Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-hvd4g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T17:50:51.989Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mind God's mind: History, development, and teaching1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 March 2016

Andreas Demetriou
Affiliation:
Department of Social Sciences, University of Nicosia, Egkomi 1700, Cyprus. ademetriou@ucy.ac.cyhttp://www.unic.ac.cy/
Nikos Makris
Affiliation:
Department of Primary Education, University of Thrace, Xanthi 671 00, Greece. nmakris@eled.duth.gr
Dimitris Pnevmatikos
Affiliation:
Department of Primary Education, University of Western Macedonia, Kozani 501 00, Greece. dpnevma@gmail.com

Abstract

We dispute the target article that belief in Big Gods facilitated development of large societies and suggest that the direction of causality might be inverted. We also suggest that plain theory of mind (ToM), although necessary, is not sufficient to conceive Big Gods. Grasp of other aspects of the mind is required. However, this theory is useful for the teaching of religion.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

We focus on two of the claims of the target article and advance an argument about its possible educational implications.

What is first: Big Gods or big societies?

The main position of the target article that Big Gods and related prosocial religions “promoted large-scale cooperation and high fertility, often leading to success in intergroup competition” (abstract) is not well founded in history. History suggests that the direction of causality might be inverted. That is, reaching a critical mass in a population, a certain level of intragroup cohesion, and role diversification serving group efficiency were necessary for pondering of rules (moral or other) governing the functioning and preservation of the group vis-à-vis the challenges of the natural environment or other competing groups. Under this conception, Big Gods and related prosocial religions emerged from, rather than resulted in, big societies.

The history of ancient Greek religion aligns with our position rather than the position of the target article: the belief in Big Gods appeared long after people began to live in big cities. In fact, several crucial supernatural characteristics attributed to Big Gods (immortality, omnipresence, ability to oversee humans, and their role in the preservation of moral and social order) resulted from other concerns rather than the preservation of the group as such. For example, the omnipresence of Big Gods was first attributed to anthropomorphic gods and seems to have come from the need to continue paying homage and respect to ancestors abandoned in remote lands. Their moral role seems to have come from a shift from aristocratic to democratic governance with all ensuing consequences for personal responsibilities, duties, and rights. There is also some evidence for this sequence in the development of Christian (Pnevmatikos Reference Pnevmatikos2002) and Muslim children (Pnevmatikos & Makris Reference Pnevmatikos, Makris and Håkansson2010). Admittedly, though, once invented and widely accepted, Big Gods might then have exerted the influences suggested by the target article.

Can every mind invent a Big God?

The fundamental assumption of the target article in concern to the grasp of Big Gods is limited. We concur with it that mentalizing is crucial for belief in entities whose properties are mental constructions: Minimally, a creature must have an insight that the world is representable and that there may be variations between representations of his co-creatures to extrapolate to the Big Holy Creature knowing and controlling everything. However, plain theory of mind (ToM) would make a very simple Big God, most probably an anthropomorphic god, if any. Mentalizing is much broader than ToM and develops much beyond it. It involves, in addition to the recognition of the representational nature of the human mind, recognition of the multiplicity of the origins of representations – perception, inference, learning from others, logic itself, and the recognition of its amenability to control by oneself and others (Demetriou et al. Reference Demetriou, Spanoudis and Shayer2014). Research suggests that grasping the representational nature of the human mind is a long process (Spanoudis et al. Reference Spanoudis, Demetriou, Kazi, Giorgala and Zenonos2015). At 4–6 years, children understand that minds represent the world, but it is only later that they discriminate the human from the god's mind by attributing different properties to each one of them (Makris & Pnevmatikos Reference Makris and Pnevmatikos2007; submitted). This differentiation is facilitated by mastering executive control that enables children to discriminate between similar minds (humans) from supernatural minds (Makris & Pnevmatikos, submitted). That, for example, gods know for good and ever, beyond human fallible thinking, create and recreate in the name of justice, judge but negotiate their decisions vis-à-vis motives and intentions, and distribute favors and punishments. This is the mind of the adolescent rather than the mind of the toddler. To extrapolate, we might even assume that gods’ minds are reshaped by humanity to reflect the possibilities of the human mind. This, however, occurs with huge time lags, in the fashion of Gödel's incompleteness theorems: human possibilities are projected to God's a few thousand years after they are attained.

Education for religion in post-modernity

Despite our previous objections, we accept that this theory is an excellent framework for rethinking the teaching of religion. Specifically, this framework may be used to develop curricula that would enable students to understand how different aspects of social, individual, and historical change resulted into important cultural institutions, such as religion. This curriculum would have to highlight how interaction between social needs (e.g., arising from the enlargement of societies), ruling and operational needs (e.g., autocratic vs. aristocratic vs. democratic governance), personal needs (e.g., existential needs), and abilities (e.g. mentalizing, reasoning, and knowledge) resulted in different religions, and how change in these needs resulted into changes in religions and religious thinking. This curriculum seems important now more than ever because vanishing time, geographical, and national boundaries and the emergence of supranational entities, such as the European Union, requires that citizens have a better understanding of the forces binding individuals and groups together. This framework may be used to compare religion with other forms of political organizations, such as justice and lawmaking and enforcement at the national and the supranational level.

Our model suggests that at different ages children can grasp different aspects of these interactions (Demetriou et al. Reference Demetriou, Spanoudis and Mouyi2011). At the preschool level, teaching about gods may help children conceive of practices that may facilitate them to cope with their obligations to others. This would facilitate executive control, which is important to master at this age. At the primary school level, children may learn that religion is one social institution among many that relate to the functioning of the society, as it regulates individual behavior, rights, and duties. Personal responsibility to ideals for humanity, life, and civilization beyond time must come later in adolescence. At this phase, God may be understood as the Big Mind, a huge historical projection of the struggle of humanity over time to understand and cope with its raison d’être and its destiny! At any phase, children must also differentiate actual causal forces governing societies and nature from mental constructions about them, however “holy” they may be.

Footnotes

1.

All authors contributed equally to the preparation of this commentary.

References

Demetriou, A., Spanoudis, G. & Mouyi, A. (2011) Educating the developing mind: Towards an overarching paradigm. Educational Psychology Review 23:601–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demetriou, A., Spanoudis, G. & Shayer, M. (2014) Inference, reconceptualization, insight, and efficiency along intellectual growth: A general theory. Enfance 66:365–96. doi: /10.4074/S0013754514003097.Google Scholar
Makris, N. & Pnevmatikos, D. (2007) Children's understanding of the human and super-natural mind. Cognitive Development 22(3):365–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Makris, N. & Pnevmatikos, D. (submitted) Inhibitory control in children's understanding of other minds.Google Scholar
Pnevmatikos, D. (2002) Conceptual changes in religious concepts of elementary schoolchildren: The case of the house where God lives. Educational Psychology 22:91110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pnevmatikos, D. & Makris, N. (2010) Cross-cultural evidence for constraints in conceptual development, or, are the places where Allah and God live the same? In: Developmental Psychology, ed. Håkansson, J., pp. 117–33. Nova Science.Google Scholar
Spanoudis, G., Demetriou, A., Kazi, S., Giorgala, K. & Zenonos, V. (2015) Embedding cognizance in intellectual development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 132:3250.Google Scholar