Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-b6zl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-05T12:23:06.140Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Birth weight interacts with a functional variant of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) to predict executive functioning in children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 May 2017

Mark Wade
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Heather Prime
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Thomas J. Hoffmann
Affiliation:
University of California at San Francisco
Louis A. Schmidt
Affiliation:
McMaster University
Thomas G. O'Connor
Affiliation:
University of Rochester
Jennifer M. Jenkins*
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Jennifer M. Jenkins, Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto, 252 Bloor Street West, Toronto, ON M5S 1V6, Canada; E-mail: jenny.jenkins@utoronto.ca.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with several psychiatric conditions characterized by deficits in executive functioning (EF). A specific OXTR variant, rs2254298, has previously been associated with brain functioning in regions implicated in EF. Moreover, birth weight variation across the entire range is associated with individual differences in cortical structure and function that underlie EF. This is the first study to examine the main and interactive effect between rs2254298 and birth weight on EF in children. The sample consisted of 310 children from an ongoing longitudinal study. EF was measured at age 4.5 using observational tasks indexing working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control. A family-based design that controlled for population admixture, stratification, and nongenomic confounds was employed. A significant genetic association between rs2254298 and EF was observed, with more copies of the major allele (G) associated with higher EF. There was also a significant interaction between rs2254298 and birth weight, such that more copies of the major allele in combination with higher birth weight predicted better EF. Findings suggest that OXTR may be associated with discrete neurocognitive abilities in childhood, and these effects may be modulated by intrauterine conditions related to fetal growth and development.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Recent initiatives from the National Institute of Mental Health such as the Research Domain Criteria highlight the importance of identifying dimensions of cognitive and psychosocial functioning that cut across multiple psychiatric categories. These “endophenotypes” are believed to carry pathways of risk for many psychopathological outcomes (Gottesman & Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003). Endophenotypes are measureable psychological or biological phenomena that are intermediate in the causal chain linking genetic liabilities to traditionally defined clinical disorders. These help to explain heterogeneity within disorders, comorbidity across disorders, and the systematization of disease classification (Miller & Rockstroh, Reference Miller and Rockstroh2013). As a result, understanding the genetic contributions to endophenotypes has considerable significance in elucidating the etiology and ultimate classification of psychiatric disorders (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2014).

Executive functioning (EF) is a prototypical endophenotype that has been implicated in several psychiatric and neurodevelopmental conditions, including schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, autism, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Luna, Doll, Hegedus, Minshew, & Sweeney, Reference Luna, Doll, Hegedus, Minshew and Sweeney2007; Minzenberg, Laird, Thelen, Cater, & Glahn, Reference Minzenberg, Laird, Thelen, Carter and Glahn2009; Mur, Portella, Martinez-Arán, Pifarré, & Vieta, Reference Mur, Portella, Martínez-Arán, Pifarré and Vieta2007; Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, Reference Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone and Pennington2005). EF refers to a clustering of higher order cognitive abilities that support goal-directed action, problem solving, and social functioning. Childhood EF sets the foundation for neuropsychological growth into adulthood (Garon, Bryson, & Smith, Reference Garon, Bryson and Smith2008) and may help to explain continuities in psychiatric difficulties across the life course (Rutter, Kim-Cohen, & Maughan, Reference Rutter, Kim-Cohen and Maughan2006). Thus, identifying genetic liabilities for EF problems in childhood constitutes an important avenue in predicting risk of psychopathology in many domains.

Behavioral genetic studies suggest that EF is heritable (Friedman, Miyake, Robinson, & Hewitt, Reference Friedman, Miyake, Robinson and Hewitt2011; Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Young, DeFries, Corley and Hewitt2008), yet the discrete molecular genetic constituents of EF remain largely unspecified. The preponderance of research points to the monoaminergic genes for dopamine, noradrenaline, and serotonin in the pathogenesis of EF impairment. These genes are believed to exert their effects on EF through prefrontal cortical functioning (Arnsten, Reference Arnsten2011). Nevertheless, the proportion of variance accounted for by individual single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) is small, usually on the order of <1%. This underscores the considerable gap between behavioral and molecular genetic studies known as “missing heritability.” These findings suggest a need to broaden the search for molecular genetic influences on EF.

A separate but related field of inquiry centers on genes of the neurohypophysial hormones, such as oxytocin (OXT). OXT is a known neuromodulator of social behavior and stress regulation, and has been proposed as a key contributor to many of the same psychiatric disorders that are characterized by EF deficits, including autism spectrum disorders, affective disorders, and schizophrenia (Feldman, Monakhov, Pratt, & Ebstein, Reference Feldman, Monakhov, Pratt and Ebstein2016). These effects are frequently attributable to disruptions in social functioning, consistent with the role of OXT in face recognition, altruism, empathy, and the ability to infer others’ mental states (Bartz, Zaki, Bolger, & Ochsner, Reference Bartz, Zaki, Bolger and Ochsner2011; Meyer-Lindenberg, Domes, Kirsch, & Heinichs, Reference Meyer-Lindenberg, Domes, Kirsch and Heinrichs2011). However, these phenomenologically complex behaviors may partly depend on executive functions such as attentional control, set shifting, working memory, and response inhibition. For instance, it has been suggested that empathy and mentalization draw on the ability to inhibit and shift perspectives (Benson, Sabbagh, Carlson, & Zelazo, Reference Benson, Sabbagh, Carlson and Zelazo2013; Vetter, Altgassen, Phillips, Mahy, & Kliegel, Reference Vetter, Altgassen, Phillips, Mahy and Kliegel2013), while altruistic sharing has been shown to engage inhibitory control strategies (Aguilar-Pardo, Martinez-Arias, & Colmenares, Reference Aguilar-Pardo, Martinez-Arias and Colmenares2013). OXT has recently been linked to “nonsocial” aspects of cognition, including spatial and episodic memory, short-term verbal memory, cognitive flexibility, and the executive component of working memory in schizophrenia (Chini, Leonzino, Braida, & Sala, Reference Chini, Leonzino, Braida and Sala2014; Michalopoulou et al., Reference Michalopoulou, Averbeck, Kalpakidou, Evans, Bobin, Kapur and Shergill2015). These effects are further supported by recent evidence suggesting that, within the postpartum period, blockage of the OXT receptor (OXTR) inhibits cognitive flexibility among mothers (Albin-Brooks, Nealer, Sabihi, Haim, & Leuner, Reference Albin-Brooks, Nealer, Sabihi, Haim and Leuner2017). Thus, there is growing evidence that traditionally “social” nonapeptides such as OXT may be related to discrete executive abilities that are impaired in psychiatric conditions and required for adaptive social behavior.

Most genetic research on the behavioral correlates of OXT has focused on variability in the OXTR gene. This gene (located on chromosome 3p25.3) has many SNPs in both the intronic and exonic regions. The most extensively studied SNPs, and those with the most support in both healthy and clinical samples, are rs53576 and rs2254298 (see Feldman et al., Reference Feldman, Monakhov, Pratt and Ebstein2016; Kumsta & Heinrichs, Reference Kumsta and Heinrichs2013). The current study focused on the latter SNP (rs2254298) for two reasons. First, there is strong meta-analytic evidence that this SNP is associated with neurodevelopmental conditions typified by EF impairment, especially after accounting for study bias (LoParo & Waldman, Reference LoParo and Waldman2015); and second, neuroimaging studies specifically point to rs2254298 as relating to structural and functional brain differences in regions implicated in EF. In particular, rs2254298 has been associated with regional volumetric differences in the amygdala and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) in both adults and children (Furman, Chen, & Gotlib, Reference Furman, Chen and Gotlib2011; Inoue et al., Reference Inoue, Yamasue, Tochigi, Abe, Liu, Kawamura and Rogers2010). The functionality of rs2254298, including the coupling of dACC to limbic structures, has been replicated in both Caucasian and Japanese samples (Tost et al., Reference Tost, Kolachana, Verchinski, Bilek, Goldman, Mattay and Meyer-Lindenberg2011; Yamasue et al., Reference Yamasue, Suga, Yahata, Inoue, Tochigi, Abe and Takei2011). Wang et al. (Reference Wang, Braskie, Hafzalla, Faskowitz, McMahon, de Zubicaray and Thompson2017) recently demonstrated that, among 10 genotyped OXTR SNPs, only rs2254298 was associated with resting-state functional connectivity between the posterior cingulate cortex and the dACC. Moreover, A-allele carriers demonstrated reduced cortical thickness in the bilateral dACC compared to individuals with a GG genotype. These findings are interesting given that nearly all models of neurocognitive functioning posit a central role of the dACC in the modulation of EF (Bush, Luu, & Posner, Reference Bush, Luu and Posner2000; Niendam et al., Reference Niendam, Laird, Ray, Dean, Glahn and Carter2012). Finally, a recent study found that rs2254298 was associated with parenting behavior through maternal decision making, supporting the notion that the effect of OXTR on social behavior may operate through discrete executive functions in adults (Cost et al., Reference Cost, Unternaehrer, Plamondon, Steiner, Meaney, Atkinson and Fleming2016). Despite these clear connections, and that many studies have examined rs2254298 in clinical conditions with documented EF impairment, no study has examined whether genetic variability in OXTR is associated with EF in children. Thus, the first goal of the present study was to examine whether genetic variability in rs2254298 is associated with individual differences in childhood EF. A sample of preschool-aged children was selected in light of the importance of early-emerging cognitive skills in the developmental onset of neuropsychiatric disorders and social behavior.

The second goal of this study was to determine whether OXTR interacted with birth weight variability on EF. Birth weight is a proxy for intrauterine growth related to the quality of the prenatal environment. Variance in neurocognitive functioning and psychopathology in adulthood involves the programming of biobehavioral systems that can often be traced to fetal development (Schlotz & Phillips, Reference Schlotz and Phillips2009). Studies suggest that normative birth weight variation is associated with individual differences in EF in the preschool period (Wade, Browne, Madigan, Plamondon, & Jenkins, Reference Wade, Browne, Madigan, Plamondon and Jenkins2014). Perhaps more interesting, it has been shown that birth weight is linearly associated with increased regional surface area and volume in the anterior cingulate cortex of children and adolescents, which in turn is associated with improved cognitive control (Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Brown, Kuperman, Chung, Hagler and Akshoomoff2013). Thus, variation in prenatal neurodevelopment appears to be linked to higher order executive functions across childhood (Raznahan, Greenstein, Lee, Clasen, & Giedd, Reference Raznahan, Greenstein, Lee, Clasen and Giedd2012). It has also been suggested that cortical differences associated with birth weight may interact with genetic risks for psychiatric disorders (Haukvik et al., Reference Haukvik, Rimol, Roddey, Hartberg, Lange, Vaskinn and Agartz2013).

A gene–environment interaction between OXTR and birth weight is indicated by several strands of evidence. First, evolutionary models suggest that the rs2254298 A (minor) allele may be a susceptibility allele that, if combined with adversity during childhood or the prenatal period, increases the risk of developing psychopathologies (Brüne, Reference Brüne2012). Second, studies on the epigenetic modulation of OXTR have demonstrated that the degree of DNA methylation is associated with neural activity in the dACC during a social attention task (Jack, Connelly, & Morris, Reference Jack, Connelly and Morris2012). This provides molecular evidence that OXTR modification may be associated with brain functioning in regions that support EF. In addition, recent studies have documented global DNA methylation patterns associated with perinatal outcomes such as birth weight (Michels, Harris, & Barault, Reference Michels, Harris and Barault2011). Consistent with both the evolutionary and molecular accounts, it has recently been shown that prenatal adversity is specifically linked to DNA methylation of OXTR in cord blood at birth (Unternaehrer et al., Reference Unternaehrer, Bolten, Nast, Staehli, Meyer, Dempster and Meinlschmidt2016). Given that birth weight is a marker of fetal development that is influenced by myriad maternal adversities, these results raise the possibility that intrauterine conditions related to fetal growth may contribute to an epigenetic mechanism of fetal programming that impacts later cognitive and mental health outcomes of children. Thus, in the current study we tested the hypothesis that birth weight variability interacts with OXTR in the prediction of children's EF. We hypothesized that children would show more problems with EF as a function of lower birth weight in combination with more copies of the previously demonstrated risk allele (A) of OXTR marker rs2254298.

Methods

Study sample

Multiparous women giving birth to infants in the cities of Toronto and Hamilton, Ontario, Canada, between 2006 and 2008, who had been contacted by the Healthy Babies Healthy Children public health program (run by Toronto and Hamilton Public Health Units), were considered for participation. Inclusion criteria for the intensive sample of the Kids, Families, Places study were as follows: (a) an English-speaking mother, (b) a newborn weighing >1500 g, (c) one or more children <4 years old in the home, and (d) agreement to the collection of observational and biological data. Thirty-four percent of mothers whose information was passed by the Healthy Babies Healthy Children program consented to participate in the study. Reasons for nonenlistment included inability to contact families, ineligibility once contacted, and refusals. The current sample was nested within a larger longitudinal project, the goals of which were to examine contextual and biological influences on children's social–emotional and cognitive development using within-family methodology, thus requiring a minimum of two children per family. However, as we were interested in EF in the preschool period, only target newborns were included. The University of Toronto Research Ethics Board approved all procedures, including informed consent.

Data for the current study were primarily drawn from Wave 4 of the Kids, Families, Places longitudinal study, at which point children were about 4.5 years old (N = 323; M age = 4.79 years, SD = 0.28). Attrition up to Wave 4 related to younger maternal age at first pregnancy, t (494) = –5.10, p < .001, lower socioeconomic status, t (498) = –5.07, p < .001, and lower maternal education, t (498) = –2.99, p < .005. Of the 323 children participating at Wave 4, outcome data on EF were unavailable for 12 of them due to child noncompliance or tester administration error. One additional child was excluded because he was 5.87 SD above the birth weight mean. The final sample therefore consisted of 310 children (51% female). Demographics of the study sample are provided in Table 1. The sample consisted of typically developing children covering a wide range of birth weight (1900–5000 g), consistent with recent findings that variability in this range is associated with individual differences in neurocognitive development.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study variables

a This is a standardized score with a mean of zero.

b Reported on a scale from 1 (no income) to 16 ($105,000 or more).

Procedure

A home visit of approximately 2 hr was conducted in which parents (usually the mother) filled out questionnaires pertaining to family life, sociodemographic information, and child health and behavior. Children also underwent a battery of observational and standardized tasks assessing multiple domains of functioning, including EF (described below).

Measures

EF

This was assessed using two previously developed and widely used tasks that were appropriate to the age of the child: Bear/Dragon (Reed, Pien, & Rothbart, Reference Reed, Pien and Rothbart1984) and the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS; Zelazo, Reference Zelazo2006). For the Bear/Dragon task, children were instructed to do what they were told by the nice bear (e.g., “touch your nose”), but not to do what they were told by the mean dragon. Children were scored on their total number of correct responses (0–10) on five dragon and five bear trials. For the DCCS, children were required to sort a series of bivalent test cards, first according to one dimension (e.g., color), and then according to the other (e.g., shape). Children who passed the postswitch phase of the standard version of the DCCS proceeded immediately to the border version, which used the same target cards as the standard version. The border version consisted of 12 trials. Children were required to sort cards based on “border” criteria (“If there's a border, play the color game. If there's no border, play the shape game”). Previous studies have shown that the Bear/Dragon and DCCS load onto the same latent factor measuring set shifting/cognitive flexibility, working memory, and inhibitory control (Bernier, Carlson, Deschênes, & Matte-Gagné, Reference Bernier, Carlson, Deschênes and Matte-Gagné2012). In the present study, Bear/Dragon and DCCS were significantly correlated, r (299) = .34 p < .001. Thus, the two tasks were z scored and averaged to create a composite, with higher scores reflecting better EF ability.

Birth weight

At Wave 1 (child was a newborn), mothers reported on their child's birth weight in kilograms or grams. Following the removal of one baby who was an outlier, described above, this variable was normally distributed: skewness = 0.022 (SE = 0.14), kurtosis = 0.60 (SE = 0.28). The range on the variable was 1.90–4.99 kg (M = 3.42, SD = 0.48). We residualized birth weight for gestational age to give a purer metric of fetal growth (Phua, Rifkin-Graboi, Saw, Meaney, & Qiu, Reference Phua, Rifkin-Graboi, Saw, Meaney and Qiu2012).Footnote 1

Covariates

Several covariates were controlled in the adjusted analysis including family income/assets, which was measured as a composite that included annual family income, assessed on a scale from 1 (no income) to 16 ($105,000 or more); number of rooms in the family's residence; and whether the family owns/co-owns their house/apartment and/or a vehicle (yes/no variables). These four variables were standardized, and averaged to create a composite for income/assets. Higher values represented higher SES; maternal education, assessed as the total number of years of formal schooling, not including kindergarten; child age (in years and months); and gender (0 = male, 1 = female).

Genotyping

DNA was extracted from cheek swab samples from both children and their biological parents. The rs2254298 SNP was genotyped with the ABI 7900-HT Sequence Detection System® (Applied Biosystems) using the TaqMan 5′ nuclease assay for allelic discrimination. The polymerase chain reactions (10 μl volume) contained 30 ng of genomic DNA, 10 μmol of TaqMan® Universal PCR Master Mix (Applied Biosystems), and 0.25 μl of allelic discrimination mix (Applied Biosystems) containing 36 μM of each primer and 8 μM of each probe. The thermal cycling conditions were 50 °C for 2 min, 95 °C for 10 min, followed by 40 cycles of 94 °C for 15 s and the annealing temperature of 60 °C for 1 min. Each 96 well plate contained two negative controls. Plates were then read on the ABI 7900HT Sequence Detection System using the allelic discrimination end-point analysis mode of Sequence Detection System software package version 2.0 (Applied Biosystems).

Regarding SNP characteristics of rs2254298, the major allele (G) had a frequency of .88. This marker is located in intron 3 of the OXTR gene, with a chromosome position of 8,802,227. This SNP conformed to Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (p = .12) in this sample.

Statistical analysis

The analysis was carried out using the Pedigree-Based Association Test (PBAT) with Golden Helix SVS. PBAT is robust to population admixture and stratification, meaning population subgroups and ethnic differences in allelic distributions are controlled. Specifically, the alleles transmitted to probands are compared to the distribution of alleles expected among offspring, conditional on parental genotypes and based on Mendelian transmission. In other words, parents are used as pseudocontrols. The result is an unbiased test statistic that allows inferences for both association and linkage. We tested an additive model in which genotypes were coded to reflect the number of copies of each allele. The quantitative trait, EF, was offset by the phenotypic mean to generate a phenotypic residual for each person (i.e., mean centered). This increases the statistical power of the PBAT statistic (Lange, DeMeo, & Laird, Reference Lange, DeMeo and Laird2002). To further improve power, we controlled for nongenomic factors (covariates) related to attrition and known to be associated with EF (Laird & Lange, Reference Laird and Lange2008).

Computation of the main genetic and interaction effect followed the procedure outlined by Vansteelandt et al. (Reference Vansteelandt, DeMeo, Lasky-Su, Smoller, Murphy, McQueen and Silverman2008). The approach uses a semiparametric model: there is a nonparametric component for the main environmental effect that is robust to misspecification (Moerkerke, Vansteelandt, & Lange, Reference Moerkerke, Vansteelandt and Lange2010), and a parametric component for the main genetic and the gene–environment interaction effect. We fit the model:

$$E\left( {{\rm Y}_{ij} \vert X_{ij}\comma \, Z_{ij}\comma \, S_i} \right){\rm} = {\rm \mu} (Z_{ij}\comma \, S_i ) + {\rm \beta} _{{\rm SNP}} X_{ij} + {\rm \beta} _{{\rm inter}} X_{ij} Z_{ij}\comma $$

where i indexes the family, j the offspring, Y is the trait (i.e., EF), X is the additive coding of the genotype (0, 1, or 2 copies), Z is the environmental exposure (i.e., birth weight), S encodes the dependency on the parental genotypes, and μ(.) is a nonparametric function that encodes the dependence of the trait on the environmental exposure and parental genotype. A positive value for the interaction indicates that there is an increase in EF with more copies of that allele combined with higher levels of the exposure variable (birth weight).

Below, we report results using the family-based association test (FBAT) and FBAT-I p value, heritability coefficient (h 2), and standard effect size estimates where appropriate. The FBAT p value is the primary test statistic for examining the main genetic effect in family-based designs. It is a generalization of the transmission disequilibrium test, appropriate for quantitative traits (Lange et al., Reference Lange, DeMeo and Laird2002). The FBAT-I p value tests the null hypothesis of no gene–environment interaction. The heritability coefficient (h 2) reflects the proportion of phenotypic variance explained by the analyzed SNP or interaction, and is thus a measure of effect size.

Results

Table 2 reports bivariate correlations among study variables. For the outcome phenotype, EF, significant associations with age, maternal education, and family income/assets were observed, while there were no gender differences on EF. There were no gender differences in the distribution of the G allele, χ2 (df = 1) = 0.022, p = .88, or the A allele, χ2 (df = 1) = 0.072, p = .79. There were also no gender differences in overall genotype, χ2 (df = 2) = 0.080, p = .96. All covariates were retained in the adjusted models outlined below.

Table 2. Bivariate correlations between study variables

p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Computation of the interaction effect using PBAT assumes genotype–environment independence, which is generally a reasonable assumption in family-based tests (Umbach & Weinberg, Reference Umbach and Weinberg2000). To confirm this assumption, we performed a preliminary association analysis between the selected OXTR SNP, rs2254298, and the environmental exposure, birth weight. There was no significant relation between rs2254298 and birth weight in either the unadjusted model (FBAT p = .54, h 2 = 0.003) or the adjusted model that controlled for covariates (p = .53, h 2 < 0.001), supporting model assumptions.

Regarding the main genetic effect, there was a significant association between the rs2254298 marker and EF in the unadjusted analysis (FBAT p = .049) and in the adjusted analysis controlling for covariates (FBAT p = .023). With regard to direction of effects, more copies of the major allele (G) were associated with higher EF, while more copies of the minor allele (A) were associated with lower EF. We then tested the FBAT association test of the interaction effect (FBAT-I), which revealed a significant interaction between rs2254298 and birth weight in both the unadjusted (FBAT-I p = .011) and adjusted analyses (FBAT-I p = .003).

Next, we examined the main genetic and interaction effects separately, each controlling for the other. In the unadjusted model, the main genetic effect (known as QBAT) was not significant for the G allele, β (SE) = 0.20 (0.13), p = .10, h 2 = 0.014, or the A allele, β (SE) = –0.16 (0.14), p = .27, h 2 = 0.010. Note that the p-values are not identical for the two alleles because of the semiparametric model used. Effects were likewise nonsignificant in the adjusted model for the G allele, β (SE) = 0.18 (0.13), p = .16, h 2 = 0.014, and the A allele, β (SE) = –0.13 (0.14), p = .36, h 2 = 0.010. In contrast, strong gene–environment interaction effects were observed between rs2254298 and birth weight. In the unadjusted model, the interaction (known as QBAT-E) was significant for the G allele, β (SE) = 0.26 (0.09), p = .0025, h 2 = 0.27, and for the A allele, β (SE) = –0.46 (0.17), p = .007, h 2 = 0.052. Results were robust in the adjusted model, with a significant interaction for the G allele, β (SE) = 0.31 (0.11), p = .0045, h 2 = 0.52, and for the A allele, β (SE) = –0.62 (0.22), p = .0051, h 2 = 0.12. The direction of effects was consistent with study hypotheses, namely, that EF increased as a function of more copies of the G (major) allele in combination with higher birth weight, and decreased with more copies of the A (minor) allele and lower birth weight. There were no significant Gene × Gender × Birth Weight interactions.

Discussion

This study aimed to determine whether a functional genetic variant in the OXTR gene, rs2254298, was associated with children's EF in the preschool period. Moreover, we aimed to examine whether OXTR interacted with children's birth weight on EF, consistent with the known consequences of birth weight variability on neural functioning in brain regions that support EF (Raznahan et al., Reference Raznahan, Greenstein, Lee, Clasen and Giedd2012; Walhovd et al., Reference Walhovd, Fjell, Brown, Kuperman, Chung, Hagler and Akshoomoff2013). We demonstrated that more copies of the major allele G were associated with higher EF, while more copies of the minor allele A were associated with lower EF. More interesting was the strong interaction between OXTR marker rs2254298 and birth weight in the prediction of EF, consistent with the idea that genetic effects may be amplified in the context of predisposing environments (Manuck & McCaffery, Reference Manuck and McCaffery2014). The current findings suggest that birth weight may be a strong modulator of OXTR’s effects on EF. These results align with previous findings on the significant impact of birth weight on postnatal cognitive and neurobehavioral functioning (Aarnoudse-Moens, Weiglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever, & Oosterlaan, Reference Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van Goudoever and Oosterlaan2009).

This is the first study to demonstrate that OXTR is related to childhood EF, and also the first to show that such effects operate in tandem with biomedical risks for neurocognitive development in young children. Previous studies have shown that another neurohypophysial hormone receptor gene, arginine vasopressin receptor 1a (AVPR1A), is also associated with EF in children (Wade, Hoffmann, & Jenkins, Reference Wade, Hoffmann and Jenkins2014). Moreover, in adults, AVPR1A and OXTR have been differentially associated with cognitive and emotional empathy, respectively (Uzefovsky et al., Reference Uzefovsky, Shalev, Israel, Edelman, Raz, Mankuta and Ebstein2015). Both types of empathy may draw on executive skills, including the capacity to inhibit self-perspectives and shift reference frames in order to understand the experiences of others. Thus, it is plausible that these two interrelated neurohypophysial systems operate together to support EF and the social behaviors that rely on these abilities. Furthermore, studies of OXTR are beginning to document the interactive effect of common polymorphisms with environmental factors such as prenatal testosterone exposure (Weisman et al., Reference Weisman, Pelphrey, Leckman, Feldman, Lu, Chong and Ebstein2015) and postnatal parenting behavior (Wade, Hoffmann, & Jenkins, Reference Wade, Hoffmann and Jenkins2015) on social cognition. The current study adds to this growing literature and further implicates birth weight variability as a moderator of OXTR on the executive control processes that may facilitate social cognition.

Intranasal administration of OXT has been shown to improve emotion recognition in youth with autism (Guastella et al., Reference Guastella, Einfeld, Gray, Rinehart, Tonge, Lambert and Hickie2010), and has a modest effect on symptomatology of other disorders (e.g., depression, posttraumatic stress disorder, anxiety, schizophrenia, obsessive–compulsive disorder, and bipolar disorder; Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn2013). The effects of OXT on cognition and behavior may depend on early biological or environmental conditions that regulate the expression of OXTR, which mediates the action of OXT (Feldman et al., Reference Feldman, Monakhov, Pratt and Ebstein2016). It has been shown that the third intron of OXTR contains a motif of 10–15 nucleotides that bind nuclear suppression proteins associated with downregulation of the gene (Mizumoto, Kimura, & Ivell, Reference Mizumoto, Kimura and Ivell1997). The rs2254298 marker is localized near or within this genomic fragment, suggesting that it may be involved in the expression of OXTR via molecular or environmental modulation. The current results provide preliminary evidence that rs2254298 A-allele carriers who also experience early biomedical adversity are at heightened risk for EF problems in the preschool period. However, these findings cannot definitively speak to whether downregulation of OXTR as a function of lower birth weight accounts for the EF difficulties of A-allele carriers. Thus, future research is needed to elucidate the mechanism(s) through which genetic variability and exposure to biomedical conditions differentially alters responsiveness to OXT on cognition and behavior.

Within the field of psychiatry there is an ongoing paradigm shift away from nosological approaches to disease classification and toward more nuanced characterizations that are idiographic and person centered. This entails the identification of cross-cutting endophenotypes that underlie many domains of psychopathology. Such an approach parallels the Research Domain Criteria and the NIMH's endorsement of studies that utilize dimensional and multilevel analysis of endophenotypes that are the product of dynamically interacting genetic and environmental exposures. Gene–environment interaction studies are well positioned to expand the bioecological scope of inquiry around the extent to which readily–identifiable risk factors and genetic variants together confer vulnerability for cognitive deficits that underlie psychopathology (Hunter, Reference Hunter2005). Recognizing the joint contribution of genetic and environmental risks for neuropsychiatric problems may in turn foster more precise methods for identifying at-risk individuals, pinpoint targets for prevention and intervention, and further characterize the conditions under which therapies yield efficacious results.

Finally, it is noteworthy that, among biomedically at-risk children, early psychosocial interventions have powerful effects on cognitive development. In particular, programs that build parenting competence have been shown to engender positive resultant effects on children's behavioral, social, and cognitive outcomes (Landry et al., Reference Landry, Smith, Swank, Zucker, Crawford and Solari2012). Interventions designed to enhance mother–child reciprocity and synchrony have been shown not only to enhance executive functions up to age 10 but also to improve sleep, reduce stress response, and increase vagal tone, suggesting that improvements in physiological organization among biologically at-risk children can be maintained via dynamic cascades involving early parental enrichment (Feldman, Rosenthal, & Eidelman, Reference Feldman, Rosenthal and Eidelman2014). Determining whether such intervention effects vary as a function of children's unique genetic makeup and biomedical history will further facilitate the personalization of treatment that considers multiple levels of biobehavioral adjustment.

Limitations and future directions

The primary limitation of this study is the lack of a replication sample, which lends to the possibility of spurious associations. Likewise, gene–environment interactions are susceptible to unreliability (Almli et al., Reference Almli, Duncan, Feng, Ghosh, Binder, Bradley and Epstein2014). Future replication is encouraged to enhance our understanding of the genetic and gene–environment relationship between OXTR and EF in childhood and later periods of development. Another potential limitation was reliance on mothers to report their child's birth weight; however, such reports have been shown to be reliable when compared to medical records in epidemiological studies (Tomeo et al., Reference Tomeo, Rich-Edwards, Michels, Berkey, Hunter, Frazier and Buka1999). In addition, birth weight is a broad marker of fetal growth, and may be additionally influenced by maternal body mass index, pregnancy complications related to prematurity, and maternal hip size (which shows ethnic disparities). While gestational age and family ethnicity were accounted for in the current analysis, the precise mechanism through which the fetal environment impacts EF alongside genetic risk requires more nuanced prenatal and molecular assessment in future studies.

Moreover, birth weight itself is partially heritable (Clausson, Leonizino, Braida, & Sala, Reference Clausson, Lichtenstein and Cnattingius2000), leading to the possibility that the current results reflect a complex set of interactions between genes at different loci and/or intrauterine conditions. Similarly, the effect of birth weight may include or be explained by postnatal factors that systematically covary with lower birth weight, such as time spent in neonatal intensive care and associated decreases in maternal touch and stimulation. While we cannot rule out these possibilities, our relatively low-risk community sample and replication in children weighing ≥2500 g likely assuages some of these concerns. It is also worth mentioning that medications needed for either encouraging or delaying labor often include OXT-relevant compounds or antagonists. Uterine contractility and OXT sensitivity during pregnancy increase as gestational age increases, and may partially predict timing of delivery (Takahashi, Diamond, Bieniarz, Yen, & Burd, Reference Takahashi, Diamond, Bieniarz, Yen and Burd1980). However, it is currently unclear how either maternal OXTR expression or circulating OXT levels during pregnancy or labor, which show marked interindividual variability, impact fetal outcomes (Levine, Zagoory-Sharon, Feldman, & Weller, Reference Levine, Zagoory-Sharon, Feldman and Weller2007), nor how child OXTR modulates such effects. In the current study, child OXTR genotype was unrelated to birth weight. Nevertheless, future research examining how child OXTR interacts with circulating OXT during pregnancy may shed further light on the mechanisms at play. For instance, it has been shown that a rising pattern of plasma OXT from early to late pregnancy improves mother–fetal bonding (Levine et al., Reference Levine, Zagoory-Sharon, Feldman and Weller2007), and OXT levels during pregnancy positively correlate with more optimal maternal behaviors that facilitate bonding in the postpartum period (Feldman, Weller, Zagoory-Sharon, & Levine, Reference Feldman, Weller, Zagoory-Sharon and Levine2007). Examining whether the degree of OXT exposure during pregnancy/labor differentially relates to child outcomes as a function of child OXTR genotype, and the meditational role of maternal behavior in this mechanism, is a rich avenue for future research. Likewise, it will be worth exploring how these effects operate in higher risk samples, including those with more pre/perinatal complications and associated developmental difficulties, to determine the robustness of effects (and comparability of mechanisms) across various groups of children. By broadening the range of pre/perinatal experiences of children, continued gene–environment interaction research in this area may shed light on whether the A allele is a “risk” allele or a “plasticity” allele, consistent with evolutionary models (Brüne, Reference Brüne2012) that consider how such low-frequency mutations may have survived over evolutionary time via their dynamic interplay with the environment.

Conclusion

In summary, the current study shows that a marker of the OXTR gene, rs2254298, which has been previously implicated in several psychiatric and neurodevelopmental conditions, and is functionally related to the neurocircuitry that supports cognitive control, is related to children's EF at age 4.5. Moreover, a robust interaction of this marker with birth weight was observed, such that children with more copies of the G allele in combination with higher birth weight showed better EF compared to their A-allele counterparts. This is the first study to show either main or interactive effects of OXTR on EF in childhood. These findings highlight one neurobiological pathway by which oxytocin gene variants may increase risk for psychopathology, namely, by compromising underlying executive capacities. Moreover, EF is not only an important endophenotype for psychiatric morbidity but also an indispensible source of human capital that serves as a keystone predictor of educational, occupational, socioeconomic, and developmental health across the life span. Uncovering the ways in which genetic variants combine with environmental factors to shape candidate endophenotypes remains a ripe area for future research.

Footnotes

We are grateful to the families who give so generously of their time, to the Hamilton and Toronto Public Health Units for facilitating recruitment of the sample, and to Mira Boskovic for project management. The “Transactional Processes in Emotional and Behavioral Regulation: Individuals in Context” grant was awarded to Jennifer M. Jenkins and Michael Boyle from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR Funding Reference 70334) and covered data collection. Mark Wade was also funded by a CIHR doctoral fellowship. The study team beyond the current authors includes Cathy Barr, Kathy Georgiades, Greg Moran, Michal Perlman, and Hildy Ross.

1. Effects were reexamined when birth weight was not residualized for gestational age, and the results were robust. We also performed a separate analysis in which we residualized birth weight for ethnicity, and the substantive results again did not change. Restricting the sample to children born weighing ≥2500 g did not affect the results; thus, all children were retained in the final analysis.

References

Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S. H., Weisglas-Kuperus, N., van Goudoever, J. B., & Oosterlaan, J. (2009). Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics, 124, 717728. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-2816 Google Scholar
Aguilar-Pardo, D., Martinez-Arias, R., & Colmenares, F. (2013). The role of inhibition in young children's altruistic behaviour. Cognitive Processing, 14, 301307. doi:10.1007/s10339-013-0552-6 Google Scholar
Albin-Brooks, C. C., Nealer, C. A., Sabihi, S., Haim, A., & Leuner, B. (2017). The influence of offspring, parity, and oxytocin on cognitive flexibility during the postpartum period. Hormones and Behavior, 89, 130136. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.12.015 Google Scholar
Almli, L. M., Duncan, R., Feng, H., Ghosh, D., Binder, E. B., Bradley, B., … Epstein, M. P. (2014). Correcting systematic inflation in genetic association tests that consider interaction effects: Application to a genome-wide association study of posttraumatic stress disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 71, 13921399. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1339 Google Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. (2011). Catecholamine influences on dorsolateral prefrontal cortical networks. Biological Psychiatry, 69, e89e99. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.027 Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M., & van IJzendoorn, M. (2013). Sniffing around oxytocin: Review and meta-analyses of trials in healthy and clinical groups with implications for pharmacotherapy. Translational Psychiatry, 3, e258. doi:10.1038/tp.2013.34 Google Scholar
Bartz, J. A., Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. N. (2011). Social effects of oxytocin in humans: Context and person matter. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 301309. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.002 Google ScholarPubMed
Benson, J. E., Sabbagh, M. A., Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers’ improvement from theory-of-mind training. Developmental Psychology, 49, 1615. doi:10.1037/a0031056 Google Scholar
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M., & Matte-Gagné, C. (2012). Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: A closer look at the caregiving environment. Developmental Science, 15, 1224. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01093.x Google Scholar
Brüne, M. (2012). Does the oxytocin receptor polymorphism (rs2254298) confer “vulnerability” for psychopathology or “differential susceptibility”? Insights from evolution. BMC Medicine, 10, 38. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-10-38 Google Scholar
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 215222. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2 Google Scholar
Chini, B., Leonzino, M., Braida, D., & Sala, M. (2014). Learning about oxytocin: Pharmacologic and behavioral issues. Biological Psychiatry, 76, 360366. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.029 Google Scholar
Clausson, B., Lichtenstein, P., & Cnattingius, S. (2000). Genetic influence on birthweight and gestational length determined by studies in offspring of twins. British Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 107, 375381. doi:10.1111/j.1471-0528.2000.tb13234.x Google Scholar
Cost, K. T., Unternaehrer, E., Plamondon, A., Steiner, M., Meaney, M., Atkinson, L., … Fleming, A. S. (2016). Thinking and doing: The effects of dopamine and oxytocin genes and executive function on mothering behaviours. Genes, Brain and Behavior. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/gbb.12337 Google Scholar
Cuthbert, B. N. (2014). Translating intermediate phenotypes to psychopathology: The NIMH Research Domain Criteria. Psychophysiology, 51, 12051206. doi:10.1111/psyp.12342 Google Scholar
Feldman, R., Monakhov, M., Pratt, M., & Ebstein, R. P. (2016). Oxytocin pathway genes: Evolutionary ancient system impacting on human affiliation, sociality, and psychopathology. Biological Psychiatry, 79, 174184. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.08.008 Google Scholar
Feldman, R., Rosenthal, Z., & Eidelman, A. I. (2014). Maternal-preterm skin-to-skin contact enhances child physiologic organization and cognitive control across the first 10 years of life. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 5664. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.012 Google Scholar
Feldman, R., Weller, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Levine, A. (2007). Evidence for a neuroendocrinological foundation of human affiliation: Plasma oxytocin levels across pregnancy and the postpartum period predict mother-infant bonding. Psychological Science, 18, 965970. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02010.x Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Robinson, J. L., & Hewitt, J. K. (2011). Developmental trajectories in toddlers' self-restraint predict individual differences in executive functions 14 years later: A behavioral genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology, 47, 14101430. doi:10.1037/a0023750 Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 201225. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201 Google Scholar
Furman, D. J., Chen, M. C., & Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Variant in oxytocin receptor gene is associated with amygdala volume. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 891897. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.12.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 3160. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31 Google Scholar
Gottesman, I. I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 636645. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636 Google Scholar
Guastella, A. J., Einfeld, S. L., Gray, K. M., Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B. J., Lambert, T. J., & Hickie, I. B. (2010). Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 692694. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.020 Google Scholar
Haukvik, U. K., Rimol, L. M., Roddey, J. C., Hartberg, C. B., Lange, E. H., Vaskinn, A., … Agartz, I. (2013). Normal birth weight variation is related to cortical morphology across the psychosis spectrum. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40, 410419. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbt005 Google Scholar
Hunter, D. J. (2005). Gene-environment interactions in human diseases. Nature Reviews Genetics, 6, 287298. doi:10.21775/cimb.010.025 Google Scholar
Inoue, H., Yamasue, H., Tochigi, M., Abe, O., Liu, X., Kawamura, Y., … Rogers, M. A. (2010). Association between the oxytocin receptor gene and amygdalar volume in healthy adults. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 10661072. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2010.07.2479 Google Scholar
Jack, A., Connelly, J. J., & Morris, J. P. (2012). DNA methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene predicts neural response to ambiguous social stimuli. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 280. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00280 Google Scholar
Kumsta, R., & Heinrichs, M. (2013). Oxytocin, stress and social behavior: Neurogenetics of the human oxytocin system. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 1116. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2012.09.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laird, N. M., & Lange, C. (2008). Family-based methods for linkage and association analysis. Advances in Genetics, 60, 219252. doi:10.1016/s0065-2660(07)00410-5 Google Scholar
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Zucker, T., Crawford, A. D., & Solari, E. F. (2012). The effects of a responsive parenting intervention on parent-child interactions during shared book reading. Developmental Psychology, 48, 969. doi:10.1037/a0026400 Google Scholar
Lange, C., DeMeo, D. L., & Laird, N. M. (2002). Power and design considerations for a general class of family-based association tests: Quantitative traits. American Journal of Human Genetics, 71, 13301341. doi:10.1086/344696 Google Scholar
Levine, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Feldman, R., & Weller, A. (2007). Oxytocin during pregnancy and early postpartum: Individual patterns and maternal–fetal attachment. Peptides, 28, 11621169. doi:10.1016/j.peptides.2007.04.016 Google Scholar
LoParo, D., & Waldman, I. D. (2015). The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 20, 640646. doi:10.1038/mp.2014.77 Google Scholar
Luna, B., Doll, S. K., Hegedus, S. J., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 474481. doi:10.1155/2012/146132 Google Scholar
Manuck, S. B., & McCaffery, J. M. (2014). Gene-environment interaction. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 4170. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115100 Google Scholar
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Domes, G., Kirsch, P., & Heinrichs, M. (2011). Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: Social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 524538. doi:10.1038/nrn3044 Google Scholar
Michalopoulou, P. G., Averbeck, B. B., Kalpakidou, A. K., Evans, S., Bobin, T., Kapur, S., & Shergill, S. S. (2015). The effects of a single dose of oxytocin on working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 162, 6263. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2014.12.029 Google Scholar
Michels, K. B., Harris, H. R., & Barault, L. (2011). Birthweight, maternal weight trajectories and global DNA methylation of LINE-1 repetitive elements. PLOS ONE, 6, e25254. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025254 Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., & Rockstroh, B. (2013). Endophenotypes in psychopathology research: Where do we stand? Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 177213. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185540 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minzenberg, M. J., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S., Carter, C. S., & Glahn, D. C. (2009). Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 811822. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91 Google Scholar
Mizumoto, Y., Kimura, T., & Ivell, R. (1997). A genomic element within the third intron of the human oxytocin receptor gene may be involved in transcriptional suppression. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology, 135, 129138. doi:10.1016/s0303-7207(97)00195-0 Google Scholar
Moerkerke, B., Vansteelandt, S., & Lange, C. (2010). A doubly robust test for gene-environment interaction in family-based studies of affected offspring. Biostatistics, 11, 213225. doi:10.1093/biostatistics/kxp061 Google Scholar
Mur, M., Portella, M. J., Martínez-Arán, A., Pifarré, J., & Vieta, E. (2007). Persistent neuropsychological deficit in euthymic bipolar patients: Executive function as a core deficit. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 68, 10781086. doi:10.4088/jcp.v68n0715 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niendam, T. A., Laird, A. R., Ray, K. L., Dean, Y. M., Glahn, D. C., & Carter, C. S. (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 241268. doi:10.3758/s13415-011-0083-5 Google Scholar
Phua, D. Y.-L., Rifkin-Graboi, A., Saw, S.-M., Meaney, M. J., & Qiu, A. (2012). Executive functions of six-year-old boys with normal birth weight and gestational age. PLOS ONE, 7, e36502. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036502 Google Scholar
Raznahan, A., Greenstein, D., Lee, N. R., Clasen, L. S., & Giedd, J. N. (2012). Prenatal growth in humans and postnatal brain maturation into late adolescence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1136611371. doi:10.1073/pnas.1203350109 Google Scholar
Reed, M. A., Pien, D. L., & Rothbart, M. K. (1984). Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 131147.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Kim-Cohen, J., & Maughan, B. (2006). Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 276295. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01614.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlotz, W., & Phillips, D. I. (2009). Fetal origins of mental health: Evidence and mechanisms. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 23, 905916. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2009.02.001 Google Scholar
Takahashi, K., Diamond, F., Bieniarz, J., Yen, H., & Burd, L. (1980). Uterine contractility and oxytocin sensitivity in preterm, term, and postterm pregnancy. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 136, 774779. doi:10.1016/0002-9378(80)90455-X Google Scholar
Tomeo, C. A., Rich-Edwards, J. W., Michels, K. B., Berkey, C. S., Hunter, D. J., Frazier, A. L., … Buka, S. L. (1999). Reproducibility and validity of maternal recall of pregnancy-related events. Epidemiology, 10, 774776. doi:10.1097/00001648-199911000-00022 Google Scholar
Tost, H., Kolachana, B., Verchinski, B. A., Bilek, E., Goldman, A. L., Mattay, V. S., … Meyer-Lindenberg, A., (2011). Neurogenetic effects of OXTR rs2254298 in the extended limbic system of healthy Caucasian adults. Biological Psychiatry, 70, e37e39. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.06.034 Google Scholar
Umbach, D. M., & Weinberg, C. R. (2000). The use of case-parent triads to study joint effects of genotype and exposure. American Journal of Human Genetics, 66, 251261. doi:10.1086/302707 Google Scholar
Unternaehrer, E., Bolten, M., Nast, I., Staehli, S., Meyer, A. H., Dempster, E., … Meinlschmidt, G. (2016). Maternal adversities during pregnancy and cord blood oxytocin receptor (OXTR) DNA methylation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 14601470. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw051 Google Scholar
Uzefovsky, F., Shalev, I., Israel, S., Edelman, S., Raz, Y., Mankuta, D., … Ebstein, R. (2015). Oxytocin receptor and vasopressin receptor 1a genes are respectively associated with emotional and cognitive empathy. Hormones and Behavior, 67, 6065. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.11.007 Google Scholar
Vansteelandt, S., DeMeo, D. L., Lasky-Su, J., Smoller, J. W., Murphy, A. J., McQueen, M., … Silverman, E. K. (2008). Testing and estimating gene-environment interactions in family-based association studies. Biometrics, 64, 458467. doi:10.1111/j.1541-0420.2007.00925.x Google Scholar
Vetter, N. C., Altgassen, M., Phillips, L., Mahy, C. E., & Kliegel, M. (2013). Development of affective theory of mind across adolescence: Disentangling the role of executive functions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 38, 114125. doi:10.1080/87565641.2012.733786 Google Scholar
Wade, M., Browne, D., Madigan, S., Plamondon, A., & Jenkins, J. (2014). Normal birth weight variation and children's neuropsychological functioning: Links between language, executive functioning, and theory of mind. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20, 909919. doi:10.1017/s1355617714000745 Google Scholar
Wade, M., Hoffmann, T. J., & Jenkins, J. M. (2014). Association between the arginine vasopressin receptor 1A (AVPR1A) gene and preschoolers’ executive functioning. Brain and Cognition, 90, 116123. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.002 Google Scholar
Wade, M., Hoffmann, T. J., & Jenkins, J. M. (2015). Gene-environment interaction between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and parenting behaviour on children's theory of mind. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10, 17491757. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv064 Google Scholar
Walhovd, K. B., Fjell, A. M., Brown, T. T., Kuperman, J. M., Chung, Y., Hagler, D. J., … Akshoomoff, N. (2013). Long-term influence of normal variation in neonatal characteristics on human brain development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 2008920094. doi:10.1073/pnas.1208180109 Google Scholar
Wang, J., Braskie, M. N., Hafzalla, G. W., Faskowitz, J., McMahon, K. L., de Zubicaray, G. I., & Thompson, P. M. (2017). Relationship of a common OXTR gene variant to brain structure and default mode network function in healthy humans. NeuroImage, 147, 500506. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.062 Google Scholar
Weisman, O., Pelphrey, K. A., Leckman, J. F., Feldman, R., Lu, Y., Chong, A., … Ebstein, R. P. (2015). The association between 2D: 4D ratio and cognitive empathy is contingent on a common polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR rs53576). Psychoneuroendocrinology, 58, 2332. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.04.007 Google Scholar
Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 13361346. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006 Google Scholar
Yamasue, H., Suga, M., Yahata, N., Inoue, H., Tochigi, M., Abe, O., … Takei, K. (2011). Reply to: Neurogenetic effects of OXTR rs2254298 in the extended limbic system of healthy Caucasian adults. Biological Psychiatry, 70, e41e42. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.021 Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1, 297301. doi:10.1038/nprot.2006.46 Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study variables

Figure 1

Table 2. Bivariate correlations between study variables