to Sam Loker for confirming our schistosome transmission classification.

Footnotes

1. An additional model of religiosity that has received much attention in the literature, called the supply-side model, suggests that religious vitality (typically measured by some aspect of religiosity) is positively associated with religious pluralism because the increased commodity possibilities under high religious pluralism allows for an individual to better find the religion that suits him or her best (see, e.g., Finke & Stark Reference Finke and Stark1988). Because people can find such great fits, they will tend to engage in greater religious behavior, leading to the prediction of a positive association between religiosity and religious pluralism. This model was supported with some empirical patterns but was largely dismissed by Chaves and Gorski (Reference Chaves and Gorski2001) on the grounds that the empirical evidence was overwhelmingly unsupportive of the basic general contention that religious pluralism was positively associated with religious vitality. For completeness, we correlated each of our three cross-national measures of religiosity described in section 4.2 with the religious pluralism index produced by McCleary and Barro (Reference McCleary and Barro2006) (Proportion of Religionists: r=−.14, n=67, p=.2553; Proportion of Believers: r=−.02, n=63, p=.8959; Religious Participation and Value: r=.05, n=63, p=.6920). These patterns do not support the supply-side model.

2. Our cross-national analyses were of geopolitical regions that maintain a separate government (e.g., United States, Hong Kong). We refer to them as countries or nations.

3. Our United States analyses did not include the District of Columbia.

References

Alesina, A. & Giuliano, P. (2007) The power of the family. Discussion paper, No. 2132. Harvard Institute of Economic Research, Harvard University. Available at: http://post.economics.harvard.edu/hier/2007papers/2007list.html.Google Scholar
Alexander, R. D. (1979) Darwinism and human affairs. University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Altizer, S., Nunn, C. L., Thrall, P. H., Gittleman, J. L., Antonovics, J., Cunningham, A. A., Dobson, A. P., Ezenwa, V., Jones, K. E., Pedersen, A. B., Poss, M. & Pulliam, J. R. C. (2003) Social organization and parasite risk in mammals: Integrating theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics 34:517–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R. M. & May, R. M. (1991) Infectious disease of humans: Dynamics and control. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersson, M. (1984) The evolution of eusociality. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 15:165–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W. & Matthews, D. (2002) Adaptationism – how to carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25:489504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, K. E. & Owens, I. P. F. (1998) Cooperative breeding in birds: A comparative test of the life history hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 265:739–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azzi, C. & Ehrenberg, R. (1975) Household allocation of time and church attendance. Journal of Political Economy 83:2756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnea, M. F. & Schwartz, S. H. (1998) Values and voting. Political Psychology 19:1740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, D. B., Kurian, G. T. & Johnson, T. M., ed. (2001) World Christian encyclopedia: A comparative survey of churches and religions in the modern world, vol. 1: The world by countries: Religionists, churches, ministries, 2nd edition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Billing, J. & Sherman, P. W. (1998) Antimicrobial functions of spices: Why some like it hot. Quarterly Review of Biology 73:349.Google Scholar
Blumstein, D. T. & Møller, A. P. (2008) Is sociality associated with high longevity in North American birds? Biology Letters 4:146–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001) Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B. (1999) The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues 55:429–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brockmann, H. J. (1984) The evolution of social behaviour in insects. In: Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B., pp. 340–61. Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Brown, J. L. (1987) Helping and communal breeding in birds: Ecology and evolution. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bulbulia, J. (2004a) Religious costs as adaptations that signal altruistic intention. Evolution and Cognition 10:1942.Google Scholar
Bulbulia, J. (2004b) The cognitive and evolutionary psychology of religion. Biology and Philosophy 19:655–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bulbulia, J. & Mahoney, A. (2008) Religious solidarity: The hand grenade experiment. Journal of Cognition and Culture 8:295320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cashdan, E. (2001a) Ethnic diversity and its environmental determinants: Effects of climate, pathogens, and habitat diversity. American Anthropologist 103:968–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cashdan, E. (2001b) Ethnocentrism and xenophobia: A cross-cultural study. Current Anthropology 42(5):760–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. & Feldman, M. (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Charnov, E. L. (1993) Life history invariants: Some explanations of symmetry in evolutionary ecology. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaves, M. & Gorski, P. S. (2001) Religious pluralism and religious participation. Annual Review of Sociology 27:261–81.Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y. & Blizinsky, K. D. (2010) Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 277:529–37.Google ScholarPubMed
Corby-Harris, V. & Promislow, D. E. L. (2008) Host ecology shapes geographical variation for resistance to bacterial infection in Drosophila melanogaster . Journal of Animal Ecology 77(4):768–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, D. H. (2007) Deadly companions: How microbes shaped our history. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (1994) Evolutionary-theories of morality and the manipulative use of signals. Zygon 29:81101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtis, V. A. (2007) Dirt, disgust and disease: A natural history of hygiene. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 61:660–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, V. A., Aunger, R. & Rabie, T. (2004) Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B (Supplement) 271:1731.Google ScholarPubMed
Delamontagne, R. (2010) High religiosity and societal dysfunction in the United States during the first decade of the twenty-first century. Evolutionary Psychology 8:617–57. Available at: www.epjournal.net/filestore/EP08617657_c.pdf.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denic, S., Nagelkerke, N. & Agarwal, M. M. (2008a) Consanguineous marriages and endemic malaria: Can inbreeding increase population fitness? Malaria Journal 7:150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denic, S., Nagelkerke, N. & Agarwal, M. M. (2008b) Consanguineous marriages: Do genetic benefits outweigh its costs in populations with alpha(+)-thalassemia, hemoglobin s, and malaria? Evolution and Human Behavior 29:364–69.Google Scholar
Denic, S. & Nicholls, M. G. (2007) Genetic benefits of consanguinity through selection of genotypes protective against malaria. Human Biology 79:145158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, J. (1998) Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Dionne, M., Miller, K. M., Dodson, J. J., Caron, F. & Bernatchez, L. (2007) Clinical variation in MHC diversity with temperature: Evidence for the role of host-pathogen interaction on local adaptation in Atlantic salmon. Evolution 61:2154–64.Google Scholar
Dobson, A. P. & Carper, E. R. (1996) Infectious diseases and human population history: throughout history the establishment of disease has been a side effect of the growth of civilization. Bioscience 46:115–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubos, R. (1980) The evolution of microbial diseases. In: Man adapting, ed. Dubos, R., pp. 163–95. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Dunn, R. R., Davies, T. J., Harris, N. C. & Gavin, M. C. (2010) Global drivers of human pathogen richness and prevalence. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B 277:2587–95.Google Scholar
Ekman, J. (2006) Family living among birds. Journal of Avian Biology 37:289–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emlen, S. T. (1994) Benefits, constraints and the evolution of the family. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 9:282–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emlen, S. T. (1995) An evolutionary-theory of the family. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 92:8092–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emlen, S. T. (1997) The evolutionary study of human family systems. Social Science (Information Sur Les Sciences Sociales) 36:563–89.Google Scholar
Ewald, P. W. (1994) Evolution of infectious disease. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H. & Duncan, L. A. (2004) Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations 7:333–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figueredo, A. J. & Wolf, P. S. A. (2009) Assortative pairing and life history strategy: A cross-cultural study. Human Nature 20:317330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincher, C. L. & Thornhill, R. (2008a) A parasite-driven wedge: Infectious diseases may explain language and other biodiversity. Oikos 117:1289–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincher, C. L. & Thornhill, R. (2008b) Assortative sociality, limited dispersal: Infectious disease and the genesis of the global patterns of religious diversity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 275:2587–94.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R. & Schaller, M. (2008) Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B 275:1279–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finke, R. & Scheitle, C. P. (2005) Accounting for the uncounted: Computing correctives for the 2000 RCMS data. Review of Religious Research 47:522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finke, R. & Stark, R. (1988) Religious economies and sacred canopies: Religious mobilization in American cities, 1906. American Sociological Review 53:4149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkel, D. N., Swartwout, P. & Sosis, R. (2009) The socio-religious brain: A developmental model. Proceedings of the British Academy 158:287312.Google Scholar
Flinn, M. V. (1997) Culture and the evolution of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior 18:2367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flinn, M. V. & Coe, K. (2007) The linked red queens of human cognition, reciprocity, and culture. In: The evolution of mind, ed. Gangestad, S. W. & Simpson, J. A., pp. 339–47. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Floate, K. D., Kearsley, M. J. C. & Whitham, T. G. (1993) Elevated herbivory in plant hybrid zones – Chrysomela confluens, Populus, and phenological sinks. Ecology 74:2056–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeland, W. J. (1976) Pathogens and the evolution of primate sociality. Biotropica 8:1224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeland, W. J. (1979) Primate social-groups as biological islands. Ecology 60:719–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G. & Buss, D. M. (2006a) Evolutionary foundations of cultural variation: Evoked culture and mate preferences. Psychological Inquiry 17:7595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G. & Buss, D. M. (2006b) Toward an integrative understanding of evoked and transmitted culture: The importance of specialized psychological design. Psychological Inquiry 17:138–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Bhawuk, D. P. S., Nishii, L. H. & Bechtold, D. J. (2004) Individualism and collectivism. In: Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies, ed. House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W. & Gupta, V., pp. 437512. Sage.Google Scholar
Georgas, J., Mylonas, K., Bafiti, T., Poortinga, Y. H., Christakopoulou, S., Kagitcibasi, C., Kwak, K., Ataca, B., Berry, J., Orung, S., Sunar, D., Charalambous, N., Goodwin, R., Wang, W. Z., Angleitner, A., Stepanikova, I., Pick, S., Givaudan, M., Zhuravliova-Gionis, I., Konantambigi, R., Gelfand, M. J., Marinova, V., Mcbride-Chang, C. & Kodic, Y. (2001) Functional relationships in the nuclear and extended family: A 16-culture study. International Journal of Psychology 36:289300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, L. K., Ellison, C. G. & Larson, D. B. (2002) Explaining the relationships between religious involvement and health. Psychological Inquiry 13:190200.Google Scholar
Gladden, P. R., Welch, J., Figueredo, A. J. & Jacobs, W. J. (2009) Moral intuitions and religiosity as spuriously correlated life history traits. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology 7(2):167–84.Google Scholar
Good, C. M. (1972) Salt, trade, and disease – aspects of development in Africa's northern Great Lakes region. International Journal of African Historical Studies 5:543–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grim, B. J. & Finke, R. (2006) International religion indexes: Government regulation, government favoritism, and social regulation of religion. Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion 2:140.Google ScholarPubMed
Guégan, J.-F. & Constantin De Magny, G. (2007) Epidemiology in a changing world: The need for a bigger picture! In: Encyclopedia of infectious diseases: Modern methodologies, ed. Tibayrenc, M., pp. 569–89. John Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guernier, V., Hochberg, M. F. & Guégan, J.-F. (2004) Ecology drives the worldwide distribution of human diseases. PLoS Biology 2:740–46.Google Scholar
Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K. & Hurtado, M. (2000) ‘It's a wonderful life’: Signaling generosity among the Ache of Paraguay. Evolution and Human Behavior 21:263–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haldane, J. B. S. (1949) Disease and evolution. La Ricerca Scientifica Supplemento A 19:6876.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964) The genetical evolution of social behavior I-II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7:152.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. & Zuk, M. (1982) Heritable true fitness and bright birds: A role for parasites? Science 218:384–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. (2009) The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior 30:244–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. & Henrich, N. (2010) The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B 277:3715–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, K. & Hurtado, A. M. (2009) Cooperative breeding in South American hunter-gatherers. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B 276:3863–70.Google Scholar
Hoben, A. D., Buunk, A. P., Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R. & Schaller, M. (2010) On the adaptive origins and maladaptive consequences of human inbreeding: Pathogen prevalence, immune functioning, and consanguineous marriage. Evolutionary Psychology 8:658–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofstede, G. (2001) Culture's consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations, 2nd edition. Sage.Google Scholar
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W. & Gupta, V., eds, (2004) Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies. Sage.Google Scholar
Iannaccone, L. R. (1990) Religious practice, a human-capital approach. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 29:297314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iannaccone, L. R. (1994) Why strict churches are strong. American Journal of Sociology 99:1180–211.Google Scholar
Inglehart, R. & Baker, W. E. (2000) Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. American Sociological Review 65:1951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irons, W. (1996) In our own self image: The evolution of morality, deception, and religion. Skeptic 4:5061.Google Scholar
Irons, W. (2001) Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment. In: Evolution and the capacity for commitment, ed. Nesse, R. M., pp. 292309. Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Irons, W. (2008) Why people believe (what other people see as) crazy ideas. In: The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, and critiques, ed. Bulbulia, J., Sosis, R., Genet, C., Genet, R. & Wyman, K., pp. 5157. Collins Foundation Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, L. M. & Hunsberger, B. (1999) An intergroup perspective on religion and prejudice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 38:509523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, C., Dimitrakakis, M., Cook, I., Sanders, R. & Stallman, N. (1989) Culture change and epidemiological patterns among the Hagahai, Papua New Guinea. Human Ecology 17:2757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. (2008) Gods of war: The adaptive logic of religious conflict. In: The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, and critiques, ed. Bulbulia, J., Sosis, R., Genet, C., Genet, R., Harris, E. & Wyman, K., pp. 111–17. Collins Foundation Press.Google Scholar
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M. & Napier, J. L. (2009) Political ideology: Its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology 60:307–37.Google Scholar
Kaltz, O., Gandon, S., Michalakis, Y. & Shykoff, J. A. (1999) Local maladaptation in the anther-smut fungus Microbotryum violaceum to its host plant Silene latifolia: Evidence from a cross-inoculation experiment. Evolution 53(2):395407.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H. S. & Gangestad, S. W. (2005) Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In: The handbook of evolutionary psychology, ed. Buss, D. M., pp. 6895. John Wiley.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y. & Kashima, E. S. (2003) Individualism, GNP, climate, and pronoun drop – Is individualism determined by affluence and climate, or does language use play a role? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 34:125–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., Ishii, K., Imada, T., Takemura, K. & Ramaswamy, J. (2006) Voluntary settlement and the spirit of independence: Evidence from Japan's ‘northern frontier’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91:369–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenig, H. G. (1997) Is religion good for your health? Effects of religion on mental and physical health. Haworth Press.Google Scholar
Kokko, H. & Ots, I. (2006) When not to avoid inbreeding. Evolution 60:467–75.Google Scholar
Kosmin, B. A., Mayer, E. & Keysar, A. (2001) American Religious Identification Survey 2001, pp. 146. The Graduate Center of the City University of New York Google Scholar
Letendre, K., Fincher, C. L. & Thornhill, R. (2010) Does infectious disease cause global variation in the frequency of intrastate armed conflict and civil war? Biological Reviewes 85:669–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, K. (1998) Pathogen resistance as the origin of kin altruism. Journal of Theoretical Biology 193:359–63.Google Scholar
Loehle, C. (1995) Social barriers to pathogen transmission in wild animal populations. Ecology 76:326–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez, A. D., Mathers, C. D., Ezzati, M., Jamison, D. T. & Murray, C. J. L. (2006) Global and regional burden of disease and risk factors, 2001: Systematic analysis of population health data. Lancet 367:1747–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Low, B. S. (1990) Marriage systems and pathogen stress in human societies. American Zoologist 30:325–39.Google Scholar
Lumsden, C. J. & Wilson, E. O. (1981) Genes, mind and culture: The coevolutionary process. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lynn, R., Harvey, J. & Nyborg, H. (2009) Average intelligence predicts atheism rates across 137 nations. Intelligence 37:1115.Google Scholar
Mattausch, J. (in press) With whom may British Gujaratis identify, to whom may be compared? In: Global Gujaratis, ed. Mawani, S. & Mukadam, A.. Brill.Google Scholar
McCleary, R. M. & Barro, R. J. (2006) Religion and political economy in an international panel. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 45:149–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McElreath, R., Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (2003) Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology 44:122–29.Google Scholar
McNeill, W. H. (1980) Migration patterns and infection in traditional societies. In: Changing disease patterns and human behavior, ed. Stanley, N. F. & Joske, R. A., pp. 2736. Academic Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, W. H. (1998) Plagues and peoples. Anchor.Google Scholar
Miller, E. N., Fadl, M., Mohamed, H. S., Elzein, A., Jamieson, S. E., Cordell, H. J., Peacock, C. S., Fakiola, M., Madhuri, R., Eltahir, A. K., Elhassan, A., Musa, M. M., Muntaser, E. I. & Blackwell, J. M. (2007) Y chromosome lineage- and village-specific genes on chromosomes 1p22 and 6q27 control visceral leishmaniasis in Sudan. PLoS Genetics 3(5):679–88. e71.Google Scholar
Mortensen, C. R., Becker, D. V., Ackerman, J. M., Neuberg, S. L. & Kenrick, D. T. (2010) Infection breeds reticence: the effects of disease salience on self-perceptions of personality and behavioral avoidance tendencies. Psychological Science 21:440–47.Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1949) Social structure. MacMillan.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R. & Schaller, M. (2010) Historical prevalence of infectious diseases within 230 geopolitical regions: A tool for investigating origins of culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 41:99108.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., Trudeau, R. & Schaller, M. (2011) On the origins of cultural differences in conformity: Four tests of the pathogen prevalence hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37:318–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navarrete, C. D. & Fessler, D. M. T. (2006) Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evolution and Human Behavior 27:270–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettle, D. (1999) Linguistic diversity. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newson, L., Postmes, T., Lea, S. E. G. & Webley, P. (2005) Why are modern families small? Toward an evolutionary and cultural explanation for the demographic transition. Personality and Social Psychology Review 9:360–75.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A. & Shariff, A. F. (2008) The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science 322:5862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, P. & Inglehart, R. (2004) Sacred and secular. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J. & Case, T. I. (2009) Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin 135:303–21.Google Scholar
Oishi, S., Schimmack, U., Diener, E. & Suh, E. M. (1998) The measurement of values and individualism-collectivism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24:1177–89.Google Scholar
Oliveira, R. F., Taborsky, M. & Brockmann, H. J. (2008) Alternative reproductive tactics: An integrative approach. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Park, J. H., Faulkner, J. & Schaller, M. (2003) Evolved disease-avoidance processes and contemporary anti-social behavior: Prejudicial attitudes and avoidance of people with physical disabilities. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 27:6587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, J. H. & Schaller, M. (2005) Does attitude similarity serve as a heuristic cue for kinship? Evidence of an implicit cognitive association. Evolution and Human Behavior 26:158–70.Google Scholar
Park, J. H., Schaller, M. & Crandall, C. S. (2007) Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people. Evolution and Human Behavior 28:410–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitchappan, R. M. (2002) Castes, migration, immunogenetics and infectious diseases in south India. Community Genetics 5:157–61.Google Scholar
Price-Smith, A. T. (2002) The health of nations: Infectious disease, environmental change, and their effects on national security and development. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Prokop, P., Usak, M. & Fancovicová, J. (2010a) Health and the avoidance of macroparasites: A preliminary cross-cultural study. Journal of Ethology 28:345–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prokop, P., Usak, M. & Fancovicová, J. (2010b) Risk of parasite transmission influences perceived vulnerability to disease and perceived danger of disease-relevant animals. Behavioural Processes 85:52–7Google Scholar
Prugnolle, F., Manica, A., Charpentier, M., Guégan, J.-F., Guernier, V. & Balloux, F. (2005) Pathogen-driven selection and worldwide HLA class I diversity. Current Biology 15:1022–27.Google Scholar
Quinlan, R. J. (2007) Human parental effort and environmental risk. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B 274:121–25.Google Scholar
Rees, T. (2009) Is personal insecurity a cause of cross-national differences in the intensity of religious belief? Journal of Religion and Society 11. Available at: moses.creighton.edu/JRS/2009/2009-17.html.Google Scholar
Ridley, M. (1993) The red queen: Sex and the evolution of human nature. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Rossano, M. J. (2007) Supernaturalizing social life: Religion and the evolution of human cooperation. Human Nature 18:272–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rougeron, V., De Meeus, T., Hide, M., Waleckx, E., Bermudez, H., Arevalo, J., Llanos-Cuentas, A., Dujardin, J. C., De Doncker, S., Le Ray, D., Ayala, F. J. & Banuls, A. L. (2009) Extreme inbreeding in Leishmania braziliensis . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106(25):10224–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sage, R. D., Heyneman, D., Lim, K. C. & Wilson, A. C. (1986) Wormy mice in a hybrid zone. Nature 324:6063.Google Scholar
Sagiv, L. & Schwartz, S. H. (1995) Value priorities and readiness for out-group social contact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69:437–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saroglou, V., Delpierre, V. & Dernelle, R. (2004) Values and religiosity: A meta-analysis of studies using Schwartz's model. Personality and Individual Differences 37:721–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, M. (2006) Parasites, behavioral defenses, and the social psychological mechanisms through which cultures are evoked. Psychological Inquiry 17:96101.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. & Duncan, L. A. (2007) The behavioral immune system: Its evolution and social psychological implications. In: Evolution and the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognition, ed. Forges, J. P., Haselton, M. G. & Von Hippel, W., pp. 293307. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. C., Miller, G. E., Gervais, W. M., Yager, S. & Chen, E. (2010) Mere visual perception of other people's disease symptoms facilitates a more aggressive immune response. Psychological Science 21:649–52.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. & Murray, D. R. (2008) Pathogens, personality, and culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95:212–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaller, M. & Neuberg, S. L. (2008) Intergroup prejudices and intergroup conflicts. In: Foundations of evolutionary psychology, ed. Crawford, C. & Krebs, D. L., pp. 399412. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (1992) Universals in the content and structures of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 25:165.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (2004) Mapping and interpreting cultural differences around the world. In: Comparing cultures: Dimensions of culture in a comparative perspective, ed. Vinkin, H., Soeters, J. & Ester, P., pp. 4373. Brill.Google Scholar
Searcy, W. A. & Nowicki, S. (2005) The evolution of animal communication: Reliability and deception in signaling systems. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Sherman, P. W., Lacey, E. A., Reeve, H. K. & Keller, L. (1995) The eusociality continuum. Behavioral Ecology 6:102108.Google Scholar
Shields, W. M. (1982) Philopatry, inbreeding, and the evolution of sex. State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Siegal, M. (1988) Children's knowledge of contagion and contamination as causes of illness. Child Development 59:1353–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, K. F., Sax, D. F., Gaines, S. D., Guernier, V. & Guégan, J.-F. (2007) Globalization of human infectious disease. Ecology 88:1903–10.Google Scholar
Sosis, R. (2000) Religion and intragroup cooperation: Preliminary results of a comparative analysis of utopian communities. Cross-Cultural Research 34:7087.Google Scholar
Sosis, R. (2003) Why aren't we all Hutterities? Costly signaling theory and religious behavior. Human Nature – An Interdisciplinary Biosocial Perspective 14:91127.Google Scholar
Sosis, R. (2005) Does religion promote trust? The role of signaling, reputation, and punishment. Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Religion 1:130.Google Scholar
Sosis, R. & Alcorta, C. (2003) Signaling, solidarity, and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology 12:264–74.Google Scholar
Sosis, R. & Bressler, E. R. (2003) Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research 37:211–39.Google Scholar
Sosis, R., Kress, H. & Boster, J. S. (2007) Scars for war: Evaluating alternative signaling explanations for cross-cultural variance in ritual costs. Evolution and Human Behavior 28:234–47.Google Scholar
Sosis, R. & Ruffle, B. J. (2003) Religious ritual and cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology 44:713–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steadman, L. B. & Palmer, C. T. (2008) The supernatural and natural selection: The evolution of religion. Paradigm.Google Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Case, T. I. & Oaten, M. J. (2009) Frequency and recency of infection and their relationship with disgust and contamination sensitivity. Evolution and Human Behavior 30:363–68.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, L. S. (2004) Illness, injury, and disability among Shiwiar forager-horticulturists: Implications of health-risk buffering for the evolution of human life history. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 123:371–89.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, L. S. & Sugiyama, M. S. (2003) Social roles, prestige, and health risk: Social niche specialization as a risk-buffering strategy. Human Nature 14:165–90.Google Scholar
Thompson, J. N. (2005) The geographic mosaic of coevolution. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R. (1990) The study of adaptation. In: Interpretation and explanation in the study of animal behavior, vol. 2: Explanation, evolution, and adaptation, ed. Bekoff, M. & Jamieson, D., pp. 3162. Westview Press.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R. & Fincher, C. L. (2007) What is the relevance of attachment and life history to political values? Evolution and Human Behavior 28:215–22.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., Fincher, C. L. & Aran, D. (2009) Parasites, democratization, and the liberalization of values across contemporary countries. Biological Reviews 84:113–31.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., Fincher, C. L., Murray, D. R. & Schaller, M. (2010) Zoonotic and non-zoonotic diseases in relation to human personality and societal values: Support for the parasite-stress model. Evolutionary Psychology 8:151–69.Google Scholar
Tibayrenc, M. (2007) Human genetic diversity and the spread of infectious diseases. In: Encyclopedia of infectious diseases – modern methodologies, ed. Tibayrenc, M., pp. 321–35. John Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinsley, M. C., Blanford, S. & Jiggins, F. M. (2006) Genetic variation in Drosophila melanogaster pathogen susceptibility. Parasitology 132:767–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooby, J. (1982) Pathogens, polymorphism, and the evolution of sex. Journal of Theoretical Biology 97:557–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Triandis, H. C. (1995) Individualism and collectivism (new directions in social psychology). Westview Press.Google Scholar
Van Valen, L. (1973) A new evolutionary law. Evolutionary Theory 1:130.Google Scholar
Vandello, J. A. & Cohen, D. (1999) Patterns of individualism and collectivism across the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77:279–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanhanen, T. (2003) Democratization: A comparative analysis of 170 countries. Routledge.Google Scholar
Vigil, J. M. (2009) A socio-relational framework of sex differences in the expression of emotion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32:375428.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widman, D. R., Corcoran, K. E. & Nagy, R. E. (2009) Belonging to the same religion enhances the opinion of others' kindness and morality. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology 3:281–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S. (2002) Darwin's cathedral: Evolution, religion, and the nature of society. The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1971) The insect societies. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wolfe, N. D., Dunavan, C. P. & Diamond, J. (2007) Origins of major human infectious diseases. Nature 447:279–83.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2004) Mortality and burden of disease estimates for WHO member states in 2002. Electronic file available at: http://www.who.int/healthinfo/bod/en/index.html.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, P. (2007) Atheism – contemporary numbers and patterns. In: The Cambridge companion to atheism, ed. Martin, M., pp. 4765. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. The correlation between Combined Parasite-Stress and In-Group Assortativeness for the 65 nations with correspondent data for all 11 items that make up the 2 variables (r=.71, p<.0001). The line is the regression line.

Figure 1

Figure 2. The correlation between Parasite-Stress USA and In-Group Assortativeness USA for the 43 states/state combinations with correspondent data for all 14 items that make up the 2 variables (r=.66, p<.0001). The line is the regression line.

Supplementary material: PDF

Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material

Electronic supplement 1

Download Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 104.8 KB
Supplementary material: File

Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material

Electronic supplement 2

Download Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material(File)
File 38.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material

Electronic supplement 3

Download Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material(File)
File 14.3 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material

Electronic supplement 4

Download Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 58.8 KB
Supplementary material: File

Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material

Electronic supplement 5

Download Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material(File)
File 21.5 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material

Electronic supplement 6

Download Fincher and Thornhill supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 87.3 KB