Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-s22k5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T13:08:27.379Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Improving the generalizability of infant psychological research: The ManyBabies model

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 February 2022

Ingmar Visser
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, 1018 WB, The Netherlandsi.visser@uva.nl; http://www.ingmar.org
Christina Bergmann
Affiliation:
Language and Development Department, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 6525 XDNijmegen, The Netherlandschristina.bergmann@mpi.nl; https://www.mpi.nl
Krista Byers-Heinlein
Affiliation:
Concordia Infant Research Laboratory, Concordia University, MontrealQCH4B 1R6, Canadak.byers@concordia.ca; dalbenwork@gmail.com; https://infantresearch.ca
Rodrigo Dal Ben
Affiliation:
Concordia Infant Research Laboratory, Concordia University, MontrealQCH4B 1R6, Canadak.byers@concordia.ca; dalbenwork@gmail.com; https://infantresearch.ca
Wlodzislaw Duch
Affiliation:
Nicolaus Copernicus University, 87-100Torun, Polandwduch@umk.pl; https://www.umk.pl/en
Samuel Forbes
Affiliation:
University of East Anglia, NorwichNR4 7TJ, UKsamuel.forbes@uea.ac.uk; https://people.uea.ac.uk/samuel_forbes
Laura Franchin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, 38068Rovereto, Italylaura.franchin@unitn.it; alessandra.geraci@unitn.it; https://webapps.unitn.it/du/en/Persona/PER0169770; https://webapps.unitn.it/du/en/Persona/PER0033078
Michael C. Frank
Affiliation:
Stanford University, Stanford, CA94301USAmcfrank@stanford.edu; http://langcog.stanford.edu
Alessandra Geraci
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Cognitive Science, University of Trento, 38068Rovereto, Italylaura.franchin@unitn.it; alessandra.geraci@unitn.it; https://webapps.unitn.it/du/en/Persona/PER0169770; https://webapps.unitn.it/du/en/Persona/PER0033078
J. Kiley Hamlin
Affiliation:
UBC Center for Infant Cognition, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BCV6T 1Z4, Canadakiley.hamlin@psych.ubc.ca; https://cic.psych.ubc.ca/
Zsuzsa Kaldy
Affiliation:
UMass Boston, Baby Lab, Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts Boston, Boston, MA02125-3393, USAzsuzsa.kaldy@umb.edu; http://babies.umb.edu
Louisa Kulke
Affiliation:
Neurocognitive Developmental Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg, 91052Erlangen, Germanylouisa.kulke@fau.de; https://neurodevpsychology.phil.fau.de/
Catherine Laverty
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, B15 2TTBirmingham, UKCML704@student.bham.ac.uk; https://carolinerichards.net/people/
Casey Lew-Williams
Affiliation:
Princeton Baby Lab, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ08540, USAcaseylw@princeton.edu; martincz@princeton.edu; https://babylab.princeton.edu/
Victoria Mateu
Affiliation:
UCLA Department of Spanish and Portuguese, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA90095-1532, USAvmateu@humnet.ucla.edu; https://www.victoriamateu.com/
Julien Mayor
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, 0373Oslo, Norwayjulien.mayor@psykologi.uio.no; https://www.sv.uio.no/psi/english/people/aca/julienma/
David Moreau
Affiliation:
Brain Dynamics Lab, University of Auckland, Auckland1010, New Zealandd.moreau@auckland.ac.nz; https://www.braindynamicslab.com
Iris Nomikou
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UKiris.nomikou@port.ac.uk; https://www.port.ac.uk/about-us/structure-and-governance/our-people/our-staff/iris-nomikou
Tobias Schuwerk
Affiliation:
Department of Pscyhology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, 80802Munich, Germanytobias.schuwerk@psy.lmu.de; https://www.psy.lmu.de/epp/personen/wiss_ma/tobias_schuwerk/
Elizabeth A. Simpson
Affiliation:
Social Cognition Laboratory, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL33124, USAsimpsone@miami.edu; https://people.miami.edu/profile/simpsone@miami.edu
Leher Singh
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore119077psyls@nus.edu.sg; http://blog.nus.edu.sg/lehersingh/
Melanie Soderstrom
Affiliation:
Baby Language Lab, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MBR3T 2N2, Canadamelsod@babylanguagelab.org; https://babylanguagelab.org/
Jessica Sullivan
Affiliation:
Developing Minds Center, Skidmore College, Saratoga Springs, NY12866, USAjsulliv1@skidmore.edu; https://www.skidmore.edu/developing_minds_center/index.php
Marion I. van den Heuvel
Affiliation:
Department of Cognitive Neuropsychology, Tilburg University, 5037 ABTilburg, The Netherlandsm.i.vdnheuvel@tilburguniversity.edu; http://marionvandenheuvel.com
Gert Westermann
Affiliation:
Department: Psychology, Lancaster University, LancasterLA1 4YW, UKg.westermann@lancaster.ac.uk; https://www.lancaster.ac.uk/people-profiles/gert-westermann
Yuki Yamada
Affiliation:
Kyushu University, Fukuoka, Japanyamadayuk@gmail.com; http://sites.google.com/site/yamadayuk/
Lorijn Zaadnoordijk
Affiliation:
Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Irelandl.zaadnoordijk@tcd.ie; https://sites.google.com/view/lorijnzaadnoordijk/homepage
Martin Zettersten
Affiliation:
Princeton Baby Lab, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ08540, USAcaseylw@princeton.edu; martincz@princeton.edu; https://babylab.princeton.edu/

Abstract

Yarkoni's analysis clearly articulates a number of concerns limiting the generalizability and explanatory power of psychological findings, many of which are compounded in infancy research. ManyBabies addresses these concerns via a radically collaborative, large-scale and open approach to research that is grounded in theory-building, committed to diversification, and focused on understanding sources of variation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Yarkoni raises concerns about widespread practices in the psychological sciences – ranging from standard statistical practices to narrow experimental designs – which hinder generalizability, theory-building, and ultimately, explanatory power. Infant research in particular faces a range of problems, including difficulties recruiting participants (often resulting in small samples), the unique challenges of designing experiments that hold infants' attention, limited numbers of observations per participant, and infants' rapid developmental changes (Bergmann et al., Reference Bergmann, Tsuji, Piccinini, Lewis, Braginsky, Frank and Cristia2018; Frank et al., Reference Frank, Bergelson, Bergmann, Cristia, Floccia, Gervain and Yurovsky2017; Oakes, Reference Oakes2017).

ManyBabies is a large-scale, multilab collaborative project that currently spans 47 countries and over 200 institutions (https://manybabies.github.io). The project provides a constructive, best-practice, grass-roots approach for addressing issues of replicability and generalizability in infant research and employs a model also utilized by other large-scale, multisite collaborations (e.g., ManyPrimates, 2019; Moshontz et al., Reference Moshontz, Campbell, Ebersole, IJzerman, Urry, Forscher and Chartier2018). Thus far, ManyBabies has focused its efforts on replicating fundamental findings in infant cognition that underpin our understanding of early cognitive development.

Features and benefits of the ManyBabies approach in addressing the issues Yarkoni identified are (see also Byers-Heinlein et al., Reference Byers-Heinlein, Bergmann, Davies, Frank, Hamlin, Kline and Soderstrom2020; Frank et al., Reference Frank, Bergelson, Bergmann, Cristia, Floccia, Gervain and Yurovsky2017; The ManyBabies Consortium, 2020):

  1. (1) Consensus-based study designs to advance theory. ManyBabies projects are focused on evaluating central theories in infant research (e.g., under which circumstances infants show preferences for familiar or novel stimuli in ManyBabies5; Hunter & Ames, Reference Hunter, Ames, Rovee-Collier and Lipsitt1988), and carefully probing the bounds of theoretical constructs by encouraging participation from researchers with diverse perspectives. ManyBabies' collaborative and consensus-building approach disrupts existing hierarchies, making space for dissent and innovation, and for adjudicating between opposing views (e.g., in the case of adversarial collaboration in ManyBabies2 addressing Theory of Mind; c.f. Baillargeon, Buttelmann, & Southgate, Reference Baillargeon, Buttelmann and Southgate2018; Cowan et al., Reference Cowan, Belletier, Doherty, Jaroslawska, Rhodes, Forsberg and Logie2020; Surian & Geraci, Reference Surian and Geraci2012). Simultaneously, it expands collaborative networks to bridge a wide variety of theoretical backgrounds, resulting in designs that clearly identify testable points of disagreement to lay the foundation for further inquiry through experiment and debate.

  2. (2) Conceptual replications. As noted by Yarkoni, direct replication is not a sensible target for improving reproducibility if there are concerns about weaknesses in paradigms or stimulus sets that could be addressed in a new experiment (e.g., ManyBabies4 will remove confounds in a paradigm developed to probe infants’ social evaluations; Hamlin, Wynn, & Bloom, Reference Hamlin, Wynn and Bloom2007; Scarf, Imuta, Colombo, & Hayne, Reference Scarf, Imuta, Colombo and Hayne2012). ManyBabies projects probe the generality of phenomena by prioritizing conceptual over exact replications, bringing together researchers from different theoretical and methodological backgrounds to build experimental designs that best capture the processes being studied.

  3. (3) Diversity in samples and scientists. By encouraging participation from labs from all over the world and supporting laboratory expenses for scientists who are new to experimental infant research, ManyBabies promotes diversity across multiple dimensions: contexts, lab practices, researchers, and participants. ManyBabies takes seriously the importance and impact of participant heterogeneity (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010), and creates datasets that are more representative of the population of interest (i.e., “human infants”) compared to single-lab studies, by testing participants with diverse linguistic and sociocultural backgrounds. Exploring the impact of diversity on the generalizability of core findings has become a prominent target in recent projects, e.g., studying infants at home rather than in a highly-controlled lab setting in ManyBabies-AtHome, thereby reaching more rural populations; assessing the replicability of initial findings with African infants in ManyBabies1A; in ManyBabies3 – studying rule-learning – making the stimuli suitable for infants from different linguistic backgrounds. In doing so, ManyBabies enables us to strike a better balance between the precision of estimation/breadth of generalization trade-off cited by Yarkoni.

  4. (4) Quantifying sources of variation. Studies following the ManyBabies approach can reveal and explicitly measure sources of variation that are difficult to estimate in single-lab studies, including effects of lab practices and methodological variation. For example, ManyBabies1 (addressing infants' preferences for infant-directed speech) tested for effects of distinct experimental methods in infant research (e.g., head-turn preference, central fixation, eye-tracking, ManyBabies Consortium, 2020); ManyBabies2 compares online and in-lab data collection. Both projects thereby probe the generalizability of observed phenomena across experimental paradigms. Specifically, variety is built in through diversity of experimental paradigms used to test a research question – a typical benefit of meta-analysis – yet at the same time we retain control over a number of design factors, as in replication efforts. Given the wide-ranging sources of methodological variation, however, there is considerable work remaining to be done on this issue.

  5. (5) Stimulus generalizability. Issues related to stimulus informativeness and generalizability (or lack thereof) are discussed by the ManyBabies project teams and wider community throughout the design process, which generates new “best test” stimuli. The focus is on conceptual replications that involve stimulus sets that differ from the original studies, in this way directly addressing the question of stimulus generalizability. The next step here is to systematically vary stimulus sets.

  6. (6) Transparent research practices. ManyBabies is committed to transparency at each research stage, and to collective governance that encourages genuine and non-hierarchical debate, defies the research status-quo, and leads to innovation in theoretical, methodological, and analytic design, as Yarkoni suggests. For example, ManyBabies maintains detailed documentation protocols and openly shares all stimuli and data, including many additional descriptive variables. In this way, additional sources of variance and alternative hypotheses can be tested.

Ensuring that verbal and quantitative expressions of our hypotheses are closely aligned is a tall task. The diversity of scientists involved in each ManyBabies project goes a long way toward developing meaningful operationalizations of the specific research questions under examination. At the same time, the diversity of samples, methods, and stimuli addresses (to an extent) many of the questions on generalizability raised by Yarkoni. Even so, much work remains to tackle concerns related to methodological/stimulus variation, generalizability, and participant heterogeneity, to develop best practices in large-scale international collaborations, and to build better theories (Borsboom, van der Maas, Dalege, Kievit, & Haig, Reference Borsboom, van der Maas, Dalege, Kievit and Haig2021). Nevertheless, we look forward to continuing to provide opportunities for learning and growth in the ManyBabies communities, creating the necessary scaffolding for even better research, and, alongside other large collaborative networks, being at the forefront of creating a psychological science that is generalizable and reproducible.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Baillargeon, R., Buttelmann, D., & Southgate, V. (2018). Invited commentary: Interpreting failed replications of early false-belief findings: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Cognitive Development, 46, 112124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.06.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergmann, C., Tsuji, S., Piccinini, P. E., Lewis, M. L., Braginsky, M. B., Frank, M. C., & Cristia, A. (2018). Promoting replicability in developmental research through meta-analyses: Insights from language acquisition research. Child Development, 89(6), 19962009. http://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13079.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borsboom, D., van der Maas, H. L., Dalege, J., Kievit, R. A., & Haig, B. D. (2021). Theory construction methodology: A practical framework for building theories in psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 756766. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620969647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Bergmann, C., Davies, C., Frank, M. C., Hamlin, J. K., Kline, M., … Soderstrom, M. (2020). Building a collaborative psychological science: Lessons learned from ManyBabies 1. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 61(4), 349. https://doi.org/10.1037/cap0000216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N., Belletier, C., Doherty, J. M., Jaroslawska, A. J., Rhodes, S., Forsberg, A., … Logie, R. H. (2020). How do scientific views change? Notes from an extended adversarial collaboration. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 10111025. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620906415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, M. C., Bergelson, E., Bergmann, C., Cristia, A., Floccia, C., Gervain, J., … Yurovsky, D. (2017). A collaborative approach to infant research: Promoting reproducibility, best practices, and theory-building. Infancy, 22(4), 421435. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450, 557559. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, M. A., & Ames, E. W. (1988). A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. In Rovee-Collier, C. & Lipsitt, L. P. (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 5 pp. 6995). Ablex.Google Scholar
ManyPrimates. (2019). Collaborative open science as a way to reproducibility and new insights in primate cognition research. Japanese Psychological Review, 62(3), 205220. https://doi.org/10.24602/sjpr.62.3_205.Google Scholar
Moshontz, H., Campbell, L., Ebersole, C. R., IJzerman, H., Urry, H. L., Forscher, P. S., … Chartier, C. R. (2018). The psychological science accelerator: Advancing psychology through a distributed collaborative network. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(4), 501515. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918797607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oakes, L. M. (2017). Sample size, statistical power, and false conclusions in infant looking-time research. Infancy, 22(4), 436469. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarf, D., Imuta, K., Colombo, M., & Hayne, H. (2012). Social evaluation or simple association? Simple associations may explain moral reasoning in infants. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e42698. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surian, L., & Geraci, A. (2012). Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 3044. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02046.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
The ManyBabies Consortium. (2020). Quantifying sources of variability in infancy research using the infant-directed-speech preference. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(1), 2452. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919900809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar