Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b95js Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:11:24.302Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Multivariate patterns of gray matter volume in thalamic nuclei are associated with positive schizotypy in healthy individuals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2019

Pasquale Di Carlo
Affiliation:
Group of Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience, and Sense Organs – University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus – Baltimore, MD, USA
Giulio Pergola
Affiliation:
Group of Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience, and Sense Organs – University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus – Baltimore, MD, USA
Linda A. Antonucci
Affiliation:
Group of Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience, and Sense Organs – University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy – Ludwig-Maximilians University, Munich, Germany
Aurora Bonvino
Affiliation:
Group of Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience, and Sense Organs – University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy IRCCS ‘Casa Sollievo della Sofferenza’, San Giovanni Rotondo, Italy
Marina Mancini
Affiliation:
Group of Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience, and Sense Organs – University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy
Tiziana Quarto
Affiliation:
Group of Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience, and Sense Organs – University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy
Antonio Rampino
Affiliation:
Group of Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience, and Sense Organs – University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Consorziale Policlinico, Bari, Italy
Teresa Popolizio
Affiliation:
IRCCS ‘Casa Sollievo della Sofferenza’, San Giovanni Rotondo, Italy
Alessandro Bertolino
Affiliation:
Group of Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience, and Sense Organs – University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Consorziale Policlinico, Bari, Italy
Giuseppe Blasi*
Affiliation:
Group of Psychiatric Neuroscience, Department of Basic Medical Science, Neuroscience, and Sense Organs – University of Bari Aldo Moro, Bari, Italy Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Consorziale Policlinico, Bari, Italy
*
Author for correspondence: Giuseppe Blasi, E-mail: giuseppe.blasi@uniba.it
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Previous models suggest biological and behavioral continua among healthy individuals (HC), at-risk condition, and full-blown schizophrenia (SCZ). Part of these continua may be captured by schizotypy, which shares subclinical traits and biological phenotypes with SCZ, including thalamic structural abnormalities. In this regard, previous findings have suggested that multivariate volumetric patterns of individual thalamic nuclei discriminate HC from SCZ. These results were obtained using machine learning, which allows case–control classification at the single-subject level. However, machine learning accuracy is usually unsatisfactory possibly due to phenotype heterogeneity. Indeed, a source of misclassification may be related to thalamic structural characteristics of those HC with high schizotypy, which may resemble structural abnormalities of SCZ. We hypothesized that thalamic structural heterogeneity is related to schizotypy, such that high schizotypal burden would implicate misclassification of those HC whose thalamic patterns resemble SCZ abnormalities.

Methods

Following a previous report, we used Random Forests to predict diagnosis in a case–control sample (SCZ = 131, HC = 255) based on thalamic nuclei gray matter volumes estimates. Then, we investigated whether the likelihood to be classified as SCZ (π-SCZ) was associated with schizotypy in 174 HC, evaluated with the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire.

Results

Prediction accuracy was 72.5%. Misclassified HC had higher positive schizotypy scores, which were correlated with π-SCZ. Results were specific to thalamic rather than whole-brain structural features.

Conclusions

These findings strengthen the relevance of thalamic structural abnormalities to SCZ and suggest that multivariate thalamic patterns are correlates of the continuum between schizotypy in HC and the full-blown disease.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Previous models posit a biological and behavioral continuum between conditions at greater risk for schizophrenia (SCZ) and the full-blown disorder (Barrantes-Vidal et al., Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Grant and Kwapil2015; Guloksuz and van Os, Reference Guloksuz and van Os2017). In this context, schizotypy refers to a set of temporally stable traits, continually distributed in the general population, that resemble, in attenuated forms, some of the symptoms of SCZ (Meehl, Reference Meehl1989; Tien, Reference Tien1991; Johns and van Os, Reference Johns and van Os2001; van Os et al., Reference van Os, Linscott, Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and Krabbendam2009; Ettinger et al., Reference Ettinger, Meyhofer, Steffens, Wagner and Koutsouleris2014). Previous studies revealed that schizotypal traits are more frequent in first-degree relatives of patients with SCZ when compared with the general population (Vollema and Postma, Reference Vollema and Postma2002; Vollema et al., Reference Vollema, Sitskoorn, Appels and Kahn2002), suggesting a link between schizotypy and familial risk for this brain disorder (Ericson et al., Reference Ericson, Tuvblad, Raine, Young-Wolff and Baker2011; Walter et al., Reference Walter, Fernandez, Snelling and Barkus2016). Other evidence suggests that schizotypy and SCZ share behavioral and biological correlates (Abi-Dargham et al., Reference Abi-Dargham, Kegeles, Zea-Ponce, Mawlawi, Martinez, Mitropoulou, O'Flynn, Koenigsberg, Van Heertum, Cooper, Laruelle and Siever2004; Fanous et al., Reference Fanous, Neale, Gardner, Webb, Straub, O'Neill, Walsh, Riley and Kendler2007; Taurisano et al., Reference Taurisano, Romano, Mancini, Di Giorgio, Antonucci, Fazio, Rampino, Quarto, Gelao, Porcelli, Papazacharias, Ursini, Caforio, Masellis, Niccoli-Asabella, Todarello, Popolizio, Rubini, Blasi and Bertolino2014). In fact, both patients with SCZ and healthy individuals with high schizotypy scores have deficits in sustained attention (Gooding et al., Reference Gooding, Matts and Rollmann2006) and working memory (Kerns and Becker, Reference Kerns and Becker2008), as well as brain structural abnormalities when compared with healthy controls (HC) (Kuhn et al., Reference Kuhn, Schubert and Gallinat2012). On this basis, schizotypy is widely recognized as a valuable construct for the investigation of biological correlates relevant to SCZ without the confounds and limitations related to disease state, such as pharmacological treatment, chronicity, and clinical symptoms (Barrantes-Vidal et al., Reference Barrantes-Vidal, Grant and Kwapil2015).

Pathophysiological models point out the thalamus as a pivotal node in the pathophysiology of SCZ (Andreasen et al., Reference Andreasen, Arndt, Swayze, Cizadlo, Flaum, O'Leary, Ehrhardt and Yuh1994; Peters et al., Reference Peters, Dunlop and Downar2016; Sherman, Reference Sherman2016). Consistently, several findings suggest that thalamic structural abnormality is a biological phenotype associated with both SCZ and schizotypy (Ettinger et al., Reference Ettinger, Meyhofer, Steffens, Wagner and Koutsouleris2014; Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Selvaggi, Trizio, Bertolino and Blasi2015). In particular, previous results indicate decreased thalamic volume in both conditions compared to HC (Byne et al., Reference Byne, Buchsbaum, Kemether, Hazlett, Shinwari, Mitropoulou and Siever2001; Kuhn et al., Reference Kuhn, Schubert and Gallinat2012; van Erp et al., Reference van Erp, Hibar, Rasmussen, Glahn, Pearlson, Andreassen, Agartz, Westlye, Haukvik, Dale, Melle, Hartberg, Gruber, Kraemer, Zilles, Donohoe, Kelly, McDonald, Morris, Cannon, Corvin, Machielsen, Koenders, de Haan, Veltman, Satterthwaite, Wolf, Gur, Gur, Potkin, Mathalon, Mueller, Preda, Macciardi, Ehrlich, Walton, Hass, Calhoun, Bockholt, Sponheim, Shoemaker, van Haren, Hulshoff Pol, Ophoff, Kahn, Roiz-Santianez, Crespo-Facorro, Wang, Alpert, Jonsson, Dimitrova, Bois, Whalley, McIntosh, Lawrie, Hashimoto, Thompson and Turner2016). However, the thalamus encompasses numerous nuclei anatomically segregated and belonging to independent brain circuits (Jones, Reference Jones2007). Accordingly, post-mortem studies show a decreased volume in specific nuclei in SCZ compared to HC (Byne et al., Reference Byne, Buchsbaum, Mattiace, Hazlett, Kemether, Elhakem, Purohit, Haroutunian and Jones2002, Reference Byne, Hazlett, Buchsbaum and Kemether2009). In particular, findings appear consistent for the pulvinar, whereas evidence is mixed for the other thalamic nuclei (Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Selvaggi, Trizio, Bertolino and Blasi2015; Dorph-Petersen and Lewis, Reference Dorph-Petersen and Lewis2017). Consistently with the post-mortem results, in vivo brain imaging findings suggest that specific thalamic nuclei contribute to thalamic volume shrinkage in SCZ (Cobia et al., Reference Cobia, Smith, Salinas, Ng, Gado, Csernansky and Wang2017; Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Trizio, Di Carlo, Taurisano, Mancini, Amoroso, Nettis, Andriola, Caforio, Popolizio, Rampino, Di Giorgio, Bertolino and Blasi2017). In fact, previous in vivo results in this brain disorder suggest a volume decrease in the pulvinar, as well as in anterior and mediodorsal nuclei (Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Selvaggi, Trizio, Bertolino and Blasi2015, Reference Pergola, Trizio, Di Carlo, Taurisano, Mancini, Amoroso, Nettis, Andriola, Caforio, Popolizio, Rampino, Di Giorgio, Bertolino and Blasi2017). Overall, post-mortem and in vivo brain imaging results highlight the relevance of nuclei-specific thalamic structural abnormalities in SCZ. Factors possibly confounding the findings include the complex architecture of thalamic nuclei and the heterogeneity of this biological phenotype. For example, HC included in between-groups comparisons might be characterized by biological and sub-clinical features progressively closer to those of SCZ. This heterogeneity may have hindered the characterization of phenotypic expression of thalamic structural abnormalities between HC and SCZ. Indeed, schizotypy in the healthy population may be relevant in this context.

An approach to investigate complex biological phenotypes is machine learning. This method allows classification and inference at the individual level (Zarogianni et al., Reference Zarogianni, Moorhead and Lawrie2013; Kambeitz et al., Reference Kambeitz, Kambeitz-Ilankovic, Leucht, Wood, Davatzikos, Malchow, Falkai and Koutsouleris2015). Additionally, it is a valuable tool to predict the likelihood of the allocation of each and every individual in a diagnostic group based on given characteristics. Thus, it may contribute to the understanding of individual heterogeneity based on imaging features (Rozycki et al., Reference Rozycki, Satterthwaite, Koutsouleris, Erus, Doshi, Wolf, Fan, Gur, Gur, Meisenzahl, Zhuo, Ying, Yan, Yue, Zhang and Davatzikos2017). Machine learning has been already used to investigate the relationship between SCZ and imaging phenotypes (Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, van Haren, Hulshoff Pol, Cahn, Kahn and Schnack2012; Salvador et al., Reference Salvador, Radua, Canales-Rodriguez, Solanes, Sarro, Goikolea, Valiente, Monte, Natividad, Guerrero-Pedraza, Moro, Fernandez-Corcuera, Amann, Maristany, Vieta, McKenna and Pomarol-Clotet2017), including those considering thalamic nuclei (Anticevic et al., Reference Anticevic, Cole, Repovs, Murray, Brumbaugh, Winkler, Savic, Krystal, Pearlson and Glahn2014). In this regard, our previous study (Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Trizio, Di Carlo, Taurisano, Mancini, Amoroso, Nettis, Andriola, Caforio, Popolizio, Rampino, Di Giorgio, Bertolino and Blasi2017) found that the multivariate volumetric patterns of thalamic nuclei discriminated HC from both SCZ and non-affected siblings, suggesting that the structural configuration of thalamic nuclei is a valid intermediate phenotype for SCZ.

Indeed, the results of this study and several others based on neuroimaging data indicated that about 25% of the subjects were misclassified by machine learning algorithms (Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, van Haren, Hulshoff Pol, Cahn, Kahn and Schnack2012; Rozycki et al., Reference Rozycki, Satterthwaite, Koutsouleris, Erus, Doshi, Wolf, Fan, Gur, Gur, Meisenzahl, Zhuo, Ying, Yan, Yue, Zhang and Davatzikos2017; Salvador et al., Reference Salvador, Radua, Canales-Rodriguez, Solanes, Sarro, Goikolea, Valiente, Monte, Natividad, Guerrero-Pedraza, Moro, Fernandez-Corcuera, Amann, Maristany, Vieta, McKenna and Pomarol-Clotet2017; Schwarz et al., Reference Schwarz, Doan, Pergola, Westlye, Kaufmann, Wolfers, Brecheisen, Quarto, Ing, Di Carlo, Gurholt, Harms, Noirhomme, Moberget, Agartz, Andreassen, Bellani, Bertolino, Blasi, Brambilla, Buitelaar, Cervenka, Flyckt, Frangou, Franke, Hall, Heslenfeld, Kirsch, McIntosh, Nothen, Papassotiropoulos, de Quervain, Rietschel, Schumann, Tost, Witt, Zink and Meyer-Lindenberg2019). It is possible that the thalamic structure of those HC matching some of the criteria to identify schizotypy may have contributed to the misclassification. In other words, it is possible that the higher the level of schizotypy in HC, the closer the multivariate thalamic volumetric pattern of HC to that of SCZ, the greater the likelihood of misclassification of HC as SCZ.

The aim of this study was to investigate whether schizotypy is related to the variability in multivariate volumetric patterns of thalamic nuclei, such that it contributes to the misclassification of HC as SCZ. With this aim, first we parceled out the thalamus in different nuclei subdivisions (Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Trizio, Di Carlo, Taurisano, Mancini, Amoroso, Nettis, Andriola, Caforio, Popolizio, Rampino, Di Giorgio, Bertolino and Blasi2017). Then, we trained a machine learning algorithm that discriminated HC from SCZ based on multivariate volumetric patterns of thalamic nuclei. Thus, we tested whether schizotypy contributes to misclassification of HC as SCZ. Furthermore, we explored whether the discrimination between these groups of individuals was specific for thalamic features. With this purpose, we used a neuroanatomical brain atlas encompassing the whole brain and verified whether schizotypy contributes to the misclassification of this algorithm. We hypothesized that HC with relatively high schizotypy scores may present thalamic patterns prone to misclassification as SCZ.

Methods

Participants

Demographics are reported in Table 1. We recruited 386 Caucasian individuals: 131 SCZ (DSM-IV-TR) selected among consecutive outpatients at the University Hospital of Bari, and 255 HC. The sample partially overlaps with that used in a previous work (Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Trizio, Di Carlo, Taurisano, Mancini, Amoroso, Nettis, Andriola, Caforio, Popolizio, Rampino, Di Giorgio, Bertolino and Blasi2017) [84/131 SCZ – ≈64%; 88/255 HC – ≈35%, online Supplementary Material (SM) section 1.1]. Exclusion criteria for all individuals were history of drug or alcohol abuse in the past year, non-psychiatric clinically relevant conditions, history of neurological diseases and head trauma with loss of consciousness. Absence of psychiatric illness in HC was established using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). Family history of psychiatric disorders was an exclusion criterion for HC. At the time of scan acquisition, all patients were on stable treatment with first and/or second-generation antipsychotics since at least four weeks. All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, revised in 2008. All participants gave their informed consent. All procedures were approved by the ethics committee at Bari University Hospital.

Table 1. Demographics of the sample used in this study

HC, healthy controls; SCZ, patients with schizophrenia; M, male; F, female; s.d., standard deviation; SES, socio-economic status; SPQ, Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire.

Demographics, neuropsychological, and clinical assessment

Participants were evaluated for handedness with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, Reference Oldfield1971) and for socio-economic status (SES) (Hollingshead, Reference Hollingshead1975). The SES considers the occupational status and the educational level of both parents. The Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) (Raine, Reference Raine1991) was administered to HC. The SPQ is a 74-item validated self-report questionnaire with a ‘yes/no’ response format for the assessment of Schizotypal Personality. We used a three-factor model including positive, negative, and disorganized factors (Vollema and Hoijtink, Reference Vollema and Hoijtink2000). Non-complete questionnaires were excluded. One hundred and seventy-four HC with complete SPQ evaluation were used for the association analyses between SPQ scores and machine learning outcomes. Demographics and SPQ scores of this sample are reported in Table 1 and eTable 1. To investigate homogeneity between levels of schizotypy in our cohort with those of larger populations, SPQ scores in our sample were compared with those of a previous study including healthy individuals (Fonseca-Pedrero et al., Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Debbane, Ortuno-Sierra, Chan, Cicero, Zhang, Brenner, Barkus, Linscott, Kwapil, Barrantes-Vidal, Cohen, Raine, Compton, Tone, Suhr, Muniz, Fumero, Giakoumaki, Tsaousis, Preti, Chmielewski, Laloyaux, Mechri, Lahmar, Wuthrich, Laroi, Badcock and Jablensky2018). SM 1.2 and eTable 1 report statistics on this comparison.

Imaging data acquisition and preprocessing

Structural magnetic resonance imaging data were acquired with a General Electric (Milwaukee, WI, USA) 3 Tesla whole-body scanner using a standard quadrature head coil. We used a whole-brain T1 inversion recovery fast spoiled gradient recalled sequence with the following parameters: TR = 26 ms/TE = 3 ms/NEX = 1; flip angle 6°; bandwidth 31.25; field of view 250 mm; matrix size 256 × 256; 124 contiguous 1.3 mm thickness axial slices; voxel size = 0.9 × 0.9 × 1.3 mm; acquisition time 6′08″. Images included in the study were free of neurological abnormalities (assessed by TP, a board-certified neuro-radiologist), acquisition, and segmentation artifacts (SM 1.3). Data were pre-processed with SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Gray matter and white matter images were normalized using DARTEL and re-sampled to 1.1 mm3 isotropic voxels, in order to soften potential issues related to non-isotropic acquisition of images (Mechelli et al., Reference Mechelli, Price, Friston and Ashburner2005).

Brain features extraction for machine learning multivariate analyses

We performed two different regions of interest (ROIs) analyses using two sets of brain features. The first set encompassed the thalamus, taking into account its subdivision in different nuclei. The second set encompassed the entire brain and was used as a negative control to suggest specificity of the results for multivariate volumetric patterns of thalamic nuclei. These two sets of brain features were obtained as follows:

  1. (1) Thalamic Subdivisions (ThSub). We used an already published procedure (Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Trizio, Di Carlo, Taurisano, Mancini, Amoroso, Nettis, Andriola, Caforio, Popolizio, Rampino, Di Giorgio, Bertolino and Blasi2017) based on the ‘Thalamus atlas’ (Krauth et al., Reference Krauth, Blanc, Poveda, Jeanmonod, Morel and Szekely2010; Jakab et al., Reference Jakab, Blanc, Berenyi and Szekely2012) to obtain gray matter volume estimates (GMV) of the whole thalamus and seven thalamic nuclei bilaterally (anterior/midline nuclei; mediodorsal thalamic nucleus; intralaminar nuclei; ventrolateral nucleus; ventral anterior region; geniculate nuclei; pulvinar; see SM 1.4).

  2. (2) Automated Anatomic Labeling (AAL). We used GMV estimates of 106 ROIs available in the Automated Anatomical Labeling atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., Reference Tzourio-Mazoyer, Landeau, Papathanassiou, Crivello, Etard, Delcroix, Mazoyer and Joliot2002). We excluded thalamic ROIs to have a spatially independent control set.

GMV estimates of each ROI were marginalized for gender, linear as well as quadratic terms of age, and total brain GMV estimate. Linear model residuals of each ROI were used as features in the following multivariate classification analyses.

Machine learning multivariate classification

We separately used each set of brain features to classify participants – HC v. SCZ – taking advantage of a machine learning approach. We used Random Forests (Breiman, Reference Breiman2001) to train two independent classifiers based on the ThSub and AAL brain features. We used the caret R package v.6.0–77 (Kuhn, Reference Kuhn2008) to implement a nested-designed supervised model (SM 1.5). All the analyses were performed with R 3.4.1. We used a nested-designed framework to separate train and test sets, to cross-validate the train set, and to estimate accuracy in the test set. Critically, the test set is independent of the train set since subjects have not been used to build the classifier rules (Bzdok and Meyer-Lindenberg, Reference Bzdok and Meyer-Lindenberg2018). To further control sampling bias, we computed model performance as the average test set accuracy and its standard deviation over re-samplings of the train set (permutations = 1000). Furthermore, we also computed sensitivity, specificity, positive, and negative predictive values. Moreover, we computed feature importance in discriminating HC v. SCZ (SM 1.5). To assess the statistical significance of the classification performance of both classifiers, we permuted diagnostic labels of each classifier to generate the correspondent random null distribution of classification accuracy (permutations = 1000). We defined the empirical p value as the number of times the accuracies in the null distribution are greater than the average accuracy of the true classifier, divided by the number of permutations. Moreover, to quantify the magnitude of the difference between the true accuracy distribution and its null distribution, we computed the Cohen's d confidence interval 95% (CI95).

To obtain an index of the likelihood of a subject to be classified as SCZ, we defined the global classification score (π-SCZ) for each subject as the average of Random Forests classification scores (SM 1.5). Note that classification scores were computed in the test set at each re-sampling, thus providing an unbiased prediction estimate. π-SCZ ranges between 0 and 1 and is an index of the likelihood to be classified as SCZ (0 = highest likelihood to be classified as HC; 1 = highest likelihood to be classified as SCZ). When π-SCZ ⩾ 0.5, the subject was classified as SCZ, otherwise was classified as HC.

Importantly, the homogeneity of the feature set is a necessary assumption to consider π-SCZ dependent on the phenotype under investigation. On this basis, in the present study, we did not consider premorbid intelligence as a feature of interest in our primary analysis, as we did in a previous investigation (Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Trizio, Di Carlo, Taurisano, Mancini, Amoroso, Nettis, Andriola, Caforio, Popolizio, Rampino, Di Giorgio, Bertolino and Blasi2017). We adopted this approach because it would have generated a different set of classification rules accounting for premorbid intelligence–thalamic interactions. Thus, π-SCZ would have been dependent on a multivariate space not specifically related to thalamic patterns only. However, we included premorbid intelligence in a supplementary analysis to control for its effect on classification (SM 1.6).

Following analyses were based on the association between HC misclassification and schizotypy. In particular, we aimed to investigate whether differences in the performance of ThSub- v. AAL-based algorithms might have driven the association between HC misclassification and schizotypy. With this purpose, we checked whether the two algorithms outperformed each other in terms of specificity (χ2-test), considering both the whole sample and those with complete SPQ evaluation. Then, we explored the overlap between predictions of the two classifiers that we quantified as the percentage of subjects attributed to the same class. In this way, despite of almost equivalent classification performances, different algorithms may correctly classify divergent groups of subjects, which may have specific phenotypic attributes. Furthermore, we investigated the algorithm performance stability running additional analyses including only HC with SPQ and adopting a dimensionality reduction technique for the AAL dataset (SM 1.6). Finally, we sought to replicate previous findings (Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Trizio, Di Carlo, Taurisano, Mancini, Amoroso, Nettis, Andriola, Caforio, Popolizio, Rampino, Di Giorgio, Bertolino and Blasi2017) including univariate t tests between individual thalamic nuclei GMV and diagnosis (SM 1.7).

Relevance of schizotypy to misclassification and classification scores

We investigated how schizotypy affected the HC v. SCZ classification outcome based on the multivariate structural patterns of thalamic nuclei. With this aim, we assessed the association of levels of schizotypy with the likelihood to be classified as SCZ (π-SCZ). As we specified above, π-SCZ closer to 1 indicated greater probability for an SCZ-like pattern, while π-SCZ close to 0 indicated greater probability for an HC-like pattern. Thus, we considered true negatives (TN, i.e. HC classified as such) the HC with a π-SCZ < 0.5, and false positives (FP, i.e. HC erroneously classified as SCZ) the HC with a π-SCZ ⩾ 0.5. Then, we performed an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with FP/TN as the independent variable. SPQ total scores and SPQ factor scores were used as the dependent variables in separate models, with age, gender, handedness, and SES as covariates. Furthermore, we explored the association of π-SCZ with SPQ scores performing multiple regressions with the same covariates.

To test the specificity of the findings for multivariate structural patterns of thalamic nuclei compared to whole-brain features, we repeated all these analyses using the classification outcome based on AAL. Results were Bonferroni corrected considering the four SPQ and the two machine learning analyses (n = 2 × 4 = 8, α = 0.00625). Effect sizes are reported as partial-r 2. To further support the relevance of multivariate patterns, we checked whether SPQ scores are individually correlated with thalamic nuclei GMV (SM 1.7).

Relevance of demographics to misclassification and classification scores

We verified whether misclassification and classification scores were associated with demographics to investigate other plausible sources of misattribution (SM 1.8).

Results

Machine learning multivariate classification

Table 2 reports machine learning performance statistics. The ThSub algorithm based on the thalamic features yielded an accuracy of 72.5% (empirical p value < 0.001, Cohen's d CI95 2.34–2.57; Fig. 1a). The AAL algorithm based on whole-brain features yielded an accuracy of 67.6% (empirical p value = 0.018, Cohen's d CI95 1.44–1.64; Fig. 1b). Despite almost equivalent prediction accuracies, the overlap among classifiers prediction at the single-subject level was 61.1%. Bilateral mediodorsal and anterior-midline nuclei showed both high multivariate feature relevance (empirical p value < 0.0001) and association with diagnosis (corrected p value < 0.05; eTable 2). Performance remained stable through sensitivity analyses (eTable 3). Notably, specificity values did not differ (χ2 = 0.3, p value = 0.57).

Fig. 1. Density plots of Random Forests classification accuracies. Density curves compare classification accuracy (x-axis) distribution over 1000 re-sampling between (a) the Thalamic Subdivisions features (ThSub) or (b) the Automated Anatomic Labeling whole-brain features (ALL) with the respective null distribution obtained through permutation of diagnostic labels.

Table 2. Machine learning classification performances

Table reports statistics of the classification performance (HC v. SCZ) obtained by the Thalamic Subdivisions (ThSub) and the Automated Anatomic Labeling (AAL) Random Forests classifiers. CI95, 95% confidence interval; NPV, negative predictive value; PPV, positive predictive value; s.d., standard deviation.

Misclassification and classification scores are related to positive schizotypy

Decomposing the performance of the ThSub classifier, we found that 42 out of 174 HC were misattributed to SCZ diagnosis (FP, specificity = 75.9%). ANCOVA indicated that FP scored higher for the total SPQ score (F 1,169 = 4.4, p value = 0.037) compared to TN. Analysis of the three SPQ subscales indicated greater scores in FP for the positive schizotypy subscale (F 1,169 = 9.5, corrected p value = 0.019, partial-r 2 = 0.053; Table 3; Fig. 2a) but not for the negative (p value = 0.073) and disorganized subscales (p value = 0.46). Furthermore, there was a positive association between π-SCZ and positive schizotypy (t 169 = 2.9, corrected p value = 0.038, partial-r 2 = 0.046; Table 3; Fig. 2b). This association was not significant for total SPQ, negative, and disorganized factors (p values > 0.1).

Fig. 2. Relevance of positive schizotypy for healthy controls classification. (a) Boxplot showing that false positives (FP, healthy controls misclassified to SCZ diagnosis) have higher positive schizotypy scores than true negatives (TN, healthy controls correctly classified). (b) Scatterplot showing the relationship between the likelihood to be classified as SCZ (π-SCZ) based on multivariate volumetric patterns of thalamic nuclei and positive schizotypy scores. Shaded area indicates 95% confidence interval.

Table 3. Association of classifiers outcome with SPQ scores and demographics in healthy controls

Table reports test statistics and uncorrected p values: *p value < 0.05; **p values surviving multiple comparisons threshold (α = 0.00625) in bold font.

ThSub, Thalamic Subdivisions; AAL, Automated Anatomic Labeling; SPQ, Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire.

All these analyses were not significant when using AAL (specificity = 82.2%) as the classifier of interest. In particular, FP were not associated with higher SPQ scores when compared with TN, nor π-SCZ was associated with SPQ scores (p values > 0.05; Table 3). Again, specificity values did not differ among classifiers (χ2 = 1.7, p value = 0.19), suggesting that the results are not dependent on algorithm performance. Moreover, SPQ scores were not associated with individual thalamic nuclei GMV (p values > 0.1).

Misclassification and classification scores are not associated with demographics

FP and TN did not differ in terms of sex, age, SES, and handedness when using the ThSub classifier (p values > 0.1; Table 3). FP had higher right-hand scores compared to TN when using the AAL classifier (p value = 0.0013). π-SCZ was not associated with demographics (p values > 0.05; Table 3).

Discussion

In the present study, we found that multivariate structural patterns of thalamic nuclei allow discrimination between healthy subjects and patients with SCZ. Furthermore, we found that high positive schizotypy confers to HC proneness to misclassification as SCZ based on such thalamic features. Overall, these findings suggest the relevance of multivariate structural thalamic patterns as a biological phenotype of SCZ. Furthermore, they suggest that an increase in the levels of schizotypy in HC implicates greater similarity between their thalamic structural patterns and those of SCZ. This biological configuration may result in misattribution of HC to the clinical population by a machine learning algorithm.

These findings are consistent with the previous reports describing thalamic structural abnormalities in HC with high schizotypy scores (Byne et al., Reference Byne, Buchsbaum, Kemether, Hazlett, Shinwari, Mitropoulou and Siever2001; Takahashi et al., Reference Takahashi, Suzuki, Zhou, Nakamura, Tanino, Kawasaki, Seal, Seto and Kurachi2008; Kuhn et al., Reference Kuhn, Schubert and Gallinat2012), first-degree relatives (Byne et al., Reference Byne, Hazlett, Buchsbaum and Kemether2009; Okada et al., Reference Okada, Fukunaga, Yamashita, Koshiyama, Yamamori, Ohi, Yasuda, Fujimoto, Watanabe, Yahata, Nemoto, Hibar, van Erp, Fujino, Isobe, Isomura, Natsubori, Narita, Hashimoto, Miyata, Koike, Takahashi, Yamasue, Matsuo, Onitsuka, Iidaka, Kawasaki, Yoshimura, Watanabe, Suzuki, Turner, Takeda, Thompson, Ozaki, Kasai and Hashimoto2016; Pergola et al., Reference Pergola, Trizio, Di Carlo, Taurisano, Mancini, Amoroso, Nettis, Andriola, Caforio, Popolizio, Rampino, Di Giorgio, Bertolino and Blasi2017), and SCZ compared to HC (Glahn et al., Reference Glahn, Laird, Ellison-Wright, Thelen, Robinson, Lancaster, Bullmore and Fox2008; Fornito et al., Reference Fornito, Yucel, Patti, Wood and Pantelis2009; van Erp et al., Reference van Erp, Hibar, Rasmussen, Glahn, Pearlson, Andreassen, Agartz, Westlye, Haukvik, Dale, Melle, Hartberg, Gruber, Kraemer, Zilles, Donohoe, Kelly, McDonald, Morris, Cannon, Corvin, Machielsen, Koenders, de Haan, Veltman, Satterthwaite, Wolf, Gur, Gur, Potkin, Mathalon, Mueller, Preda, Macciardi, Ehrlich, Walton, Hass, Calhoun, Bockholt, Sponheim, Shoemaker, van Haren, Hulshoff Pol, Ophoff, Kahn, Roiz-Santianez, Crespo-Facorro, Wang, Alpert, Jonsson, Dimitrova, Bois, Whalley, McIntosh, Lawrie, Hashimoto, Thompson and Turner2016). Furthermore, they are consistent with the evidence of anomalies in thalamo-cortical circuitry in SCZ (Lynall et al., Reference Lynall, Bassett, Kerwin, McKenna, Kitzbichler, Muller and Bullmore2010; van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Mandl, Stam, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2010; Salomon et al., Reference Salomon, Bleich-Cohen, Hahamy-Dubossarsky, Dinstien, Weizman, Poyurovsky, Kupchik, Kotler, Hendler and Malach2011; Anticevic et al., Reference Anticevic, Cole, Repovs, Murray, Brumbaugh, Winkler, Savic, Krystal, Pearlson and Glahn2014; Woodward et al., Reference Woodward, Leong, Sanford, Tipper and Lavigne2016) and in individuals at familial risk for this disorder (Anticevic et al., Reference Anticevic, Haut, Murray, Repovs, Yang, Diehl, McEwen, Bearden, Addington, Goodyear, Cadenhead, Mirzakhanian, Cornblatt, Olvet, Mathalon, McGlashan, Perkins, Belger, Seidman, Tsuang, van Erp, Walker, Hamann, Woods, Qiu and Cannon2015; Antonucci et al., Reference Antonucci, Taurisano, Fazio, Gelao, Romano, Quarto, Porcelli, Mancini, Di Giorgio, Caforio, Pergola, Popolizio, Bertolino and Blasi2016). Moreover, they are in line with the psychosis dimensional model, which posits a continuous phenotypic variation in the general population with patients laying at the extreme of the phenotypic distribution (Claridge, Reference Claridge and Claridge1997; Ettinger et al., Reference Ettinger, Mohr, Gooding, Cohen, Rapp, Haenschel and Park2015; Lenzenweger, Reference Lenzenweger2015). Within this framework, they suggest that multivariate thalamic structural patterns are biological correlates of a continuum between schizotypy and SCZ.

Interestingly, we found that the thalamic-based misclassification is specifically related to the cognitive-perceptual (i.e. positive) domain of schizotypy. This relationship is consistent with the models involving thalamic nuclei in salience (Gilbert and Sigman, Reference Gilbert and Sigman2007; Peters et al., Reference Peters, Dunlop and Downar2016) and in the regulation of high-order cognitive functioning (Sherman, Reference Sherman2016). In SCZ, abnormalities of the structure of the thalamus (Andreasen et al., Reference Andreasen, Arndt, Swayze, Cizadlo, Flaum, O'Leary, Ehrhardt and Yuh1994) may implicate abnormal integration of perceptual inputs and sensory gating, which may result in positive symptoms (Pynn and DeSouza, Reference Pynn and DeSouza2013; Vukadinovic, Reference Vukadinovic2014), and in cognitive anomalies related to the disruption of thalamo-cortical reciprocal connections (Gilbert and Sigman, Reference Gilbert and Sigman2007; Bolkan et al., Reference Bolkan, Stujenske, Parnaudeau, Spellman, Rauffenbart, Abbas, Harris, Gordon and Kellendonk2017; Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Wimmer, Nakajima, Happ, Mofakham and Halassa2017). In this perspective, a possible speculation on our findings is that a thalamic volumetric configuration related to schizotypy may contribute to cognitive-perceptual sub-clinical phenomena lying in a continuum with symptoms of full-blown SCZ. Future brain imaging studies might investigate this topic from a longitudinal perspective (Ferguson and Gao, Reference Ferguson and Gao2014).

Interestingly, we did not find the association between schizotypy and misclassification based on whole-brain features. Moreover, we found that the individuals correctly discriminated by ThSub only partially overlap with those correctly discriminated by AAL classifier. These results suggest that there is no relationship between multivariate whole-brain features and schizotypy. Indeed, this construct appears more specifically related to the thalamic structural patterns.

The HC/SCZ classification accuracy of the present study was comparable with those found in other reports using neuroimaging brain features only (Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, van Haren, Hulshoff Pol, Cahn, Kahn and Schnack2012; Anticevic et al., Reference Anticevic, Cole, Repovs, Murray, Brumbaugh, Winkler, Savic, Krystal, Pearlson and Glahn2014; Rozycki et al., Reference Rozycki, Satterthwaite, Koutsouleris, Erus, Doshi, Wolf, Fan, Gur, Gur, Meisenzahl, Zhuo, Ying, Yan, Yue, Zhang and Davatzikos2017; Salvador et al., Reference Salvador, Radua, Canales-Rodriguez, Solanes, Sarro, Goikolea, Valiente, Monte, Natividad, Guerrero-Pedraza, Moro, Fernandez-Corcuera, Amann, Maristany, Vieta, McKenna and Pomarol-Clotet2017), confirming the potential role of the thalamus as a disease biomarker (Anticevic et al., Reference Anticevic, Cole, Repovs, Murray, Brumbaugh, Winkler, Savic, Krystal, Pearlson and Glahn2014; Pratt et al., Reference Pratt, Morris, Dawson, Pratt and Hall2018). Differently, other approaches (Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, Schnack, van Haren, Lappin, Morgan, Reinders, Gutierrez-Tordesillas, Roiz-Santianez, Schaufelberger, Rosa, Zanetti, Busatto, Crespo-Facorro, McGorry, Velakoulis, Pantelis, Wood, Kahn, Mourao-Miranda and Dazzan2017; Dwyer et al., Reference Dwyer, Cabral, Kambeitz-Ilankovic, Sanfelici, Kambeitz, Calhoun, Falkai, Pantelis, Meisenzahl and Koutsouleris2018) model simultaneously neuroanatomical and behavioral data to predict diagnosis or SCZ development in cohorts of individuals at high risk (Zarogianni et al., Reference Zarogianni, Storkey, Johnstone, Owens and Lawrie2017). However, while the latter approaches point to improve prediction accuracy by adding more layers of information, here we investigated possible sources of misclassification associated with inter-individual phenotypic heterogeneity of thalamic nuclei. Indeed, prediction accuracy of multivariate predictive models remains steadily between 70% and 80% in the psychiatric field (Nieuwenhuis et al., Reference Nieuwenhuis, Schnack, van Haren, Lappin, Morgan, Reinders, Gutierrez-Tordesillas, Roiz-Santianez, Schaufelberger, Rosa, Zanetti, Busatto, Crespo-Facorro, McGorry, Velakoulis, Pantelis, Wood, Kahn, Mourao-Miranda and Dazzan2017; Rozycki et al., Reference Rozycki, Satterthwaite, Koutsouleris, Erus, Doshi, Wolf, Fan, Gur, Gur, Meisenzahl, Zhuo, Ying, Yan, Yue, Zhang and Davatzikos2017; Salvador et al., Reference Salvador, Radua, Canales-Rodriguez, Solanes, Sarro, Goikolea, Valiente, Monte, Natividad, Guerrero-Pedraza, Moro, Fernandez-Corcuera, Amann, Maristany, Vieta, McKenna and Pomarol-Clotet2017; Schwarz et al., Reference Schwarz, Doan, Pergola, Westlye, Kaufmann, Wolfers, Brecheisen, Quarto, Ing, Di Carlo, Gurholt, Harms, Noirhomme, Moberget, Agartz, Andreassen, Bellani, Bertolino, Blasi, Brambilla, Buitelaar, Cervenka, Flyckt, Frangou, Franke, Hall, Heslenfeld, Kirsch, McIntosh, Nothen, Papassotiropoulos, de Quervain, Rietschel, Schumann, Tost, Witt, Zink and Meyer-Lindenberg2019). The reasons behind this boundary merit further investigation. In this regard, we excluded that the demographical variables collected in this study were a source of misclassification, confounding machine learning outcome.

A limitation of this study is that it is based on the dimensional model of schizotypy in continuum with SCZ (Ettinger et al., Reference Ettinger, Mohr, Gooding, Cohen, Rapp, Haenschel and Park2015). However, the alternative categorical model (Meehl, Reference Meehl1989; Lenzenweger, Reference Lenzenweger2015) should be considered for future investigations. Moreover, SPQ scores in our sample are lower when compared with other healthy populations (Fonseca-Pedrero et al., Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Debbane, Ortuno-Sierra, Chan, Cicero, Zhang, Brenner, Barkus, Linscott, Kwapil, Barrantes-Vidal, Cohen, Raine, Compton, Tone, Suhr, Muniz, Fumero, Giakoumaki, Tsaousis, Preti, Chmielewski, Laloyaux, Mechri, Lahmar, Wuthrich, Laroi, Badcock and Jablensky2018) and show reduced variability (SM 1.2 and eTable 1). This is particularly true for the disorganized factor: thus, the non-significant association may reflect a floor effect. Another limitation is that HC and SCZ were not homogeneous for age, gender, and SES. We considered the effects of age and gender on GMV estimates and did not find any association of SES with classification outcomes. Indeed, a full match of these variables between groups would have dramatically decreased the sample size. A further limitation is that machine learning algorithms could be theoretically confounded by a large number of individual attributes that have not been collected in this study. Future machine learning studies should assess further sources of misclassification and whether risk- or resilience-related structural variations for psychosis may be present in the brain. Finally, intra-diagnostic variability merits further investigation. Here, we did not collect SPQ for SCZ. Although schizotypy construct may be valid also in patients, literature is mixed on this topic (Brosey and Woodward, Reference Brosey and Woodward2015; Cicero et al., Reference Cicero, Jonas, Li, Perlman and Kotov2019).

In conclusion, the findings of the present study suggest that the multivariate signatures of thalamic nuclei structure relate to positive schizotypy in healthy individuals and strengthen the role of the thalamus in cognitive-perceptual disturbances related to SCZ. Future studies are needed to fully investigate the potential of this topic and further disentangle how structural thalamic abnormalities are key for the pathophysiology of this brain disorder.

Author ORCIDs

Pasquale Di Carlo, 0000-0002-5150-5274; Giulio Pergola, 0000-0002-9193-1841; Linda A. Antonucci, 0000-0002-7919-7402; Aurora Bonvino, 0000-0001-8772-5992; Marina Mancini, 0000-0003-4175-8769; Antonio Rampino, 0000-0002-9654-3266; Teresa Popolizio, 0000-0002-9364-9663; Giuseppe Blasi, 0000-0003-0878-1131.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719001430

Acknowledgements

The license for use of the ‘Thalamus Atlas’ has been kindly provided to GP by Professor Gabor Szekely, Swiss Federal Institute of Technology, Zurich (Switzerland). This work has been possible thanks to the contribution to data collection kindly provided by Dr Grazia Caforio, Dr Leonardo Fazio, Dr Barbara Gelao, Dr Annamaria Porcelli, Dr Raffaella Romano, Dr Paolo Taurisano, Dr Luisa Longo. We gratefully acknowledge Marina Cariello who contributed to data analysis.

Financial support

European Union Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development, and demonstration under grant agreement no. 602450 (IMAGEMEND) awarded to AB. GP's position is funded by the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 798181 (FLOURISH). This paper reflects only the authors' views and the European Union and Research Executive Agency are not liable for any use that may be made of the information contained therein.

Conflict of interest

GB received lecture fees from Jannsen and Lundbeck. AB received lecture fees from Otsuka, Jannsen, Lundbeck, and consultant fees from Biogen. AR received travel fees from Lundbeck. All other authors have no biomedical financial interests and no potential conflicts of interest.

References

Abi-Dargham, A, Kegeles, LS, Zea-Ponce, Y, Mawlawi, O, Martinez, D, Mitropoulou, V, O'Flynn, K, Koenigsberg, HW, Van Heertum, R, Cooper, T, Laruelle, M and Siever, LJ (2004) Striatal amphetamine-induced dopamine release in patients with schizotypal personality disorder studied with single photon emission computed tomography and [123I]iodobenzamide. Biological Psychiatry 55, 10011006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, NC, Arndt, S, Swayze, V, Cizadlo, T II, Flaum, M, O'Leary, D, Ehrhardt, JC and Yuh, WT (1994) Thalamic abnormalities in schizophrenia visualized through magnetic resonance image averaging. Science 266, 294298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anticevic, A, Cole, MW, Repovs, G, Murray, JD, Brumbaugh, MS, Winkler, AM, Savic, A, Krystal, JH, Pearlson, GD and Glahn, DC (2014) Characterizing thalamo-cortical disturbances in schizophrenia and bipolar illness. Cerebral Cortex 24, 31163130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anticevic, A, Haut, K, Murray, JD, Repovs, G, Yang, GJ, Diehl, C, McEwen, SC, Bearden, CE, Addington, J, Goodyear, B, Cadenhead, KS, Mirzakhanian, H, Cornblatt, BA, Olvet, D, Mathalon, DH, McGlashan, TH, Perkins, DO, Belger, A, Seidman, LJ, Tsuang, MT, van Erp, TG, Walker, EF, Hamann, S, Woods, SW, Qiu, M and Cannon, TD (2015) Association of thalamic dysconnectivity and conversion to psychosis in youth and young adults at elevated clinical risk. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 882891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antonucci, LA, Taurisano, P, Fazio, L, Gelao, B, Romano, R, Quarto, T, Porcelli, A, Mancini, M, Di Giorgio, A, Caforio, G, Pergola, G, Popolizio, T, Bertolino, A and Blasi, G (2016) Association of familial risk for schizophrenia with thalamic and medial prefrontal functional connectivity during attentional control. Schizophrenia Research 173, 2329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrantes-Vidal, N, Grant, P and Kwapil, TR (2015) The role of schizotypy in the study of the etiology of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41 (Suppl. 2), S408S416.10.1093/schbul/sbu191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolkan, SS, Stujenske, JM, Parnaudeau, S, Spellman, TJ, Rauffenbart, C, Abbas, AI, Harris, AZ, Gordon, JA and Kellendonk, C (2017) Thalamic projections sustain prefrontal activity during working memory maintenance. Nature Neuroscience 20, 987996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breiman, L (2001) Random forests. Machine Learning 45, 532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brosey, E and Woodward, ND (2015) Schizotypy and clinical symptoms, cognitive function, and quality of life in individuals with a psychotic disorder. Schizophrenia Research 166, 9297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byne, W, Buchsbaum, MS, Kemether, E, Hazlett, EA, Shinwari, A, Mitropoulou, V and Siever, LJ (2001) Magnetic resonance imaging of the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus and pulvinar in schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder. Archives Of General Psychiatry 58, 133140.10.1001/archpsyc.58.2.133CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byne, W, Buchsbaum, MS, Mattiace, LA, Hazlett, EA, Kemether, E, Elhakem, SL, Purohit, DP, Haroutunian, V and Jones, L (2002) Postmortem assessment of thalamic nuclear volumes in subjects with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 5965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byne, W, Hazlett, EA, Buchsbaum, MS and Kemether, E (2009) The thalamus and schizophrenia: current status of research. Acta Neuropathologica 117, 347368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bzdok, D and Meyer-Lindenberg, A (2018) Machine learning for precision psychiatry: opportunities and challenges. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging 3, 223230.Google ScholarPubMed
Cicero, DC, Jonas, KG, Li, K, Perlman, G and Kotov, R (2019) Common taxonomy of traits and symptoms: linking schizophrenia symptoms, schizotypy, and normal personality. Schizophrenia Bulletin (published online ahead of print 9 February).10.1093/schbul/sbz005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Claridge, G (1997) Theoretical background and issues. In Claridge, G (ed.), Schizotypy: Implications for Illness and Health. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, pp. 318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cobia, DJ, Smith, MJ, Salinas, I, Ng, C, Gado, M, Csernansky, JG and Wang, L (2017) Progressive deterioration of thalamic nuclei relates to cortical network decline in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 180, 2127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorph-Petersen, KA and Lewis, DA (2017) Postmortem structural studies of the thalamus in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 180, 2835.10.1016/j.schres.2016.08.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dwyer, DB, Cabral, C, Kambeitz-Ilankovic, L, Sanfelici, R, Kambeitz, J, Calhoun, V, Falkai, P, Pantelis, C, Meisenzahl, E and Koutsouleris, N (2018) Brain subtyping enhances the neuroanatomical discrimination of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 44, 10601069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericson, M, Tuvblad, C, Raine, A, Young-Wolff, K and Baker, LA (2011) Heritability and longitudinal stability of schizotypal traits during adolescence. Behaviour Genetics 41, 499511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ettinger, U, Meyhofer, I, Steffens, M, Wagner, M and Koutsouleris, N (2014) Genetics, cognition, and neurobiology of schizotypal personality: a review of the overlap with schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry 5, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ettinger, U, Mohr, C, Gooding, DC, Cohen, AS, Rapp, A, Haenschel, C and Park, S (2015) Cognition and brain function in schizotypy: a selective review. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41 (Suppl. 2), S417S426.10.1093/schbul/sbu190CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fanous, AH, Neale, MC, Gardner, CO, Webb, BT, Straub, RE, O'Neill, FA, Walsh, D, Riley, BP and Kendler, KS (2007) Significant correlation in linkage signals from genome-wide scans of schizophrenia and schizotypy. Molecular Psychiatry 12, 958965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, BR and Gao, WJ (2014) Development of thalamocortical connections between the mediodorsal thalamus and the prefrontal cortex and its implication in cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8, 1027.Google ScholarPubMed
Fonseca-Pedrero, E, Debbane, M, Ortuno-Sierra, J, Chan, RCK, Cicero, DC, Zhang, LC, Brenner, C, Barkus, E, Linscott, RJ, Kwapil, T, Barrantes-Vidal, N, Cohen, A, Raine, A, Compton, MT, Tone, EB, Suhr, J, Muniz, J, Fumero, A, Giakoumaki, S, Tsaousis, I, Preti, A, Chmielewski, M, Laloyaux, J, Mechri, A, Lahmar, MA, Wuthrich, V, Laroi, F, Badcock, JC and Jablensky, A (2018) The structure of schizotypal personality traits: a cross-national study. Psychological Medicine 48, 451462.10.1017/S0033291717001829CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fornito, A, Yucel, M, Patti, J, Wood, SJ and Pantelis, C (2009) Mapping grey matter reductions in schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Schizophrenia Research 108, 104113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, CD and Sigman, M (2007) Brain states: top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron 54, 677696.10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glahn, DC, Laird, AR, Ellison-Wright, I, Thelen, SM, Robinson, JL, Lancaster, JL, Bullmore, E and Fox, PT (2008) Meta-analysis of gray matter anomalies in schizophrenia: application of anatomic likelihood estimation and network analysis. Biological Psychiatry 64, 774781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gooding, DC, Matts, CW and Rollmann, EA (2006) Sustained attention deficits in relation to psychometrically identified schizotypy: evaluating a potential endophenotypic marker. Schizophrenia Research 82, 2737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guloksuz, S and van Os, J (2018) The slow death of the concept of schizophrenia and the painful birth of the psychosis spectrum. Psychological Medicine 48, 229244.10.1017/S0033291717001775CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, AA (1975) Four-factor index of Social status. Unpublished manuscript, New Haven, CT: Yale University.Google Scholar
Jakab, A, Blanc, R, Berenyi, EL and Szekely, G (2012) Generation of individualized thalamus target maps by using statistical shape models and thalamocortical tractography. American Journal of Neuroradiology 33, 21102116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, LC and van Os, J (2001) The continuity of psychotic experiences in the general population. Clinical Psychology Review 21, 11251141.10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00103-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, EG (2007) The Thalamus. Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press, 1708.Google Scholar
Kambeitz, J, Kambeitz-Ilankovic, L, Leucht, S, Wood, S, Davatzikos, C, Malchow, B, Falkai, P and Koutsouleris, N (2015) Detecting neuroimaging biomarkers for schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of multivariate pattern recognition studies. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 17421751.10.1038/npp.2015.22CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerns, JG and Becker, TM (2008) Communication disturbances, working memory, and emotion in people with elevated disorganized schizotypy. Schizophrenia Research 100, 172180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krauth, A, Blanc, R, Poveda, A, Jeanmonod, D, Morel, A and Szekely, G (2010) A mean three-dimensional atlas of the human thalamus: generation from multiple histological data. Neuroimage 49, 20532062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, M (2008) Caret package. Journal of Statistical Software 28, 126.Google Scholar
Kuhn, S, Schubert, F and Gallinat, J (2012) Higher prefrontal cortical thickness in high schizotypal personality trait. Journal of Psychiatric Research 46, 960965.10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.04.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenzenweger, MF (2015) Thinking clearly about schizotypy: hewing to the schizophrenia liability core, considering interesting tangents, and avoiding conceptual quicksand. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41 (Suppl. 2), S483S491.10.1093/schbul/sbu184CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynall, ME, Bassett, DS, Kerwin, R, McKenna, PJ, Kitzbichler, M, Muller, U and Bullmore, E (2010) Functional connectivity and brain networks in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 94779487.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0333-10.2010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mechelli, A, Price, CJ, Friston, KJ and Ashburner, J (2005) Voxel-based morphometry of the human brain: methods and applications. Current Medical Imaging Reviews 1, 105113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meehl, PE (1989) Schizotaxia revisited. Archives of General Psychiatry 46, 935944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieuwenhuis, M, van Haren, NE, Hulshoff Pol, HE, Cahn, W, Kahn, RS and Schnack, HG (2012) Classification of schizophrenia patients and healthy controls from structural MRI scans in two large independent samples. Neuroimage 61, 606612.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.079CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieuwenhuis, M, Schnack, HG, van Haren, NE, Lappin, J, Morgan, C, Reinders, AA, Gutierrez-Tordesillas, D, Roiz-Santianez, R, Schaufelberger, MS, Rosa, PG, Zanetti, MV, Busatto, GF, Crespo-Facorro, B, McGorry, PD, Velakoulis, D, Pantelis, C, Wood, SJ, Kahn, RS, Mourao-Miranda, J and Dazzan, P (2017) Multi-center MRI prediction models: predicting sex and illness course in first episode psychosis patients. Neuroimage 145, 246253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okada, N, Fukunaga, M, Yamashita, F, Koshiyama, D, Yamamori, H, Ohi, K, Yasuda, Y, Fujimoto, M, Watanabe, Y, Yahata, N, Nemoto, K, Hibar, DP, van Erp, TG, Fujino, H, Isobe, M, Isomura, S, Natsubori, T, Narita, H, Hashimoto, N, Miyata, J, Koike, S, Takahashi, T, Yamasue, H, Matsuo, K, Onitsuka, T, Iidaka, T, Kawasaki, Y, Yoshimura, R, Watanabe, Y, Suzuki, M, Turner, JA, Takeda, M, Thompson, PM, Ozaki, N, Kasai, K and Hashimoto, R (2016) Abnormal asymmetries in subcortical brain volume in schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry 21, 14601466.10.1038/mp.2015.209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oldfield, RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pergola, G, Selvaggi, P, Trizio, S, Bertolino, A and Blasi, G (2015) The role of the thalamus in schizophrenia from a neuroimaging perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 54, 5775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pergola, G, Trizio, S, Di Carlo, P, Taurisano, P, Mancini, M, Amoroso, N, Nettis, MA, Andriola, I, Caforio, G, Popolizio, T, Rampino, A, Di Giorgio, A, Bertolino, A and Blasi, G (2017) Grey matter volume patterns in thalamic nuclei are associated with familial risk for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 180, 1320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, SK, Dunlop, K and Downar, J (2016) Cortico-striatal-thalamic loop circuits of the salience network: a central pathway in psychiatric disease and treatment. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 10, 104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pratt, JA, Morris, B and Dawson, N (2018) Deconstructing schizophrenia: advances in preclinical models for biomarker identification. In: Pratt, J, Hall, J (eds) Biomarkers in Psychiatry. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, vol 40. Springer, Cham.Google Scholar
Pynn, LK and DeSouza, JF (2013) The function of efference copy signals: implications for symptoms of schizophrenia. Vision Research 76, 124133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A (1991) The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin 17, 555564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozycki, M, Satterthwaite, TD, Koutsouleris, N, Erus, G, Doshi, J, Wolf, DH, Fan, Y, Gur, RE, Gur, RC, Meisenzahl, EM, Zhuo, C, Ying, H, Yan, H, Yue, W, Zhang, D and Davatzikos, C (2017) Multisite machine learning analysis provides a robust structural imaging signature of schizophrenia detectable across diverse patient populations and within individuals. Schizophrenia Bulletin 44, 10351044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salomon, R, Bleich-Cohen, M, Hahamy-Dubossarsky, A, Dinstien, I, Weizman, R, Poyurovsky, M, Kupchik, M, Kotler, M, Hendler, T and Malach, R (2011) Global functional connectivity deficits in schizophrenia depend on behavioral state. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 1297212981.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvador, R, Radua, J, Canales-Rodriguez, EJ, Solanes, A, Sarro, S, Goikolea, JM, Valiente, A, Monte, GC, Natividad, MDC, Guerrero-Pedraza, A, Moro, N, Fernandez-Corcuera, P, Amann, BL, Maristany, T, Vieta, E, McKenna, PJ and Pomarol-Clotet, E (2017) Evaluation of machine learning algorithms and structural features for optimal MRI-based diagnostic prediction in psychosis. PLoS ONE 12, e0175683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, LI, Wimmer, RD, Nakajima, M, Happ, M, Mofakham, S and Halassa, MM (2017) Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains attentional control. Nature 545, 219223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarz, E, Doan, NT, Pergola, G, Westlye, LT, Kaufmann, T, Wolfers, T, Brecheisen, R, Quarto, T, Ing, AJ, Di Carlo, P, Gurholt, TP, Harms, RL, Noirhomme, Q, Moberget, T, Agartz, I, Andreassen, OA, Bellani, M, Bertolino, A, Blasi, G, Brambilla, P, Buitelaar, JK, Cervenka, S, Flyckt, L, Frangou, S, Franke, B, Hall, J, Heslenfeld, DJ, Kirsch, P, McIntosh, AM, Nothen, MM, Papassotiropoulos, A, de Quervain, DJ, Rietschel, M, Schumann, G, Tost, H, Witt, SH, Zink, M and Meyer-Lindenberg, A and Imagemend Consortium (2019) Reproducible grey matter patterns index a multivariate, global alteration of brain structure in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Translational Psychiatry 9, 12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherman, SM (2016) Thalamus plays a central role in ongoing cortical functioning. Nature Neuroscience 19, 533541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takahashi, T, Suzuki, M, Zhou, SY, Nakamura, K, Tanino, R, Kawasaki, Y, Seal, ML, Seto, H and Kurachi, M (2008) Prevalence and length of the adhesio interthalamica in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Psychiatry Research 164, 9094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taurisano, P, Romano, R, Mancini, M, Di Giorgio, A, Antonucci, LA, Fazio, L, Rampino, A, Quarto, T, Gelao, B, Porcelli, A, Papazacharias, A, Ursini, G, Caforio, G, Masellis, R, Niccoli-Asabella, A, Todarello, O, Popolizio, T, Rubini, G, Blasi, G and Bertolino, A (2014) Prefronto-striatal physiology is associated with schizotypy and is modulated by a functional variant of DRD2. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8, 235.10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00235CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tien, AY (1991) Distributions of hallucinations in the population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 26, 287292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tzourio-Mazoyer, N, Landeau, B, Papathanassiou, D, Crivello, F, Etard, O, Delcroix, N, Mazoyer, B and Joliot, M (2002) Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Heuvel, MP, Mandl, RC, Stam, CJ, Kahn, RS and Hulshoff Pol, HE (2010) Aberrant frontal and temporal complex network structure in schizophrenia: a graph theoretical analysis. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 1591515926.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2874-10.2010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Erp, TG, Hibar, DP, Rasmussen, JM, Glahn, DC, Pearlson, GD, Andreassen, OA, Agartz, I, Westlye, LT, Haukvik, UK, Dale, AM, Melle, I, Hartberg, CB, Gruber, O, Kraemer, B, Zilles, D, Donohoe, G, Kelly, S, McDonald, C, Morris, DW, Cannon, DM, Corvin, A, Machielsen, MW, Koenders, L, de Haan, L, Veltman, DJ, Satterthwaite, TD, Wolf, DH, Gur, RC, Gur, RE, Potkin, SG, Mathalon, DH, Mueller, BA, Preda, A, Macciardi, F, Ehrlich, S, Walton, E, Hass, J, Calhoun, VD, Bockholt, HJ, Sponheim, SR, Shoemaker, JM, van Haren, NE, Hulshoff Pol, HE, Ophoff, RA, Kahn, RS, Roiz-Santianez, R, Crespo-Facorro, B, Wang, L, Alpert, KI, Jonsson, EG, Dimitrova, R, Bois, C, Whalley, HC, McIntosh, AM, Lawrie, SM, Hashimoto, R, Thompson, PM and Turner, JA (2016) Subcortical brain volume abnormalities in 2028 individuals with schizophrenia and 2540 healthy controls via the ENIGMA consortium. Molecular Psychiatry 21, 547553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Os, J, Linscott, RJ, Myin-Germeys, I, Delespaul, P and Krabbendam, L (2009) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychological Medicine 39, 179195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vollema, MG and Hoijtink, H (2000) The multidimensionality of self-report schizotypy in a psychiatric population: an analysis using multidimensional Rasch models. Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 565575.10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollema, MG and Postma, B (2002) Neurocognitive correlates of schizotypy in first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin 28, 367377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vollema, MG, Sitskoorn, MM, Appels, MC and Kahn, RS (2002) Does the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire reflect the biological-genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Research 54, 3945.10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00350-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vukadinovic, Z (2014) NMDA receptor hypofunction and the thalamus in schizophrenia. Physiology and Behavior 131, 156159.10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.04.038CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walter, EE, Fernandez, F, Snelling, M and Barkus, E (2016) Genetic consideration of schizotypal traits: a review. Frontiers in Psychology 7, 1769.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, TS, Leong, K, Sanford, N, Tipper, CM and Lavigne, KM (2016) Altered balance of functional brain networks in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 248, 94104.10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zarogianni, E, Moorhead, TW and Lawrie, SM (2013) Towards the identification of imaging biomarkers in schizophrenia, using multivariate pattern classification at a single-subject level. Neuroimage: Clinical 3, 279289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zarogianni, E, Storkey, AJ, Johnstone, EC, Owens, DG and Lawrie, SM (2017) Improved individualized prediction of schizophrenia in subjects at familial high risk, based on neuroanatomical data, schizotypal and neurocognitive features. Schizophrenia Research 181, 612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographics of the sample used in this study

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Density plots of Random Forests classification accuracies. Density curves compare classification accuracy (x-axis) distribution over 1000 re-sampling between (a) the Thalamic Subdivisions features (ThSub) or (b) the Automated Anatomic Labeling whole-brain features (ALL) with the respective null distribution obtained through permutation of diagnostic labels.

Figure 2

Table 2. Machine learning classification performances

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Relevance of positive schizotypy for healthy controls classification. (a) Boxplot showing that false positives (FP, healthy controls misclassified to SCZ diagnosis) have higher positive schizotypy scores than true negatives (TN, healthy controls correctly classified). (b) Scatterplot showing the relationship between the likelihood to be classified as SCZ (π-SCZ) based on multivariate volumetric patterns of thalamic nuclei and positive schizotypy scores. Shaded area indicates 95% confidence interval.

Figure 4

Table 3. Association of classifiers outcome with SPQ scores and demographics in healthy controls

Supplementary material: File

Di Carlo et al. supplementary material

Di Carlo et al. supplementary material 1

Download Di Carlo et al. supplementary material(File)
File 256.5 KB