Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T19:56:29.231Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perceived ethnic discrimination as a risk factor for psychotic symptoms: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 April 2020

Olivier Bardol
Affiliation:
Grenoble Alpes University, School of Medicine, Grenoble, France
Stéphanie Grot
Affiliation:
Centre de recherche, Institut Universitaire en Santé Mentale de Montréal, Montreal, Canada
Hans Oh
Affiliation:
University of Southern California, Los Angeles, USA
Emmanuel Poulet
Affiliation:
Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier, Bron, France INSERM, U1028; CNRS, UMR5292; Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Psychiatric Disorders: from Resistance to Response – PSYR2 Team, Lyon, F-69000, France University Lyon 1, Villeurbanne, F-69000, France Department of Emergency Psychiatry, University Hospital Edouard Herriot, Hospices civils de Lyon, Lyon, France
Halima Zeroug-Vial
Affiliation:
Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier, Bron, France
Jérôme Brunelin
Affiliation:
Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier, Bron, France INSERM, U1028; CNRS, UMR5292; Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Psychiatric Disorders: from Resistance to Response – PSYR2 Team, Lyon, F-69000, France University Lyon 1, Villeurbanne, F-69000, France
Edouard Leaune*
Affiliation:
Centre Hospitalier Le Vinatier, Bron, France INSERM, U1028; CNRS, UMR5292; Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, Psychiatric Disorders: from Resistance to Response – PSYR2 Team, Lyon, F-69000, France University Lyon 1, Villeurbanne, F-69000, France Institute for Philosophical Research, Lyon 3 University, Lyon, France
*
Author for correspondence: Edouard Leaune, E-mail: edouard.leaune@ch-le-vinatier.fr
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Previous studies have shown an elevated risk of psychotic symptoms (PS) and experiences (PEs) among ethnic minority groups, with significant variation between groups. This pattern may be partially attributable to the unfavorable socio-environmental conditions that surround ethnic minority groups. Perceived ethnic discrimination (PED) in particular has been a salient putative risk factor to explain the increased risk.

Methods

We conducted a systematic literature review and meta-analysis to assess the impact of PED on reporting PS/PEs in ethnic minorities. This review abides by the guidelines set forth by Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses. The included studies were obtained from the databases: Medline, PsycINFO, and Web Of Science. Sub-group analyses were performed assessing the effect of PED in different subtypes of PS, the influence of ethnicity and moderating/mediating factors.

Results

Seventeen studies met the inclusion criteria, and nine were used to conduct the meta-analysis. We found a positive association between PED and the occurrence of PS/PEs among ethnic minorities. The combined odds ratio were 1.77 (95% CI 1.26–2.49) for PS and 1.94 (95% CI 1.42–2.67) for PEs. We found that the association was similar across ethnic groups and did not depend on the ethnic origin of individuals. Weak evidence supported the buffering effects of ethnic identity, collective self-esteem and social support; and no evidence supported the moderating effect of ethnic density. Sensitivity to race-based rejection significantly but only slightly mediated the association.

Conclusion

These findings suggest that PED is involved in the increased risk of PS/PEs in ethnic minority populations.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

A substantial proportion of the general population experiences psychotic symptoms (PS), including both people with mental health disorders and healthy populations (Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013). When not reaching the threshold for clinical disorders, the self-reported delusional or hallucinatory symptoms are referred to as psychotic experiences (PEs). The prevalence of lifetime PEs is estimated to be 7.2% of the general population (Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013; van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul, & Krabbendam, Reference van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and Krabbendam2009), though this can widely vary depending on the country (Nuevo et al., Reference Nuevo, Chatterji, Verdes, Naidoo, Arango and Ayuso-Mateos2010). For nearly 70% of individuals, however, these symptoms are only transient and do not reach the level of severity that would constitute clinical psychosis (Linscott & van Os, Reference Linscott and van Os2013). Nevertheless, their presence determines a high-risk state of psychotic transition estimated at about 30% within 2 years (Dominguez, Wichers, Lieb, Wittchen, & van Os, Reference Dominguez, Wichers, Lieb, Wittchen and van Os2011; Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Bonoldi, Yung, Borgwardt, Kempton, Valmaggia and McGuire2012). While PEs emerge on the least severe end of the psychosis continuum, they have become a public health concern because they are still associated with higher rates of psychiatric disorders (McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Andrade, Benjet, Bromet and Kessler2016), suicidal behaviors (Honings, Drukker, Groen, & van Os, Reference Honings, Drukker, Groen and van Os2016) and disabilities (Navarro-Mateu et al., Reference Navarro-Mateu, Alonso, Lim, Saha, Aguilar-Gaxiola and Al-Hamzawi2017), poorer physical health conditions (Oh, Waldman, Stickley, DeVylder, & Koyanagi, Reference Oh, Waldman, Stickley, DeVylder and Koyanagi2019; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Saha, Lim, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso and McGrath2018) and treatment-seeking behaviors (Bhavsar et al., Reference Bhavsar, Maccabe, Hatch, Hotopf, Boydell and McGuire2017; Murphy, Shevlin, Houston, & Adamson, Reference Murphy, Shevlin, Houston and Adamson2012), and higher mortality (Sharifi et al., Reference Sharifi, Eaton, Wu, Roth, Burchett and Mojtabai2015).

Recent meta-analyses identified a two-fold increased risk for PS and PEs (Leaune et al., Reference Leaune, Dealberto, Luck, Grot, Zeroug-Vial, Poulet and Brunelin2019; Tortelli et al., Reference Tortelli, Nakamura, Suprani, Schürhoff, Van der Waerden, Szöke and Pignon2018) and a two- to three-fold increased risk for psychotic disorders (Radua et al., Reference Radua, Ramellaamellame, Ioannidis, Reichenberg, Phiphopthatsanee, Amir and Fusar-Poli2018) among ethnic minorities when compared with the majority groups, with significant variations in the level of risk between and within ethnic minority groups. These variations may be related to differential exposures to socio-environmental risk factors (Morgan, Charalambides, Hutchinson, & Murray, Reference Morgan, Charalambides, Hutchinson and Murray2010; Veling, Reference Veling2013). Epidemiological findings highlight the social determinants of health, examining the role of discrimination, social exclusion, and social defeat in the etiopathogenesis of psychosis (Kirkbride et al., Reference Kirkbride, Boydell, Ploubidis, Morgan, Dazzan, McKenzie and Jones2008; Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Kirkbride, Hutchinson, Craig, Morgan, Dazzan and Fearon2008; Oh, Reference Oh2016; Selten, van der Ven, Rutten, & Cantor-Graae, Reference Selten, van der Ven, Rutten and Cantor-Graae2013; Stilo et al., Reference Stilo, Gayer-Anderson, Beards, Hubbard, Onyejiaka, Keraite and Morgan2017). The socio-developmental model developed by Morgan et al. (Reference Morgan, Charalambides, Hutchinson and Murray2010) emphasizes the complexity of interactions between several risk factors (genetic, individual, and environmental) and the importance of considering the type and quality of these interactions. According to this model, social risk factors (such as racial or ethnic discrimination) may contribute to the accumulation of adverse experiences over the life course, impacting early neuro-developmental processes, increasing exposure to harmful chronic stress, which ultimately increases vulnerability to PS/PEs (Monroe, Reference Monroe2008; Radua et al., Reference Radua, Ramellaamellame, Ioannidis, Reichenberg, Phiphopthatsanee, Amir and Fusar-Poli2018). Repeated exposure to social adversity can link to PS/PEs through the generation of cognitive biases and affective states that predispose one to develop symptoms. Discriminatory experiences have been proposed as a putative factor for the development of PS/PEs in ethnic minorities, as they are more prevalent in these populations compared to the general population. They may, for example, link to delusional symptoms through increased sensitivity to rejection, threat anticipation, anxiety, and a tendency to jump to conclusions (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Charalambides, Hutchinson and Murray2010).

Discrimination is the unfair and prejudicial treatment of a person or a category of people on the basis of some identity marker (e.g. sex, disability, religion) (Dovidio, Hewstone, Glick, & Esses, Reference Dovidio, Hewstone, Glick and Esses2010). A common form of discrimination is motivated by hostility toward people on the basis of their ethnicity and/or race which are related but distinct concepts, with the race being a social construct to categorize people based on physical characteristics (such as skin color), while ethnicity being a term to refer to the shared culture (such as language, customs) and identity based on common heritage, ancestry, or nationality. Racial and ethnic discrimination are prevalent across countries (Lee, Perez, Boykin, & Mendoza-Denton, Reference Lee, Perez, Boykin and Mendoza-Denton2019) and major social stressors due to their tendency to foster self-depreciation, low self-esteem, emotional stress, and loss of control (Carter, Reference Carter2007; de Freitas et al., Reference de Freitas, Fernandes-Jesus, Ferreira, Coimbra, Teixeira, de Moura and Fontaine2018; Ruggiero & Taylor, Reference Ruggiero and Taylor1995). Discrimination also operates at multiple levels to impact health by promoting an unequal distribution of salutary resources (e.g. political authority, access to healthcare and education)(Gee, Reference Gee2002; Harrell et al., Reference Harrell, Burford, Cage, Nelson, Shearon, Thompson and Green2011; Tajfel & Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Hogg and Abrams2001) and limits the possibilities of upward mobility and social ascension (Carter, Reference Carter2007; McEwen, Reference McEwen2012), which potentially proliferates even more stress. Indeed, several meta-analyses have reported that populations exposed to various types of discrimination have lower overall health status and a higher risk of developing mental disorders (e.g. depression, anxiety disorders, suicidal behaviors, psychosis) (de Freitas et al., Reference de Freitas, Fernandes-Jesus, Ferreira, Coimbra, Teixeira, de Moura and Fontaine2018; Pascoe & Richman, Reference Pascoe and Richman2009). It is thus imperative to examine how perceived ethnic discrimination (PED) serves as a putative risk factor for PS/PEs and may help explain the increased prevalence of PS/PEs among individuals from ethnic minority groups (Missinne & Bracke, Reference Missinne and Bracke2012). While the literature has demonstrated the significant association between ethnic minority position and PS/PEs, as well as the association between discrimination and poor health status, to our knowledge, scholars have yet to summarize existing literature in a meta-analysis to show the associations between PED and PS/PEs. In a recent systematic review on the association between discrimination and psychosis, Pearce, Rafiq, Simpson, and Varese (Reference Pearce, Rafiq, Simpson and Varese2019) demonstrated the association between discrimination and PS. However, their review did not focus on PED but included several discriminatory experiences (sexual orientation, gender, social class…). Moreover, they concluded by expressing the need to perform a meta-analysis on the association between discrimination and PS. In a recent meta-analysis, Leaune et al. (Reference Leaune, Dealberto, Luck, Grot, Zeroug-Vial, Poulet and Brunelin2019) reported higher odds for hallucinatory symptoms compared to delusional symptoms in ethnic minorities. The impact of PED on the types of PS/PEs would thus be interesting to evaluate. Prior studies have identified socio-environmental and psychological factors – such as ethnic identity, ethnic density or social support – that can potentially buffer the detrimental effects of PED on health (Straiton, Aambø, & Johansen, Reference Straiton, Aambø and Johansen2019; Yip, Wang, Mootoo, & Mirpuri, Reference Yip, Wang, Mootoo and Mirpuri2019). However, no systematic review has evaluated their role in moderating the associations between PED and PS/PEs. Further, some psychological factors, such as sensitivity to rejection, have been discussed in existing studies and may mediate the association by promoting negative cognitive schemas and by increasing the liability to the deleterious effects of PED.

Aims of the study

The aim of our study was to systematically review the literature and to conduct a meta-analysis to examine the association between PED and PS/PEs among ethnic minority groups. We hypothesized that PED would contribute to an increased risk for PS and PEs in ethnic minority groups. A secondary objective was to investigate (a) which types of PS/PEs (i.e. hallucinatory symptoms or delusional symptoms) are especially associated with PED, and (b) whether ethnicity moderates the impact of PED on PS/PEs across minority groups. We also aimed to explore the role of other putative moderators (ethnic density, ethnic identity, social support) or mediators (sensitivity to rejection) with respect to the association between PED and PS/PEs.

Method

Systematic research

The review protocol was based on the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) (Liberati et al., Reference Liberati, Altman, Tetzlaff, Mulrow, Gotzsche, Ioannidis and Moher2009) and was registered on PROSPERO (registration number: CRD 42019118523), in January 2019. The search was conducted on the following databases: Medline, Web of Science and PsycINFO, using the following search terms, grouped into three categories as follows: (a) population: (migration OR migrant OR ethnic* OR general population); (b) exposure: (discrimin* OR racis* OR social exclusion OR ‘social defeat’); (c) outcome: (psychotic OR psychosis OR hallucination OR delusion OR paranoia OR schizo* OR UHR). To ensure the exhaustivity of the search and the relevance of the algorithm, a pilot electronic research was performed in December 2018. To be exhaustive, the electronic search was supplemented with a manual search using the reference lists of relevant publications.

Inclusion criteria

Inclusion criteria were (a) articles published in peer-review journals in the English language; (b) articles published between January 1960 and 31 January 2019; (c) articles using a validated psychometric scale to assess the presence of PS and/or PEs; (d) articles assessing perceived discrimination in one or several ethnic minorities. No age restriction was applied to the population.

Based on methodologies of previous meta-analyses that assessed PS (Honings et al., Reference Honings, Drukker, Groen and van Os2016; Kaymaz et al., Reference Kaymaz, Drukker, Lieb, Wittchen, Werbeloff, Weiser and van Os2012; Leaune et al., Reference Leaune, Dealberto, Luck, Grot, Zeroug-Vial, Poulet and Brunelin2019), were retained studies that assessed the report of PS under the following terms: ‘psychotic symptoms’, ‘psychotic experiences’, ‘psychotic-like experiences’, ‘delusional experiences’, ‘hallucinatory experiences’, ‘hallucination-like experiences’, ‘paranoia’ or ‘delusion-like experiences’. The outcome was defined as PEs when people with a psychotic disorder were excluded from the analyses in the included articles. Ethnicity was defined as an identity and group identification, based on social and cultural commonalities (Morgan et al., Reference Morgan, Dazzan, Morgan, Jones, Harrison, Leff and Fearon2006; Schofield et al., Reference Schofield, Das-Munshi, Bécares, Morgan, Bhavsar, Hotopf and Hatch2016).

Selection process

Two authors (OB and EL) performed the electronic search between January and March 2019. The studies identified through the databases were then subjected to three selection filters, depending on the titles, abstracts, and comprehensive full-text reading of each selected paper thereafter. The authors compared their results at each stage. When discrepancies arose, the choice to include or exclude an article was discussed with other co-authors.

Data analysis

Two authors (OB and EL) assessed the quality of included studies according to the STROBE statement (Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) (von Elm et al., Reference von Elm, Altman, Egger, Pocock, Gotzsche and Vandenbroucke2007). The level of quality was as follows: very good quality (26–32); good quality (20–26); average quality (14–20); and poor quality (<14).

Systematic review

A systematic review was carried out which involved separately and independently extracting quantitative data by two authors (OB and EL) for each of the following outcomes: (a) the association between PED and PS/PEs; (b) the role of ethnicity, and (c) the role of factors mediating or moderating the association between PED and PS/PEs. The extracted data were then grouped and the evidence were summarized for each outcome. All discrepancies were discussed, and the consensus was reached between all authors on the final summary.

To assess the role of associated socio-environmental factors (e.g. ethnic identity, ethnic density, and social support) on the association between PED and PS/PEs, we examined the evidence on their mediating effects (i.e. explaining some portion of the association) or moderating effects (i.e. affecting the strength of the association).

Meta-analysis

Studies that presented odd ratios (ORs) for the primary outcome (e.g. ORs assessing the association between PED and PS/PEs) were included in the meta-analysis. The data were analyzed using Comprehensive Meta-analysis software (version 2 of Borenstein). Pooled ORs were calculated using a random-effect model in order to consider between-study variability and provide a more realistic estimation of the summary ORs (Cooper, Hedges, & Valentine, Reference Cooper, Hedges and Valentine2009). For each study, ORs were converted into the log OR. The weighted sum of the log OR was measured and then reconverted into OR. When available, the meta-analysis was performed with an adjusted OR (adjusted for age, gender, and individual socio-economic position).

Heterogeneity between studies was quantified with the Q statistic and the I 2. The Q statistic determines whether the observed variations in OR were caused by a between-study true difference and not by the within-study sampling error. A significant Q-value reflects a true variation of OR between studies. I 2 is the proportion of inconsistency between studies' results attributable to heterogeneity. I 2 values of 25, 50, and 75%, respectively, reflect a small, moderate, or high degree of heterogeneity (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, Reference Higgins, Thompson, Deeks and Altman2003).

A p value >0.05 threshold was used to assess statistical significance.

A first subgroups analysis was performed to examine the effect of PED on the different types of PS/PEs, e.g. on hallucinatory and delusional symptoms. Pooled ORs were computed for each subgroup.

A second subgroups analysis was performed to determine if there was a significant effect of ethnicity on the association between PED and PS/PEs. Pooled ORs were separately computed according to different ethnic groups and compared using the Q statistic. The between-subgroup Q statistic was calculated with a random-effect model. A significant Q statistic reflects significantly different ORs between subgroups. The included studies allowed us to undertake this subgroup analysis only in the UK on five main ethnic groups, who were identified as Bangladeshi, Indian, Black Caribbean, Irish, and Pakistani.

Publication bias

Publication bias was assessed by visual inspection of a funnel plot of the standard error by the log OR and by Egger's regression intercept (Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider, & Minder, Reference Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider and Minder1997). In the absence of bias, the plot should resemble a symmetrical inverted funnel. An asymmetric funnel indicates a systematic relationship between reported effects and study size.

Results

Included studies

Using the research protocol, 1209 articles were retrieved from the databases. After removal of duplicates and the application of selection filters, 17 articles met the inclusion criteria (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Flow chart diagram of study selection according to the PRISMA guidelines.

The scores at the STROBE checklist ranged between 16 and 27 (mean score = 23.55, s.d. = 2.45), highlighting the good quality of the included articles. The studies included a total of 33 211 people. The age of the participants ranged between 13 and 74. The ethnic groups studied were heterogeneous and mainly from low- and middle-income countries of the southern hemisphere. Data were collected in a total of five countries (USA, Netherland, UK, Romania, and Norway). All studies were cross-sectional, with samples ranging from 70 to 8990 people. Publication dates ranged from 2005 to 2019, with half published in the last 5 years. Seven studies assessed the presence of PEs (Anglin, Lighty, Greenspoon, & Ellman, Reference Anglin, Lighty, Greenspoon and Ellman2014; Becares, Nazroo, & Stafford, Reference Becares, Nazroo and Stafford2009; Chakraborty, McKenzie, Hajat, & Stansfeld, Reference Chakraborty, McKenzie, Hajat and Stansfeld2010; el Bouhaddani, van Domburgh, Schaefer, Doreleijers, & Veling, Reference el Bouhaddani, van Domburgh, Schaefer, Doreleijers and Veling2019; Karlsen, Nazroo, McKenzie, Bhui, & Weich, Reference Karlsen, Nazroo, McKenzie, Bhui and Weich2005; Oh, Yang, Anglin, & DeVylder, Reference Oh, Yang, Anglin and DeVylder2014, Reference Oh, Cogburn, Anglin, Lukens and DeVylder2016). Three main scales were studied to assess the prevalence of PS or PEs, the Prodromal Questionnaire (PQ) (n = 6), the Psychosis Screening Questionnaire (PSQ) (n = 3), and the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) (n = 3). A total of 9 scales was used to assess PED. The characteristics of the included studies are displayed in Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics of studies included in the systematic review

PS, psychotic symptoms; PEs, psychotic experiences; PSQ, Psychosis Screening Questionnaire, CIDI, Composite International Diagnostic Interview; PANS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; PQ, Prodromal Questionnaire.

a Quality of studies was assessed with the 32-items STROBE statement, a score of 1 (presence) or 0 (absence) was coded for each item (total score on 32).

Association between PED and PS/PEs

Of the 17 included articles, 15 studies reported a significant correlation between PED and PS/PEs. Two studies (Oh et al., Reference Oh, Yang, Anglin and DeVylder2014; Veling et al., Reference Veling, Selten, Susser, Laan, Mackenbach and Hoek2007) reported a dose-response association between PED and PS/PEs, with higher levels of PED being associated with higher ORs for PS/PEs.

Nine studies were included in the meta-analysis (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, Lighty, Greenspoon and Ellman2014; Becares et al., Reference Becares, Nazroo and Stafford2009; Chakraborty et al., Reference Chakraborty, McKenzie, Hajat and Stansfeld2010; el Bouhaddani et al., Reference el Bouhaddani, van Domburgh, Schaefer, Doreleijers and Veling2019; Karlsen et al., Reference Karlsen, Nazroo, McKenzie, Bhui and Weich2005; Oh et al., Reference Oh, Yang, Anglin and DeVylder2014, Reference Oh, Cogburn, Anglin, Lukens and DeVylder2016; Veling et al., Reference Veling, Selten, Susser, Laan, Mackenbach and Hoek2007; Reference Veling, Hoek and Mackenbach2008). A significant positive correlation was found between PED and PS [OR 1.82, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.41–2.36](Fig. 2). The result remained significant after adjustment for socio-demographic factors (n = 5; OR 1.77, 95% CI 1.26–2.49). When considering PEs, the pooled OR was slightly higher (n = 7; OR 1.94, 95% CI 1.42–2.67). The heterogeneity between studies was high (I 2 = 79.08; Q(7) = 33.47; p < 0.0001).

Fig. 2. Pooled meta-analytic odds ratios for the association between PED and PS.

The type of discriminatory event influenced the association in two studies, with a more pronounced effect for racial harassment (Karlsen et al., Reference Karlsen, Nazroo, McKenzie, Bhui and Weich2005), police discrimination, and denied loan or promotion (Oh, Cogburn, Anglin, Lukens, & DeVylder, Reference Oh, Cogburn, Anglin, Lukens and DeVylder2016).

PED and types of PS/PEs

Three studies (el Bouhaddani et al., Reference el Bouhaddani, van Domburgh, Schaefer, Doreleijers and Veling2019; Oh et al., Reference Oh, Yang, Anglin and DeVylder2014, Reference Oh, Cogburn, Anglin, Lukens and DeVylder2016) reported a significant association between PED and both hallucinatory symptoms and delusional symptoms. The association was found to be strongest for delusional symptoms (OR 2.53, 95% CI 1.60–4.01) than for hallucinatory symptoms (OR 1.65, 95% CI 1.29–2.14). The heterogeneity between studies was low for delusional symptoms (I 2 = 0; Q(2) = 0.66; p = 0.72) and hallucinatory symptoms (I 2 = 0; Q(2) = 1.08; p = 0.58).

Ethnicity

Three studies provided results in OR based on ethnicity for the association between PED and PS/PEs (Anglin, Lui, Espinosa, Tikhonov, & Ellman, Reference Anglin, Lui, Espinosa, Tikhonov and Ellman2009; Chakraborty et al., Reference Chakraborty, McKenzie, Hajat and Stansfeld2010; Karlsen et al., Reference Karlsen, Nazroo, McKenzie, Bhui and Weich2005) covering 5 ethnic groups living in the UK (e.g. Bangladeshi, Black Caribbean, Indian, Irish and Pakistani). The association between PED and PS/PEs was found to be significant for all ethnic groups, except Irish (see Fig. 3). Between subgroups, analysis showed no significant variation between ethnic groups (Q = 2.22; df(Q) = 4; p = 0.695). The heterogeneity between studies was low (I 2 = 0; Q(13) = 12.09; p = 0.52).

Fig. 3. Forest plot of pooled odds ratios for different ethnic sub-groups.

Putative mediators and moderators

Eight articles assessed the role of socio-environmental or psychological factors with respect to the association between PED and PS/PEs (Anglin, Greenspoon, Lighty, & Ellman, Reference Anglin, Greenspoon, Lighty and Ellman2016, Reference Anglin, Lui, Espinosa, Tikhonov and Ellman2018; Becares et al., Reference Becares, Nazroo and Stafford2009; Chakraborty et al., Reference Chakraborty, McKenzie, Hajat and Stansfeld2010; el Bouhaddani et al., Reference el Bouhaddani, van Domburgh, Schaefer, Doreleijers and Veling2019; Kong, Reference Kong2016; van de Beek, van der Krieke, Schoevers, & Veling, Reference van de Beek, van der Krieke, Schoevers and Veling2017; Veling, Hoek, & Mackenbach, Reference Veling, Hoek and Mackenbach2008). Five moderators were evaluated: ethnic identity (n = 3), ethnic density (n = 2), social support (n = 2), and collective self-esteem (n = 1); one mediator was examined: sensitivity to race-based rejection (n = 1). Anglin et al. (Reference Anglin, Lui, Espinosa, Tikhonov and Ellman2018) found that the development of a strong ethnic identity significantly buffered the association between PED and PS (b = −0.45, s.e. = 0.20, p < 0.05), whereas Veling et al. (Reference Veling, Hoek and Mackenbach2008) and el Bouhaddani et al. (Reference el Bouhaddani, van Domburgh, Schaefer, Doreleijers and Veling2019) found no buffering effect. Living in an environment of high ethnic density was not shown to have a significant moderating effect on the association between PED and PS in two studies (Becares et al., Reference Becares, Nazroo and Stafford2009; Veling et al., Reference Veling, Hoek and Mackenbach2008). Two studies that evaluated the effect of social support on the association showed contrasting results; van de Beek et al. (Reference van de Beek, van der Krieke, Schoevers and Veling2017) found a significant moderating effect (R 2 change 0.021, p = 0.013) while Chakraborty et al. (Reference Chakraborty, McKenzie, Hajat and Stansfeld2010) did not. Kong (Reference Kong2016) found a significant moderating effect of collective self-esteem to buffer the association (β = −0.26; p < 0.001; 95% CI −0.38 to −0.14). Anglin et al. (Reference Anglin, Greenspoon, Lighty and Ellman2016) did not find evidence that sensitivity to race-based rejection was a moderator but did find evidence that it partially (though only slightly) mediated the effect of PED on PS (bias-corrected CI 0.0119–0.0466).

Publication bias

Visual inspection of the funnel plot of the standard error by the log OR showed symmetry. Egger's regression was not significant (r = 1.82; p = 0.23), and the fail-safe N was n =180. This does not suggest the presence of publication bias regarding the association between PED and PS/PEs in ethnic minority groups (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4. Funnel plot assessing publication bias for the association between PED and PS.

Discussion

Summary of results

Our systematic review and meta-analysis aimed to assess the association between PED and PS/PEs. First, we found a significant association between PED and both PS and PEs, which may partly explain the greater risk for PS and PEs previously observed in people from ethnic minorities. This effect was found to be significant for both delusional and hallucinatory symptoms, with the strongest association for delusional symptoms. Second, we found that the association was similar across ethnic groups and did not depend on the ethnic origin of individuals. Finally, only weak evidence supported the moderating effect of ethnic identity, social support, and collective self-esteem to buffer the association between PED and PS/PEs. One study reported the indirect and partial mediating effect of sensitivity to race-based rejection. The results on secondary outcomes must be interpreted with caution as they were based on a small number of studies.

Strength and limitations

We performed the first meta-analysis on the association between PED and PS/PEs in ethnic minority groups. Our results are consistent with the literature and rely on a systematic quantitative synthesis of data extracted from good quality articles. Our review and meta-analysis add important evidence to the literature on PS/PEs, discrimination, and the experience of ethnic minorities in high-income countries.

However, our review and meta-analysis have several potential limitations. First, a small number of studies was included in this meta-analysis, and these studies were conducted in a limited number of countries, mainly high-income countries. Our results notably prevent us from making any conclusion regarding the association between PED and PS/PEs in low- or middle-income countries. The number of included studies was particularly low for our secondary results so that these results must be interpreted with caution. For example, while we found no strong evidence for ethnic density as a moderator, we arrived at this conclusion using data from only three available studies. Another example is that the subgroup analyses were mainly drawn from three UK studies including only five specific ethnic minorities. Thus, more research is needed to examine PED and PS/PEs among racial and ethnic minorities across various contexts. Second, the heterogeneity among the included studies was high in terms of definition and assessment of outcomes and predictors. The assessment of PS/PEs was conducted using different scales and most often by self-questionnaires, which are known to have a potentially significant number of false positives (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Chan, Chang, Lee, Hui and Chen2016). However, all included studies used validated measures to assess PS/PEs; the WHO CIDI specifically has been used widely across multiple countries (McGrath et al., Reference McGrath, Saha, Al-Hamzawi, Alonso, Bromet, Bruffaerts and Kessler2015). Third, the assessment of discrimination by several self-questionnaires may also be subject to social desirability bias. The phenomena of discrimination-discrepancy may, for example, result in an underestimation of individual discrimination and an overestimation of discrimination against the group (Taylor, Wright, Moghaddam, & Lalonde, Reference Taylor, Wright, Moghaddam and Lalonde1990). Finally, the validity of the concept of ethnicity should be discussed in the included studies. In a large number of studies, ethnicity was assessed through the place of birth of the individual or his/her/their parents and did not measure one's sense of belonging or connectedness to a given culture.

Interpretation of results

Our results are consistent with the previous literature on the association between discrimination and PS (Pearce et al., Reference Pearce, Rafiq, Simpson and Varese2019). Janssen et al. (Reference Janssen, Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, de Graaf, Vollebergh and van Os2003) performed the first longitudinal study that showed a significant association between perceived discrimination and the occurrence of delusional ideation. Recently, Fonseca de Freitas et al. (Reference de Freitas, Fernandes-Jesus, Ferreira, Coimbra, Teixeira, de Moura and Fontaine2018) performed a large meta-analysis on psychological correlates of PED in Europe and found a significant association between PED and symptoms of psychosis (r = 0.21 95% CI 0.08–0.33, p = 0.002), based on four European studies. In comparison, our meta-analysis examined a broader range of studies performed in both Europe and the USA, ensuring the greater generalizability of our results. However, most of the studies included in our review were case-control studies. This methodological design is known to suffer from threats to validity and has been associated with recall bias. In particular, cross-sectional design decreases the ability to make a causal inference and only allows us to ascertain correlation, without defining the direction of the effect. Notably, people with PS/PEs may be more prone to report recent discrimination events due to their attributional style (Becares et al., Reference Becares, Nazroo and Stafford2009; Chakraborty et al., Reference Chakraborty, McKenzie, Hajat and Stansfeld2010; Karlsen et al., Reference Karlsen, Nazroo, McKenzie, Bhui and Weich2005; Veling et al., Reference Veling, Hoek and Mackenbach2008). However, this should mainly be evident for delusion, and less so far for hallucinations.

Our results raise the question of the putative mechanisms involved in the association between PED and PS/PEs. As previously proposed by Pascoe and Richman (Reference Pascoe and Richman2009), discrimination may increase the risk for PS/PEs through three pathways: (1) an indirect social determinant of health; (2) a direct non-specific effect as a chronic social stressor; and (3) a direct specific effect by impairment of social cognition. First, discrimination may result in a limitation of access to healthcare, life-promoting resources, and education (Carter & Forsyth, Reference Carter and Forsyth2010; Gee, Spencer, Chen, & Takeuchi, Reference Gee, Spencer, Chen and Takeuchi2007; Liu, Meeuwesen, van Wesel, & Ingleby, Reference Liu, Meeuwesen, van Wesel and Ingleby2015). This may result in increased exposure to other specific risk factors during the early stages of development (prenatal and first month of life) such as infections, nutritional and immuno-related risk factors, but also later in life such as childhood adversity, social adversity in adulthood, and behavioral risk factors, such as cannabis use (McKnight et al., Reference McKnight, Shumway, Masson, Pouget, Jordan, Des Jarlais and Perlman2017). Secondly, as a social stressor, PED may directly cause significant emotional stress, and chronic exposure to stressors could lead to alterations of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (Akdeniz, Tost, & Meyer-Lindenberg, Reference Akdeniz, Tost and Meyer-Lindenberg2014; Berger & Sarnyai, Reference Berger and Sarnyai2015; Kudielka, Schommer, Hellhammer, & Kirschbaum, Reference Kudielka, Schommer, Hellhammer and Kirschbaum2004), resulting in increased cortisol and dopamine secretion in stressful situations (Pruessner, Champagne, Meaney, & Dagher, Reference Pruessner, Champagne, Meaney and Dagher2004). PED could also lead to functional and structural changes in the central nervous system (Clark, Miller, & Hegde, Reference Clark, Miller and Hegde2018). Finally, PED may have specific direct effects by promoting the development of impairment in social cognition in exposed individuals. Discrimination has been shown to promote the adoption of a paranoid external attributional style (Janssen et al., Reference Janssen, Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, de Graaf, Vollebergh and van Os2003), which could eventually predispose the individual to PS/PEs (Bentall, Kinderman, & Kaney, Reference Bentall, Kinderman and Kaney1994; Kinderman & Bentall, Reference Kinderman and Bentall1996; Sharpley, Hutchinson, Murray, & McKenzie, Reference Sharpley, Hutchinson, Murray and McKenzie2001) by promoting the development of ‘high aberrant salience’ (Kapur, Reference Kapur2003). Discriminatory treatment could also foster a sense of social failure and more particularly low self-esteem (Crocker & Major, Reference Crocker and Major1989; Saleem et al., Reference Saleem, Stowkowy, Cadenhead, Cannon, Cornblatt, McGlashan and Addington2014) and thus lead to a low self-concept clarity (Campbell, Reference Campbell1990), which is in part due to internalized stigma (Mathur, Bhola, Khanam, & Thirthalli, Reference Mathur, Bhola, Khanam and Thirthalli2014). These specific disturbances in social cognition may lead to the development of PS/PEs (Moller & Husby, Reference Moller and Husby2000). Moreover, we found that the association between PED and PS/PEs was strongest for delusional symptoms than for hallucinatory symptoms. According to the cognitive model of psychosis developed by Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman, and Bebbington (Reference Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman and Bebbington2001), exposure to PED may promote the development of negative cognitive schemas, especially a paranoid attributional style. This may explain why delusional symptoms would be more strongly associated with PED and is consistent with a study showing that social deprivation predicts paranoia but not hallucinations (Wickham, Taylor, Shevlin, & Bentall, Reference Wickham, Taylor, Shevlin and Bentall2014).

When measuring the effect of PED on the occurrence of PS/PEs, we found no significant differences in the association across the different ethnic groups. At the same time, meta-analyses have found significant differences in the level of risk for PS/PEs across several ethnic minority groups (Leaune et al., Reference Leaune, Dealberto, Luck, Grot, Zeroug-Vial, Poulet and Brunelin2019; Tortelli et al., Reference Tortelli, Nakamura, Suprani, Schürhoff, Van der Waerden, Szöke and Pignon2018). As reported in the studies of Janssen et al. (Reference Janssen, Hanssen, Bak, Bijl, de Graaf, Vollebergh and van Os2003) and Oh et al. (Reference Oh, Yang, Anglin and DeVylder2014), this result may be attributable in part to a ‘dose-response’ effect in the association between discrimination and PS/PEs. In the Netherlands, for example, Veling et al. (Reference Veling, Selten, Susser, Laan, Mackenbach and Hoek2007) found that the ethnic groups that experience the most discrimination showed the highest risk for PS. It is important to note that ethnic groups are not equally exposed to discrimination in terms of frequency and intensity, and so it is conceivable that minorities that experience the most discrimination have the highest risk for PS/PEs (e.g. African Caribbean and African Americans) (Bourque, van der Ven, & Malla, Reference Bourque, van der Ven and Malla2011; Cantor-Graae & Selten, Reference Cantor-Graae and Selten2005). Further research is needed to better explore this phenomenon as only three studies analyzed the role of ethnicity in our meta-analysis.

Furthermore, we found weak evidence that the association between PED and PS/PEs was moderated and mediated by socio-environmental and psychological factors. Notably, Anglin et al. (Reference Anglin, Lui, Espinosa, Tikhonov and Ellman2018) found that having a strong ethnic identity may diminish the association between PED and PS/PEs. Ethnic identity may play a crucial role in facilitating the processes of affiliation and social identification among individuals (el Bouhaddani et al., Reference el Bouhaddani, van Domburgh, Schaefer, Doreleijers and Veling2019), thereby enhancing adaptation to acculturative stress, and limiting the development of a paranoid attribution style (Ruggiero & Taylor, Reference Ruggiero and Taylor1995). Having a strong ethnic identity would also promote access to a meaningful and efficient social network for better access to healthcare and education. Finally, according to the socio-developmental model developed by Morgan et al. (Reference Morgan, Charalambides, Hutchinson and Murray2010), people exposed to discriminatory experiences may develop an emotional state of hypervigilance and hypersensitivity in reaction to the threat, increasing perceived social stress during a new event (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, Greenspoon, Lighty and Ellman2016; Carter, Reference Carter2007). The mediating role of sensitivity to race-based rejection reported by Anglin et al. (Reference Anglin, Greenspoon, Lighty and Ellman2016) is consistent with this hypothesis, as sensitivity to race-based rejection is defined as the extent to which individuals perceive and anticipate threats of rejection due to their race. Discriminatory experiences are prevalent and frequent among Black Americans (English et al., Reference English, Lambert, Tynes, Bowleg, Zea and Howard2020; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Perez, Boykin and Mendoza-Denton2019), and these experiences are associated with increased sensitivity to rejection, potentially resulting in excessive mistrust and altered perception of reality (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, Greenspoon, Lighty and Ellman2016; Sue, Capodilupo, & Holder, Reference Sue, Capodilupo and Holder2008). In terms of the buffering effect of ethnic density (Bosqui, Hoy, & Shannon, Reference Bosqui, Hoy and Shannon2014), we found no significant effect of high ethnic density as a buffer of the association between PED and PS/PEs (Becares et al., Reference Becares, Nazroo and Stafford2009; Veling et al., Reference Veling, Hoek and Mackenbach2008), though this may be due to a lack of power. Living in a high ethnic density area, in theory, should buffer the impact of ethnic discrimination, and individuals residing in these areas should have greater access to ethnicity-based social support and could have a stronger sense of ethnic identity. However, we also found conflicting results regarding the moderating effect of social support, with one Dutch study (van de Beek et al., Reference van de Beek, van der Krieke, Schoevers and Veling2017) reporting a buffering effect while a study performed in the UK (Chakraborty et al., Reference Chakraborty, McKenzie, Hajat and Stansfeld2010) did not. It has been hypothesized that social support would provide a sense of gratification and a sense of affiliation, while also granting access to instrumental support (Chakraborty et al., Reference Chakraborty, McKenzie, Hajat and Stansfeld2010). Kong (Reference Kong2016) for example showed that collective self-esteem buffers the detrimental effects of PED. This result suggests that the sense of social affiliation and belonging may help mitigate the deleterious impact of PED. Interestingly, el Bouhaddani et al. (Reference el Bouhaddani, van Domburgh, Schaefer, Doreleijers and Veling2019) reported that two acculturation styles (e.g. marginalization or assimilation) were associated with an increased risk of reporting PEs in adolescents from ethnic minorities. This is also consistent with previous studies that showed adaptive acculturation is associated with greater quality of life, global and psychological health (Berry, Reference Berry and Berman1990; Berry & Sabatier, Reference Berry and Sabatier2010; Weisman de Mamani et al., Reference Weisman de Mamani, Weintraub, Maura, Martinez de Andino, Brown and Gurak2017), and resilience (Berry, Reference Berry2005). At the same time, it has been shown that individuals experiencing greater discrimination tend to reject both the dominant culture and their own cultural heritage (Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga, & Szapocznik, Reference Schwartz, Unger, Zamboanga and Szapocznik2010). We can thus hypothesize that PED may promote the adoption of an acculturation style unfavorable to mental health, such as marginalization and assimilation. Moreover, marginalization and assimilation may both hamper the feelings of affiliation and belongingness and thus limit the buffering effects on the association between PED and PS/PEs of moderating factors such as ethnic identity or collective self-esteem.

The risk for misdiagnosis or overestimation of clinical psychosis diagnoses in ethnic minorities has been widely documented (Reeves et al., Reference Reeves, Hudson, Fletcher, Sauer, Stewart and Howard2003; Zandi et al., Reference Zandi, Havenaar, Limburg-Okken, van Es, Sidali, Kadri and Kahn2008, Reference Zandi, Havenaar, Smits, Limburg-Okken, van Es, Cahn and van den Brink2010, Reference Zandi, Havenaar, Laan, Kahn and van den Brink2011). According to our results, some psychological reactions to discrimination may partly explain this misdiagnosis risk. Indeed, according to the ‘rejection-identification’ model, the paranoid attribution style may correspond to an operational adaptation strategy against discrimination (Bentall et al., Reference Bentall, Kinderman and Kaney1994; Branscombe, Schmitt, & Harvey, Reference Branscombe, Schmitt and Harvey1999). This suggests that discrimination may increase the occurrence of PEs which may be wrongly interpreted as clinical psychosis by clinicians and may be at the origin of an overestimation of psychosis in ethnic minorities (Oh et al., Reference Oh, Yang, Anglin and DeVylder2014). Indeed, some have argued that psychiatry's history of over-diagnosing Black Americans with psychosis has been a form of institutional racism (Metzl, Reference Metzl2009). However, as recently pointed out by Morgan, Knowles, and Hutchinson (Reference Morgan, Knowles and Hutchinson2019), misdiagnosis is not a satisfying hypothesis to explain the high rates of psychotic disorders reported for decades in ethnic minorities and migrants. Moreover, PS/PEs are also indicative of wider later mental health problems, and several included studies reported that discrimination was significantly associated with an increased prevalence of depression, anxiety disorders, substance use disorder, and post-traumatic stress disorder (Anglin et al., Reference Anglin, Greenspoon, Lighty and Ellman2016; Combs et al., Reference Combs, Penn, Cassisi, Michael, Wood, Wanner and Adams2006; van de Beek et al., Reference van de Beek, van der Krieke, Schoevers and Veling2017; Veling et al., Reference Veling, Hoek and Mackenbach2008). According to our results, it remains unclear if PED is a risk factor along the whole psychotic continuum, or only for subclinical psychosis.

Conclusion

Our results support the hypothesis that ethnic discrimination may partly explain the higher prevalence of PS/PEs in ethnic minority groups. These findings underline the importance of considering socio-environmental risk factors, such as discrimination, in the etiopathogenesis of PS. Further studies are needed to better elucidate the association between ethnic discrimination and PS/PEs. Notably, longitudinal studies should explore the association between PED and PS/PEs across ethnic groups and elucidate the role of associated socio-environmental and psychological factors such as ethnic identity, ethnic density, and social support to moderate the association. The mechanisms involved in the association also need to be further explored.

References

Akdeniz, C., Tost, H., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2014). The neurobiology of social environmental risk for schizophrenia: An evolving research field. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49(4), 507517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anglin, D. M., Greenspoon, M., Lighty, Q., & Ellman, L. M. (2016). Race-based rejection sensitivity partially accounts for the relationship between racial discrimination and distressing attenuated positive psychotic symptoms. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 10, 411418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anglin, D. M., Lighty, Q., Greenspoon, M., & Ellman, L. M. (2014). Racial discrimination is associated with distressing subthreshold positive psychotic symptoms among US urban ethnic minority young adults. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49, 15451555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anglin, D. M., Lui, F., Espinosa, A., Tikhonov, A., & Ellman, L. (2018). Ethnic identity, racial discrimination and attenuated psychotic symptoms in an urban population of emerging adults. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 12, 380390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becares, L., Nazroo, J., & Stafford, M. (2009). The buffering effects of ethnic density on experienced racism and health. Health & Place, 15, 670678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentall, R. P., Kinderman, P., & Kaney, S. (1994). The self, attributional processes and abnormal beliefs: Towards a model of persecutory delusions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 331341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berg, A. O., Melle, I., Rossberg, J. I., Romm, K. L., Larsson, S., Lagerberg, T. V., & Hauff, E. (2011). Perceived discrimination is associated with severity of positive and depression/anxiety symptoms in immigrants with psychosis: A cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry, 11, 77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berger, M., & Sarnyai, Z. (2015). “More than skin deep”: Stress neurobiology and mental health consequences of racial discrimination. Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands), 18, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berry, J. W. (1990). Psychology of acculturation. In Berman, J. J. (ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1989: Cross-cultural perspectives, current theory and research in motivation (Vol. 37). Lincoln, NE, US: University of Nebraska Press, pp. 201234.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (2005). Acculturation: Living successfully in two cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29, 697712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W., & Sabatier, C. (2010). Acculturation, discrimination, and adaptation among second generation immigrant youth in Montreal and Paris. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 34, 191207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhavsar, V., Maccabe, J. H., Hatch, S. L., Hotopf, M., Boydell, J., & McGuire, P. (2017). Subclinical psychotic experiences and subsequent contact with mental health services. British Journal of Psychiatry Open, 7, 6470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosqui, T. J., Hoy, K., & Shannon, C. (2014). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the ethnic density effect in psychotic disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49, 519529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourque, F., van der Ven, E., & Malla, A. (2011). A meta-analysis of the risk for psychotic disorders among first- and second-generation immigrants. Psychological Medicine, 41, 897910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branscombe, N., Schmitt, M., & Harvey, R. (1999). Perceiving pervasive discrimination among African Americans: Implications for group identification and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 135149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 538549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cantor-Graae, E., & Selten, J. P. (2005). Schizophrenia and migration: A meta-analysis and review. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, R. T. (2007). Racism and psychological and emotional injury: Recognizing and assessing race-based traumatic stress. The Counseling Psychologist, 35, 13105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, R., & Forsyth, J. (2010). Reactions to racial discrimination: Emotional stress and help-seeking behaviors. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research. Practice, and Policy, 2, 183191.Google Scholar
Chakraborty, A. T., McKenzie, K. J., Hajat, S., & Stansfeld, S. A. (2010). Racism, mental illness and social support in the UK. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 45, 11151124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, U. S., Miller, E. R., & Hegde, R. R. (2018). Experiences of discrimination are associated with greater resting amygdala activity and functional connectivity. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3, 367378.Google ScholarPubMed
Combs, D. R., Penn, D. L., Cassisi, J., Michael, C., Wood, T., Wanner, J., & Adams, S. (2006). Perceived racism as a predictor of paranoia among African Americans. Journal of Black Psychology, 32, 87104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, H., Hedges, L.V., & Valentine, J.C. (2009). The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Crocker, J., & Major, B. (1989). Social stigma and self-esteem: The self-protective properties of stigma. Psychological Review, 96, 608630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Freitas, D. F., Fernandes-Jesus, M., Ferreira, P. D., Coimbra, S., Teixeira, P. M., de Moura, A., & Fontaine, A. M. (2018). Psychological correlates of perceived ethnic discrimination in Europe: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Violence, 8, 712725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dominguez, M. D. G., Wichers, M., Lieb, R., Wittchen, H.-U., & van Os, J. (2011). Evidence that onset of clinical psychosis is an outcome of progressively more persistent subclinical psychotic experiences: An 8-Year cohort study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37, 8493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J.F., Hewstone, M., Glick, P., Esses, V.M. (2010). Prejudice, stereotyping and discrimination: Theoretical and empirical overview. In SAGE Publication Ltd (ed.), The SAGE handbook of prejudice, stereotyping and discrimination. London/Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications Ltd, 1 Oliver's Yard, Pre City Road, pp. 328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
el Bouhaddani, S., van Domburgh, L., Schaefer, B., Doreleijers, T. A. H., & Veling, W. (2019). Psychotic experiences among ethnic majority and minority adolescents and the role of discrimination and ethnic identity. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 54, 343353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
English, D., Lambert, S. F., Tynes, B. M., Bowleg, L., Zea, M. C., & Howard, L. C. (2020). Daily multidimensional racial discrimination among Black US American adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 66, 101068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P., Bonoldi, I., Yung, A. R., Borgwardt, S., Kempton, M. J., Valmaggia, L., & McGuire, P. (2012). Predicting psychosis: Meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69, 220229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., Freeman, D., & Bebbington, P. E. (2001). A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 31, 189–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, G. C. (2002). A multilevel analysis of the relationship between institutional and individual racial discrimination and health status. American Journal of Public Health, 92, 615623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, G. C., Spencer, M. S., Chen, J., & Takeuchi, D. (2007). A nationwide study of discrimination and chronic health conditions among Asian Americans. American Journal of Public Health, 97, 12751282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrell, C. J. P., Burford, T. I., Cage, B. N., Nelson, T. M., Shearon, S., Thompson, A., & Green, S. (2011). Multiple pathways linking racism to health outcomes. Du Bois Review, 8, 143157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327, 557560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honings, S., Drukker, M., Groen, R., & van Os, J. (2016). Psychotic experiences and risk of self-injurious behaviour in the general population: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46, 237251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janssen, I., Hanssen, M., Bak, M., Bijl, R. V., de Graaf, R., Vollebergh, W., & van Os, J. (2003). Discrimination and delusional ideation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 7176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapur, S. (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlsen, S., Nazroo, J. Y., McKenzie, K., Bhui, K., & Weich, S. (2005). Racism, psychosis and common mental disorder among ethnic minority groups in England. Psychological Medicine, 35, 17951803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaymaz, N., Drukker, M., Lieb, R., Wittchen, H.-U., Werbeloff, N., Weiser, M., & van Os, J. (2012). Do subthreshold psychotic experiences predict clinical outcomes in unselected non-help-seeking population-based samples? A systematic review and meta-analysis, enriched with new results. Psychological Medicine, 42, 22392253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinderman, P., & Bentall, R. P. (1996). Self-discrepancies and persecutory delusions: Evidence for a model of paranoid ideation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 106113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkbride, J. B., Boydell, J., Ploubidis, G. B., Morgan, C., Dazzan, P., McKenzie, K., & Jones, P. B. (2008). Testing the association between the incidence of schizophrenia and social capital in an urban area. Psychological Medicine, 38, 10831094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kong, D. T. (2016). Ethnic minorities' paranoia and self-preservative work behaviors in response to perceived ethnic discrimination, with collective self-esteem as a buffer. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 21, 334351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudielka, B. M., Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2004). Acute HPA axis responses, heart rate, and mood changes to psychosocial stress (TSST) in humans at different times of day. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 983992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leaune, E., Dealberto, M.-J., Luck, D., Grot, S., Zeroug-Vial, H., Poulet, E., & Brunelin, J. (2019). Ethnic minority position and migrant status as risk factors for psychotic symptoms in the general population: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 49, 545558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K.-W., Chan, K.-W., Chang, W.-C., Lee, E. H.-M., Hui, C. L.-M., & Chen, E. Y.-H. (2016). A systematic review on definitions and assessments of psychotic-like experiences. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 10, 316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, R. T., Perez, A. D., Boykin, C. M., & Mendoza-Denton, R. (2019). On the prevalence of racial discrimination in the United States. PLoS One, 14, e0210698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gotzsche, P. C., Ioannidis, J. P. A., & Moher, D. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: Explanation and elaboration. British Medical Journal, 339, b2700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linscott, R. J., & van Os, J. (2013). An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: On the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychological Medicine, 43, 11331149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, C.-H., Meeuwesen, L., van Wesel, F., & Ingleby, D. (2015). Why do ethnic Chinese in the Netherlands underutilize mental health care services? Evidence from a qualitative study. Transcultural Psychiatry, 52, 331352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mathur, S., Bhola, P., Khanam, F., & Thirthalli, J. (2014). Restoring the sense of self in the process of recovery in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychosocial Rehabilitation in Mental Health, 1, 3135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEwen, B.S. (2012). Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109 (Suppl 2), 17180–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrath, J. J., Saha, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., Alonso, J., Bromet, E. J., Bruffaerts, R., & Kessler, R. C. (2015). Psychotic experiences in the general population: A cross-national analysis based on 31261 respondents from 18 countries. JAMA Psychiatry, 72, 697705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrath, J. J., Saha, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., Andrade, L., Benjet, C., Bromet, E. J., & Kessler, R. C. (2016). The bidirectional associations between psychotic experiences and DSM-IV mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 9971006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKnight, C., Shumway, M., Masson, C. L., Pouget, E. R., Jordan, A. E., Des Jarlais, D. C., & Perlman, D. C. (2017). Perceived discrimination among racial and ethnic minority drug users and the association with health care utilization. Journal of Ethnicity in Substance Abuse, 16, 404419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metzl, J. M. (2009). The protest psychosis: How schizophrenia became a black disease. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Missinne, S., & Bracke, P. (2012). Depressive symptoms among immigrants and ethnic minorities: A population based study in 23 European countries. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47, 97109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moller, P., & Husby, R. (2000). The initial prodrome in schizophrenia: Searching for naturalistic core dimensions of experience and behavior. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 217232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, S. M. (2008). Modern approaches to conceptualizing and measuring human life stress. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 3352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, C., Charalambides, M., Hutchinson, G., & Murray, R. M. (2010). Migration, ethnicity, and psychosis: Toward a sociodevelopmental model. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36, 655664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, C., Dazzan, P., Morgan, K., Jones, P., Harrison, G., Leff, J., & Fearon, P. (2006). First episode psychosis and ethnicity: Initial findings from the AESOP study. World Psychiatry, 5, 4046.Google ScholarPubMed
Morgan, C., Kirkbride, J., Hutchinson, G., Craig, T., Morgan, K., Dazzan, P., & Fearon, P. (2008). Cumulative social disadvantage, ethnicity and first-episode psychosis: A case-control study. Psychological Medicine, 38, 17011715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, C., Knowles, G., & Hutchinson, G. (2019). Migration, ethnicity and psychoses: Evidence, models and future directions. World Psychiatry, 18, 247258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, J., Shevlin, M., Houston, J., & Adamson, G. (2012). A population based analysis of subclinical psychosis and help-seeking behavior. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 360367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navarro-Mateu, F., Alonso, J., Lim, C.C.W., Saha, S., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., & WHO World Mental Health Survey Collaborators. (2017). The association between psychotic experiences and disability: Results from the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 136, 7484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nuevo, R., Chatterji, S., Verdes, E., Naidoo, N., Arango, C., & Ayuso-Mateos, J. L. (2010). The continuum of psychotic symptoms in the general population: A cross-national study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 475485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, H. (2016). Social defeat and psychotic experiences in the United States: findings from the Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiological Surveys. ProQuest Information & Learning.Google Scholar
Oh, H., Cogburn, C. D., Anglin, D., Lukens, E., & DeVylder, J. (2016). Major discriminatory events and risk for psychotic experiences among Black Americans. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 86, 277285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, H., Waldman, K., Stickley, A., DeVylder, J. E., & Koyanagi, A. (2019). Psychotic experiences and physical health conditions in the United States. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 90, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, H., Yang, L. H., Anglin, D. M., & DeVylder, J. E. (2014). Perceived discrimination and psychotic experiences across multiple ethnic groups in the United States. Schizophrenia Research, 157, 259267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pascoe, E. A., & Richman, L. S. (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 531554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearce, J, Rafiq, S, Simpson, J, & Varese, F. (2019). Perceived discrimination and psychosis: A systematic review of the literature. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 54, 10231044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruessner, J. C., Champagne, F., Meaney, M. J., & Dagher, A. (2004). Dopamine release in response to a psychological stress in humans and its relationship to early life maternal care: A positron emission tomography study using [11C]raclopride. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 28252831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radua, J., Ramellaamellame, V., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Reichenberg, A., Phiphopthatsanee, N., Amir, T., & Fusar-Poli, P. (2018). What causes psychosis? An umbrella review of risk and protective factors. World Psychiatry, 17, 4966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeves, S., Hudson, S., Fletcher, H., Sauer, J., Stewart, R., & Howard, R. (2003). Are Black Caribbean patients more likely to receive an incorrect diagnosis of very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis than their white British counterparts? American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11, 674777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruggiero, K. M., & Taylor, D. M. (1995). Coping with discrimination: How disadvantaged group members perceive the discrimination that confronts them. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 826838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saleem, M. M., Stowkowy, J., Cadenhead, K. S., Cannon, T. D., Cornblatt, B. A., McGlashan, T. H., & Addington, J. (2014). Perceived discrimination in those at clinical high risk for psychosis: Perceived discrimination in clinical high risk. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 8, 7781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schofield, P., Das-Munshi, J., Bécares, L., Morgan, C., Bhavsar, V., Hotopf, M., & Hatch, S. L. (2016). Minority status and mental distress: A comparison of group density effects. Psychological Medicine, 46, 30513059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, S. J., Unger, J. B., Zamboanga, B. L., & Szapocznik, J. (2010). Rethinking the concept of acculturation: Implications for theory and research. American Psychology, 65, 237251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, K. M., Saha, S., Lim, C. C. W., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Al-Hamzawi, A., Alonso, J., & McGrath, J. J. (2018). Psychotic experiences and general medical conditions: A cross-national analysis based on 28 002 respondents from 16 countries in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Psychological Medicine, 48, 27302739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selten, J.-P., van der Ven, E., Rutten, B. P. F., & Cantor-Graae, E. (2013). The social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia: An update. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39, 11801186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaikh, M., Ellett, L., Dutt, A., Day, F., Laing, J., Kroll, J., & Valmaggia, L. R. (2016). Perceived ethnic discrimination and persecutory paranoia in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Psychiatry Research, 241, 309314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharifi, V., Eaton, W. W., Wu, L. T., Roth, K. B., Burchett, B. M., & Mojtabai, R. (2015). Psychotic experiences and risk of death in the general population: 24–27 year follow-up of the Epidemiologic Catchment Area study. British Journal Psychiatry, 207, 3036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpley, M., Hutchinson, G., Murray, R. M., & McKenzie, K. (2001). Understanding the excess of psychosis among the African-Caribbean population in England. British Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 6068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephan, W. G., Ybarra, O., & Bachman, G. (1999). Prejudice toward immigrants. Journal of Applied SocialPsychology, 29, 22212237.Google Scholar
Stilo, S. A., Gayer-Anderson, C., Beards, S., Hubbard, K., Onyejiaka, A., Keraite, A., & Morgan, C. (2017). Further evidence of a cumulative effect of social disadvantage on risk of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 47, 913924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Straiton, M. L., Aambø, A. K., & Johansen, R. (2019). Perceived discrimination, health and mental health among immigrants in Norway: The role of moderating factors. BMC Public Health, 19, 325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sue, D. W., Capodilupo, C. M., & Holder, A. M. B. (2008). Racial microaggressions in the life experience of Black Americans. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 39, 329336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. (2001). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Hogg, M. A. & Abrams, D. (eds.), Intergroup relations: Essential readings, key readings in social psychology. New York, NY, US: Psychology Press, pp. 94109.Google Scholar
Taylor, D. M., Wright, S. C., Moghaddam, F. M., & Lalonde, R. N. (1990). The personal/group discrimination discrepancy: Perceiving my group, but not myself, to be a target for discrimination. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 254262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tortelli, A., Nakamura, A., Suprani, F., Schürhoff, F., Van der Waerden, J., Szöke, A., & Pignon, B. (2018). Subclinical psychosis in adult migrants and ethnic minorities: Systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry Open, 4, 510518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Triana, M. D. C., & Garcia, M. F. (2009). Valuing diversity: A group-value approach to understanding theimportance of organizational efforts to support diversity. Journal of Organizational Behavio, 30, 941962.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van de Beek, M. H., van der Krieke, L., Schoevers, R. A., & Veling, W. (2017). Social exclusion and psychopathology in an online cohort of Moroccan-Dutch migrants: Results of the MEDINA-study. PLoS ONE, 12, e0179827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Os, J., Linscott, R. J., Myin-Germeys, I., Delespaul, P., & Krabbendam, L. (2009). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: Evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychological Medicine, 39, 179195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veling, W. (2013). Ethnic minority position and risk for psychotic disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 26, 166171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veling, W., Hoek, H. W., & Mackenbach, J. P. (2008). Perceived discrimination and the risk of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities: A case-control study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43, 953959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veling, W., Selten, J.-P., Susser, E., Laan, W., Mackenbach, J. P., & Hoek, H. W. (2007). Discrimination and the incidence of psychotic disorders among ethnic minorities in The Netherlands. International Journal of Epidemiology, 36, 761768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Elm, E., Altman, D. G., Egger, M., Pocock, S. J., Gotzsche, P. C., Vandenbroucke, J. P., & STROBE Initiative. (2007). The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. Lancet (London, England) 370, 14531457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vremaroiu, A. M., & Fodoreanu, L. (2013). Perceived discrimination, a risk factor for developing psychoses in Romanian emigrants? Clujul Medical, 86, 5356.Google ScholarPubMed
Weisman de Mamani, A., Weintraub, M. J., Maura, J., Martinez de Andino, A., Brown, C. A., & Gurak, K. (2017). Acculturation styles and their associations with psychiatric symptoms and quality of life in ethnic minorities with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 255, 418423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickham, S., Taylor, P., Shevlin, M., & Bentall, R. P. (2014). The impact of social deprivation on paranoia, hallucinations, mania and depression: The role of discrimination social support, stress and trust. PLoS One, 9, e105140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yip, T., Wang, Y., Mootoo, C., & Mirpuri, S. (2019). Moderating the association between discrimination and adjustment: A meta-analysis of ethnic/racial identity. Developmental Psychology, 55, 12741298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zandi, T., Havenaar, J. M., Laan, W., Kahn, R. S., & van den Brink, W. (2011). Predictive validity of a culturally informed diagnosis of schizophrenia: A 30 month follow-up study with first episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 133, 2935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zandi, T., Havenaar, J. M., Limburg-Okken, A. G., van Es, H., Sidali, S., Kadri, N., & Kahn, R. S. (2008). The need for culture sensitive diagnostic procedures: A study among psychotic patients in Morocco. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatr Epidemiology, 43, 244250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zandi, T., Havenaar, J. M., Smits, M., Limburg-Okken, A. G., van Es, H., Cahn, W., & van den Brink, W. (2010). First contact incidence of psychotic disorders among native Dutch and Moroccan immigrants in the Netherlands: Influence of diagnostic bias. Schizophrenia Research, 119, 2733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Flow chart diagram of study selection according to the PRISMA guidelines.

Figure 1

Table 1. Characteristics of studies included in the systematic review

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Pooled meta-analytic odds ratios for the association between PED and PS.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Forest plot of pooled odds ratios for different ethnic sub-groups.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Funnel plot assessing publication bias for the association between PED and PS.