Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-g4j75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T01:46:53.910Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Putting social cognitive mechanisms back into cumulative technological culture: Social interactions serve as a mechanism for children's early knowledge acquisition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2020

Amanda S. Haber
Affiliation:
Boston University, Wheelock College of Education and Human Development, Boston, MA02215. haber317@bu.edukcorriv@bu.edu
Kathleen H. Corriveau
Affiliation:
Boston University, Wheelock College of Education and Human Development, Boston, MA02215. haber317@bu.edukcorriv@bu.edu

Abstract

Osiurak and Reynaud offer a unified cognitive approach to cumulative technological culture, arguing that it begins with non-social cognitive skills that allow humans to learn and develop new technical information. Drawing on research focusing on how children acquire knowledge through interactions others, we argue that social learning is essential for humans to acquire technical information.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Drawing from research and theory from variety of disciplines, Osiurak and Reynaud (O&R) offer a unified cognitive approach to cumulative technological culture, arguing that it begins with non-social cognitive skills that allow humans to learn and develop new technical information. We agree with the authors that technical reasoning can be considered a critical part of cumulative technological culture. However, although it is important to consider non-social cognitive skills, here, we argue that social learning is essential for humans to acquire technical information. Thus, an examination of developmental psychological research focusing on how children acquire knowledge through interactions with others is critical to understanding why social mechanisms are a central part of children's technical reasoning skills.

Children are curious learners who actively seek information from others, often caregivers. At an early age, infants have the capacity to engage in social referencing, using eye gaze (e.g., Brooks & Meltzoff Reference Brooks and Meltzoff2015), affective facial expressions and pointing to get an adult's attention (e.g., Butterworth Reference Butterworth2003) and decide what to do in a situation. As children's linguistic abilities develop, children use questions and explanations from caregivers to acquire knowledge (e.g., Butler et al. Reference Butler, Ronfard and Corriveau2020; Chouinard Reference Chouinard2007; Frazier et al. Reference Frazier, Gelman and Wellman2009; Harris Reference Harris2012; Harris et al. Reference Harris, Koenig, Corriveau and Jaswal2018; Kurkul & Corriveau Reference Kurkul and Corriveau2018; Ronfard et al. Reference Ronfard, Zambrana, Hermansen and Kelemen2018). More specifically, children use testimony from adults to learn about a variety of topics including scientific (e.g., Callanan & Oakes Reference Callanan and Oakes1992; Tabors et al. Reference Tabors, Roach, Snow, Dickinson and Tabors2001; Willard et al. Reference Willard, Busch, Cullum, Letourneau, Sobel, Callanan and Legare2019) and mathematical information (e.g., Hanner et al. Reference Hanner, Braham, Elliott and Libertus2019; Levine et al. Reference Levine, Suriyakham, Rowe, Huttenlocher and Gunderson2010; Ramani et al. Reference Ramani, Rowe, Eason and Leech2015). This is especially true when children are learning about scientific phenomena that are challenging to understand through first-hand observation alone such as the shape of the earth or the existence of germs. Similarly, acquiring an understanding of scientific processes such as electricity often require more than first hand-observation because the mechanisms are sometimes opaque or associated with a time delay (Corriveau et al. Reference Corriveau, Min, Chin and Doan2016; Harris & Corriveau Reference Harris, Corriveau, Einav and Robinson2014; Harris & Koenig Reference Harris and Koenig2006).

Moreover, children often rely on information from others to enhance their first-hand understanding of mechanisms underlying a causal process (Callanan & Jipson Reference Callanan, Jipson, Crowley, Schunn and Okada2001; Crowley & Siegler Reference Crowley and Siegler1999; Crowley et al. Reference Crowley, Callanan, Jipson, Galco, Topping and Shrager2001a; Reference Crowley, Callanan, Tenenbaum and Allen2001b; Haden Reference Haden2010). To date, a substantial number of studies have found that through interactions with children, adult explanations serve as a source of information that significantly contribute to children's scientific understanding and learning (e.g., Jipson et al. Reference Jipson, Gülgöz and Gelman2016; Lombrozo et al. Reference Lombrozo, Bonawitz and Scalise2018; Vasilyeva & Lombrozo Reference Vasilyeva, Lombrozo, Rogers, Rau, Zhu and Kalish2018; Willard et al. Reference Willard, Busch, Cullum, Letourneau, Sobel, Callanan and Legare2019). Through parent–child conversations, children learn about causal mechanisms, demonstrate how to generate as well as test hypotheses and share important mechanistic information, all of which enhances 4- and 5-year-old children's ability to transfer knowledge to subsequent scientific activities (e.g., Kurkul et al. under review; Leech et al., Reference Leech, Haber, Jalkh and Corriveauin press). In their target article, O&R propose that cumulative technological culture emerges because of humans' non-social cognitive skills. Although technical reasoning, especially the ability to generalize what is learned from one situation to another one, is a fundamental part of the humans' capacity to develop new techniques or knowledge, we argue that these skills develop through interactions with others, suggesting that social mechanisms are integral to cumulative technological culture.

In sum, the unified cognitive approach to cumulative technological culture highlights the importance of technical reasoning skills in humans' ability to acquire and develop new content.

However, only focusing on the cognitive structures under this framework does not account for the fundamental role of social learning in the emergence of children's technical reasoning skills. We argue for a more integrated approach to cumulative technical culture, grounded in sociocultural theory (e.g., Vygotsky Reference Vygotsky1978). Such an approach should highlight the important cognitive skills children bring to bear to acquire technical information, yet also recognize that most learning situations occur in interactions with others. Thus, children's knowledge is strengthened through conversations with and observation of others.

Financial support

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation (grant number 1652224).

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Brooks, R. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2015) Connecting the dots from infancy to childhood: A longitudinal study connecting gaze following, language, and explicit theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 130:6778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, L. P., Ronfard, S. & Corriveau, K. H., eds. (2020) The questioning child: Insights from psychology and education. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G. (2003) Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In: Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet, ed. S. Kita, pp. 933. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Callanan, M. A. & Oakes, L. M. (1992) Preschoolers questions and parents’ explanations: Causal thinking in everyday activity. Cognitive Development 7(2):213–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callanan, M. A. & Jipson, J. L. (2001). Explanatory conversations and young children's developing scientific literacy. In: Designing for science: Implications from everyday, classroom, and professional settings, eds. Crowley, K., Schunn, C. D. & Okada, T., pp. 2149, Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Chouinard, M. M. (2007) Children's questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 72(1):1112.Google ScholarPubMed
Corriveau, K. H., Min, G., Chin, J. & Doan, S. (2016) Do as I do, not as I say: Children's learning of self-regulatory strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 143:179–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowley, K., Callanan, M., Jipson, J. L., Galco, J., Topping, K. & Shrager, J. (2001a) Shared scientific thinking in everyday parent–child activity. Science Education 85(6):712–32. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.1035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R. & Allen, E. (2001b) Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared scientific thinking. Psychological Science 12(3):258–61. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowley, K. & Siegler, R. S. (1999) Explanation and generalization in young children's strategy learning. Child Development 70(2):304–16. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frazier, B. N., Gelman, S. A. & Wellman, H. M. (2009) Preschoolers’ search for explanatory information within adult–child conversation. Child Development 80(6):1592–611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haden, C. A. (2010) Talking about science in museums. Child Development 4(1):6267.Google Scholar
Hanner, E., Braham, E. J., Elliott, L. & Libertus, M. E. (2019) Promoting math talk in adult–child interactions through grocery store signs. Mind, Brain, and Education 13(2):110–18. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2012) Trusting what you're told: How children learn from others. The Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L. & Corriveau, K. H. (2014) Learning from testimony about religion and science. In: Trust and skepticism: Children's selective learning from testimony, eds. Einav, S. & Robinson, E., pp. 2841. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. & Koenig, M. (2006) Trust in testimony: How children learn about science and religion. Child Development 77(3):505–24. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00886.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L., Koenig, M. A., Corriveau, K. H. & Jaswal, V. K. (2018) Cognitive foundations of learning from testimony. Annual Review of Psychology 69:251–73. Available at doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jipson, J. L., Gülgöz, S. & Gelman, S. A. (2016) Parent–child conversations regarding the ontological status of a robotic dog. Cognitive Development 39:2135. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurkul, K., Castine, E., Leech, K. & Corriveau, K. H.(under revised review) ‘How does a switch work?’ The role of mechanistic language in children's learning from parents. Submitted to Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
Kurkul, K. E. & Corriveau, K. H. (2018) Question, explanation, follow-up: A mechanism for learning from others? Child Development 89(1):280–94. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leech, K. A., Haber, A. S., Jalkh, Y. & Corriveau, K. H. (in press) Embedding explanations into storybooks impacts children's scientific discourse and learning. Frontiers in Psychology 11:1016. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, S. C., Suriyakham, L. W., Rowe, M. L., Huttenlocher, J. & Gunderson, E. A. (2010) What counts in the development of young children's number knowledge? Developmental Psychology 46(5):1309–19. Available at: doi: 10.1037/a0019671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lombrozo, T., Bonawitz, E. B. & Scalise, N. R. (2018) Young children's learning and generalization of teleological and mechanistic explanations. Journal of Cognition and Development 19(2):220–32. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2018.1427099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramani, G. B., Rowe, M. L., Eason, S. H. & Leech, K. A. (2015) Math talk during informal learning activities in head start families. Cognitive Development 35:1533. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.11.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ronfard, S., Zambrana, I. M., Hermansen, T. K. & Kelemen, D. (2018) Question-asking in childhood: A review of the literature and a framework for understanding its development. Developmental Review 49:101–20. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.05.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabors, P. O., Roach, K. A. & Snow, C. E. (2001) Home language and literacy environment: Final results. In: Beginning literacy with language: Young children learning at home and school, eds. Dickinson, D. K. & Tabors, P. O., pp. 111–38. Paul H Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Vasilyeva, N. & Lombrozo, T. (2018) When and how children use explanations to guide generalizations. In: Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, eds. Rogers, T. T., Rau, M., Zhu, X. & Kalish, C. W., pp. 2069–614. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978) Mind in society: Development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Willard, A. K., Busch, J. T. A., Cullum, K. A., Letourneau, S. M., Sobel, D. M., Callanan, M. & Legare, C. H. (2019) Explain this, explore that: A study of parent–child interaction in a children's museum. Child Development 90(5):e598e617. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar