Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-v2bm5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-11T10:33:33.110Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive reserve attenuates age-related cognitive decline in the context of putatively accelerated brain ageing in schizophrenia-spectrum disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2019

Tamsyn E. Van Rheenen*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Melbourne, Australia Centre for Mental Health, Faculty of Health, Arts and Design, School of Health Sciences, Swinburne University, Melbourne, Australia
Vanessa Cropley
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Melbourne, Australia Centre for Mental Health, Faculty of Health, Arts and Design, School of Health Sciences, Swinburne University, Melbourne, Australia
Birgitte Fagerlund
Affiliation:
Center for Neuropsychiatric Schizophrenia Research and Center for Clinical Intervention and Neuropsychiatric Schizophrenia Research (CINS), Mental Health Center, Glostrup, Denmark Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
Cassandra Wannan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Melbourne, Australia
Jason Bruggemann
Affiliation:
School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, New South Wales, Australia Neuroscience Research Australia, New South Wales, Australia
Rhoshel K. Lenroot
Affiliation:
School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, New South Wales, Australia Neuroscience Research Australia, New South Wales, Australia
Suresh Sundram
Affiliation:
Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Melbourne, Australia Department of Psychiatry, School of Clinical Sciences, Monash University, Clayton, Australia Mental Health Program, Monash Health, Clayton, Victoria, Australia
Cynthia Shannon Weickert
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Melbourne, Australia School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, New South Wales, Australia Neuroscience Research Australia, New South Wales, Australia Department of Neuroscience & Physiology, Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, New York13210, USA
Thomas W. Weickert
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Melbourne, Australia School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, New South Wales, Australia Neuroscience Research Australia, New South Wales, Australia
Andrew Zalesky
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Melbourne, Australia Department of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University of Melbourne, VIC, Australia
Chad A. Bousman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Melbourne, Australia Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Melbourne, Australia Departments of Medical Genetics, Psychiatry, and Physiology & Pharmacology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
Christos Pantelis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Melbourne, Australia Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Melbourne, Australia Department of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University of Melbourne, VIC, Australia
*
Author for correspondence: Tamsyn E. Van Rheenen, E-mail: tamsyn.van@unimelb.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

In schizophrenia, relative stability in the magnitude of cognitive deficits across age and illness duration is inconsistent with the evidence of accelerated deterioration in brain regions known to support these functions. These discrepant brain–cognition outcomes may be explained by variability in cognitive reserve (CR), which in neurological disorders has been shown to buffer against brain pathology and minimize its impact on cognitive or clinical indicators of illness.

Methods

Age-related change in fluid reasoning, working memory and frontal brain volume, area and thickness were mapped using regression analysis in 214 individuals with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder and 168 healthy controls. In patients, these changes were modelled as a function of CR.

Results

Patients showed exaggerated age-related decline in brain structure, but not fluid reasoning compared to controls. In the patient group, no moderation of age-related brain structural change by CR was evident. However, age-related cognitive change was moderated by CR, such that only patients with low CR showed evidence of exaggerated fluid reasoning decline that paralleled the exaggerated age-related deterioration of underpinning brain structures seen in all patients.

Conclusions

In schizophrenia-spectrum illness, CR may negate ageing effects on fluid reasoning by buffering against pathologically exaggerated structural brain deterioration through some form of compensation. CR may represent an important modifier that could explain inconsistencies in brain structure – cognition outcomes in the extant literature.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Introduction

Accelerated brain ageing has been implicated in schizophrenia, where an increase in the rate of grey matter loss at certain timepoints throughout the lifespan – and by proxy, the illness course – translates to the pronounced morphological differences seen in patients v. controls (Hulshoff Pol et al., Reference Hulshoff Pol, Schnack, Bertens, van Haren, van der Tweel, Staal, Baaré and Kahn2002; Schnack et al., Reference Schnack, van Haren, Nieuwenhuis, Pol, Cahn and Kahn2016; Cropley et al., Reference Cropley, Klauser, Lenroot, Bruggemann, Sundram, Bousman, Pereira, Di Biase, Weickert, Shannon Weickert, Pantelis and Zalesky2017). Current evidence suggests that while the most extensive brain changes occur in early illness stages (Schnack et al., Reference Schnack, van Haren, Nieuwenhuis, Pol, Cahn and Kahn2016), there is a pattern of exaggerated brain tissue loss, particularly in frontal regions, extending into the sixth decade and corresponding to ~15–20 years post illness onset (Pol and Kahn, Reference Pol and Kahn2008; Cropley et al., Reference Cropley, Klauser, Lenroot, Bruggemann, Sundram, Bousman, Pereira, Di Biase, Weickert, Shannon Weickert, Pantelis and Zalesky2017).

The frontal cortex is highly susceptible to the effects of ageing (Raz and Rodrigue, Reference Raz and Rodrigue2006), and its integrity important for fluid cognitive processes that are vulnerable to age-related change (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Sattler and Lopez2000; Kievit et al., Reference Kievit, Davis, Mitchell, Taylor, Duncan, Tyler, Brayne, Bullmore, Calder and Cusack2014; Harvey and Rosenthal, Reference Harvey and Rosenthal2018). These include reasoning and working memory, which are executive processes that interact to allow for novel problem solving independent of past knowledge or experiences; and can be considered relative to crystallized intelligence which is acquired with experience and intellectual stimulation (e.g. education) and is relatively resistant to age-related decline (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Sattler and Lopez2000; Lindenberger, Reference Lindenberger, Smelser and Baltes2001). Pronounced deficits in fluid cognition is evident in schizophrenia irrespective of age and across all illness stages; and can be so severe that patients as young as 40 have been shown to perform at the level of healthy adults as much as 30 years older (Pantelis et al., Reference Pantelis, Barnes, Nelson, Tanner, Weatherley, Owen and Robbins1997; Loewenstein et al., Reference Loewenstein, Czaja, Bowie and Harvey2012; Harvey and Rosenthal, Reference Harvey and Rosenthal2018). There is some evidence to suggest a greater burden of increasing age on certain executive functions in patients relative to controls (Loewenstein et al., Reference Loewenstein, Czaja, Bowie and Harvey2012). However, generally, studies show a proportionate decline in fluid cognitive performance, such that the relative magnitude of deficits in schizophrenia appears to remain stable over time (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Paulsen, McAdams, Kuck, Zisook, Braff, Harris and Jeste1994; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Gladsjo, Palmer, Kuck, Marcotte and Jeste2001; Harvey and Rosenthal, Reference Harvey and Rosenthal2018). This is inconsistent with the evidence of progressive age-related deterioration in brain regions known to support these functions (Hulshoff Pol et al., Reference Hulshoff Pol, Schnack, Bertens, van Haren, van der Tweel, Staal, Baaré and Kahn2002; Cropley et al., Reference Cropley, Klauser, Lenroot, Bruggemann, Sundram, Bousman, Pereira, Di Biase, Weickert, Shannon Weickert, Pantelis and Zalesky2017). In addition, there appears to be a subgroup of patients with significant brain structural and functional deficits who have normal levels of fluid cognition despite being of an equivalent age to controls or schizophrenia patients with compromised cognition, which further complicates interpretation of pertinent findings (Heinrichs et al., Reference Heinrichs, Pinnock, Parlar, Hawco, Hanford and Hall2017; Lewandowski et al., Reference Lewandowski, McCarthy, Öngür, Norris, Liu, Juelich and Baker2019; Van Rheenen et al., Reference Van Rheenen, Cropley, Wells, Bruggemann, Swaminathan, Sundram, Weinberg, Jacomb, Lenroot, Pereira, Zalesky, Bousman, Shannon Weickert, Weickert and Pantelis2018).

These discrepant brain–cognition outcomes may be partially reconciled by the concept of cognitive reserve (CR), which was proposed to account for individual differences in the cognitive or clinical manifestations of age or illness-related brain pathology (e.g. accelerated brain ageing) (Stern, Reference Stern2002). CR has been studied extensively in the context of neurological illness, where patients with the same disease burden (brain pathology) show marked variability in the expression of disease symptoms as a function of high or low levels of crystallized intelligence (Sumowski et al., Reference Sumowski, Wylie, Chiaravalloti and DeLuca2010a; Stern, Reference Stern2012). Better outcomes are taken to reflect the manifestation of some form of active compensation – possibly involving plastic neural reorganization – by which crystallized intelligence builds CR to enable resilience to brain pathology by minimizing its impact on cognitive or clinical indicators of illness (Stern, Reference Stern2002, Reference Stern2009, Reference Stern2012). This is in contrast to evidence from healthy cohorts showing that greater CR is associated with both better cognition (Opdebeeck et al., Reference Opdebeeck, Martyr and Clare2016) and preserved brain volume (Solé-Padullés et al., Reference Solé-Padullés, Bartrés-Faz, Junqué, Vendrell, Rami, Clemente, Bosch, Villar, Bargalló and Jurado2009; Bartrés-Faz and Arenaza-Urquijo, Reference Bartrés-Faz and Arenaza-Urquijo2011), which suggests a more preventative or neuroprotective effect of CR on neuroanatomy and manifest behaviour in health as opposed to disease.

Proxies of CR include measures of reading or vocabulary knowledge, which are commonly used to estimate premorbid (crystallized) intellectual functioning (Stern, Reference Stern2009; Sumowski et al., Reference Sumowski, Wylie, DeLuca and Chiaravalloti2010b). Schizophrenia studies tend to show lower estimated premorbid intelligence in patients compared to controls (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Pantelis, Carruthers, Speller, Baxendale and Barnes1990). However, the magnitude of this deficit is typically less than that of fluid cognitive domains. Moreover, an overlap in the distribution of scores on direct and proxy measures of premorbid intelligence across patients and controls – as much as 67% (Woodberry et al., Reference Woodberry, Giuliano and Seidman2008) – indicates that a sizeable proportion of those with a schizophrenia diagnosis perform within the range of most healthy individuals (Weickert et al., Reference Weickert, Goldberg, Gold, Bigelow, Egan and Weinberger2000; Woodward and Heckers, Reference Woodward and Heckers2015; Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Lenroot, Jacomb, Allen, Bruggemann, Wells, Balzan, Liu, Galletly, Catts, Shannon Weickert and Weickert2016; Van Rheenen et al., Reference Van Rheenen, Cropley, Wells, Bruggemann, Swaminathan, Sundram, Weinberg, Jacomb, Lenroot, Pereira, Zalesky, Bousman, Shannon Weickert, Weickert and Pantelis2018). Notably, schizophrenia patients with better estimated premorbid intelligence have been found to have less severe symptoms and better clinical and occupational outcomes (Leeson et al., Reference Leeson, Sharma, Harrison, Ron, Barnes and Joyce2011; Wells et al., Reference Wells, Swaminathan, Sundram, Weinberg, Bruggemann, Jacomb, Cropley, Lenroot, Pereira and Zalesky2015). They also have better fluid cognition and more positive generalizability effects of cognitive remediation therapy than those with low premorbid intelligence (Weickert et al., Reference Weickert, Goldberg, Gold, Bigelow, Egan and Weinberger2000; Holthausen et al., Reference Holthausen, Wiersma, Sitskoorn, Hijman, Dingemans, Schene and van den Bosch2002; Fiszdon et al., Reference Fiszdon, Choi, Bryson and Bell2006; Kontis et al., Reference Kontis, Huddy, Reeder, Landau and Wykes2013; Van Rheenen et al., Reference Van Rheenen, Lewandowski, Ongur, Tan, Neill, Gurvich, Pantelis, Malhotra, Rossell and Burdick2017). In the absence of brain imaging data, however, it is not clear whether these better behavioural outcomes reflect a greater capacity to tolerate brain pathology (resilience), or simply less brain pathology itself (neuroprotection) (Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Anstey, Parslow, Maller, Mackinnon and Sachdev2007; Vuoksimaa et al., Reference Vuoksimaa, Panizzon, Chen, Eyler, Fennema-Notestine, Fiecas, Fischl, Franz, Grant, Jak, Lyons, Neale, Thompson, Tsuang, Xian, Dale and Kremen2013).

Certainly, a resilience effect of CR could explain findings of discrepant brain and behaviour change. In this context, only patients with low CR would show pathological age-related cognitive changes that parallel exaggerated age-related changes seen in the brain. In contrast, patients with higher CR would be resilient to these detrimental brain changes, as demonstrated by an absence/attenuation of age-related cognitive decline. In this case, correlations between the brain and behaviour would vary as a function of CR.

Here, we present the first study to examine CR in schizophrenia-spectrum illness using indices of both the brain and behaviour in the context of age. Use of these indices in combination is needed to establish whether CR confers neuroprotective or resilience effects in individuals on the schizophrenia spectrum (Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Anstey, Parslow, Maller, Mackinnon and Sachdev2007). Hence, we used a large, age-diverse, cross-sectional dataset comprising structural neuroimaging and cognitive data, to identify the circumstances whereby fluid cognitive functions in schizophrenia-spectrum illness parallel the putative trajectory of exaggerated age-related deterioration in brain structure. In line with past research, we hypothesized that effects consistent with accelerated ageing would only be evident in analyses of brain structure but not fluid cognition when controls were compared to all patients. However, within the patient group, we expected that the putative rate of age-related cognitive change would be moderated by CR, such that only those patients with low levels of CR would show evidence of exaggerated age-related decline. No moderation of putative age-related brain structural change by CR was predicted for the patient group, as the overall pattern of findings was expected to correspond to a resilience effect of CR (as opposed to neuroprotection) in those with schizophrenia-spectrum illness.

Given the evidence of more pronounced age-related changes in the frontal cortex (Raz and Rodrigue, Reference Raz and Rodrigue2006; Cropley et al., Reference Cropley, Klauser, Lenroot, Bruggemann, Sundram, Bousman, Pereira, Di Biase, Weickert, Shannon Weickert, Pantelis and Zalesky2017), we focused our analyses a-priori on this region and the fluid cognitive functions that it is known to impact. Here, we extend previous work by focusing not only on putative changes in grey matter volume, but also on its morphological drivers – cortical thickness and surface area.

Method

Neuroimaging, cognitive and clinical data from 214 individuals with schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder and 168 controls was obtained from the Australian Schizophrenia Research Bank (ASRB). All participants provided informed consent for the analysis of their stored data. Study procedures were approved by the Melbourne Health Human Research Ethics Committee. The Diagnostic Interview for Psychosis (Castle et al., Reference Castle, Jablensky, McGrath, Carr, Morgan, Watereus, Valuri, Stain, McGuffin and Farmer2006) was used to obtain clinical symptom ratings and confirm patient diagnoses according to ICD-10 or DSM-IV criteria. The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (Andreasen, Reference Andreasen1983) was used to assess negative symptoms. Further details regarding participant characterization are given in the Supplementary material.

Measures

CR was assessed through a composite score of two measures available in the ASRB; the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Vocabulary Test. These measures assess either reading of irregularly pronounced words or the depth and breadth of vocabulary knowledge (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997a; Holdnack, Reference Holdnack2001). Performance on them is considered to be resistant to age or illness-related performance decline in adulthood (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Sattler and Lopez2000), and may even improve with age (Ben-David et al., Reference Ben-David, Erel, Goy and Schneider2015). This was supported in our data by a very small positive correlation between age and the composite measure (r = 0.11, p = 0.03). These measures are associated with crystallized intelligence – which is partially heritable (Plomin and Deary, Reference Plomin and Deary2015), but they are also uniquely predicted by intellectually enriching activities such as education and reading even after controlling for general intellectual functioning (Stine-Morrow et al., Reference Stine-Morrow, Hussey and Ng2015). Raw scores on both measures were standardized and summed, where patients with composite scores below the 10th percentile of the healthy control sample were classified as having below-average CR (low CR group) and those above this considered to have CR within the normal range (average CR group). We elected to classify patients using this method because scores on these tests correlate highly with verbal and full-scale intelligence quotient (IQ) scores, where performance in the 10th percentile or lower corresponds closely to the cut-off between ‘low average’ and ‘borderline’ IQ ranges; the 10th percentile cut-point is a landmark neuropsychological percentile rank frequently used to define the lowest scoring individuals in a sample (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997a; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997b; Crawford and Garthwaite, Reference Crawford and Garthwaite2009; Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Sherman, Iverson, Slick, Strauss, Smelser and Baltes2011; de Zeeuw et al., Reference de Zeeuw, Weusten, van Dijk, van Belle and Durston2012; Woodward and Heckers, Reference Woodward and Heckers2015; Czepielewski et al., Reference Czepielewski, Wang, Gama and Barch2016).

Cognitive tests were selected from those in the ASRB if they met two criteria based on available evidence; performance on the test is known to deteriorate with age across the range of the sample (18–65 years) and is clearly linked to frontal brain functioning. The Letter Number Sequencing Test (LNS) and Matrix Reasoning Test met these criteria (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Sattler and Lopez2000; Barbey et al., Reference Barbey, Colom, Paul and Grafman2014; Kievit et al., Reference Kievit, Davis, Mitchell, Taylor, Duncan, Tyler, Brayne, Bullmore, Calder and Cusack2014)Footnote 1Footnote . The LNS requires participants to verbally reorder a series of numbers and letters according to a specific rule set (e.g. numbers followed by letters). The Matrix Reasoning Test requires that participants complete a visual pattern by selecting the missing pattern piece from an array of possibilities. These tests assess working memory and fluid reasoning and provide prototypical estimates of both verbal and performance-based fluid cognition respectively. Higher scores on both tests indicate better performance. Details are provided elsewhere (Randolph et al., Reference Randolph, Tierney, Mohr and Chase1998; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999).

MRI image acquisition and processing

T1-weighted (MPRAGE) structural scans were acquired using Siemens Avanto 1.5 Tesla scanners. T1-weighted images comprised 176 sagittal slices/brain of 1 mm thickness without gap; field of view = 250 × 250 mm2; repetition time/echo time = 1980/4.3 ms; data matrix size = 256 × 256; voxel dimensions = 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm. The same acquisition sequence was acquired at all ASRB sites. Image processing was conducted using the Freesurfer software package (version 5.1.0, http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/), which consists of a volume-based and a surface-based stream (Dale et al., Reference Dale, Fischl and Sereno1999; Fischl et al., Reference Fischl, Sereno and Dale1999, Reference Fischl, Salat, Busa, Albert, Dieterich, Haselgrove, Van Der Kouwe, Killiany, Kennedy and Klaveness2002; Fischl and Dale, Reference Fischl and Dale2000). The former was used to extract volume estimates (including intracranial volume), while the latter was used to extract cortical thickness and surface area estimates by reconstructing a three-dimensional cortical surface model. This includes segmentation of the pial surface and the grey/white matter boundaries for each hemisphere, using image intensity and continuity information from the MRI volume. Surfaces were initially inspected for skull stripping and surface boundary defects. Inaccuracies in outlining cortical surfaces and brain structures were manually corrected with Freesurfer's editing tools in accordance with an internal, standardized quality control and editing protocol. Edited images were then reprocessed through the Freesurfer pipeline and the output visually inspected again. This process was repeated until all surface errors were corrected, and any images that failed this process were excluded from the analysis. Four trained raters performed the Freesurfer processing and manual correction, blind to participant diagnosis. Inter-rater reliability of the final volume estimates (after correction) was calculated for 34 brain regions from a subset of 20 volumes. The intra-class coefficient was >0.90 for all regions except for the left (0.72) and right (0.59) temporal pole and the left (0.81) and right (0.82) frontal pole.

Thickness measures were obtained by calculating the shortest distance between the grey/white matter boundary and the pial surface at vertices on a uniform triangular grid with 1 mm spacing across the cortex. The surface area was obtained using the shortest distance between vertices on the white surface.

Statistical analysis

Intracranial volume and cortical volume, thickness and surface area estimates for each of the frontal regions delineated by the Desikan–Killiany Atlas (Desikan et al., Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson, Blacker, Buckner, Dale, Maguire and Hyman2006) (online Supplementary Fig. S1) were imported into the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 24. Given that fluid reasoning and working memory index frontal brain systems bilaterally (Petrides et al., Reference Petrides, Alivisatos, Meyer and Evans1993; Prabhakaran et al., Reference Prabhakaran, Smith, Desmond, Glover and Gabrieli1997; Christoff et al., Reference Christoff, Prabhakaran, Dorfman, Zhao, Kroger, Holyoak and Gabrieli2001), the left and right hemispheres for each frontal region were summed to create total volume, thickness or surface area scores. This also served to constrain the number of comparisons required. Global frontal scores were also generated for each imaging measure by summing each region within the frontal cortex bilaterally.

Moderation analyses were implemented using the Preacher and Hayes PROCESS plugin for SPSS. Data were analysed in sequential steps (Supplementary Fig. S1) and modelled linearly given evidence that age-related grey matter change in the frontal cortex is linear (Raz et al., Reference Raz, Lindenberger, Rodrigue, Kennedy, Head, Williamson, Dahle, Gerstorf and Acker2005; Hutton et al., Reference Hutton, Draganski, Ashburner and Weiskopf2009; Giorgio et al., Reference Giorgio, Santelli, Tomassini, Bosnell, Smith, De Stefano and Johansen-Berg2010). Initially, we regressed age, diagnosis and their interaction on each of the cognitive tests of interest, as well as on each of the frontal cortical volume scores (Step 1). In brain regions in which an interaction effect was evident, we further explored whether the effect was driven by differential age-related changes in surface area or thickness by diagnostic group (Step 2). Once the regions of volume, thickness or area showing pathological variation in putative age-related decline in patients v. controls were established, we ascertained whether their association with the cognitive tests of interest differed between patients with low or average CR (Step 3). We did not examine variation by CR in controls given the limited number of cases in the low CR group (n = 17). For brain and cognitive measures whose association in patients was moderated by CR, we tested whether the effect of age on these measures was also moderated by CR (Step 4). Finally, in cases in which age-related change in cognition and/or brain structure differed in those with low v. average CR, the age-related slopes of each patient subgroup were modelled relative to controls (Step 5). Comparison of simple slopes was performed for significant interaction effects. A False Discovery Rate (FDR) of 5% was set to correct for multiple testing. This correction was applied to the interaction effects of each of Steps 1–4 separately (13, 8, 16, 8 tests, respectively) as well as the corresponding post-hoc simple slopes for each group (2 per interaction Step 1–4, 3 per Step 5).

In diagnostic comparisons, gender and site were entered as covariates in the analyses of cognitive tests, while site and intracranial volumeFootnote 2 were covaried in the analyses of brain measures. Intracranial volumeFootnote 3 was included as a covariate alongside site in the within-schizophrenia brain measures analysis a-priori, in order to link our findings to CR independent of brain reserve. Gender did not differ between the patient subgroups and was therefore not controlled in the within-schizophrenia analyses. Age, group (diagnostic or CR) and covariates were always entered into each model at Block 1, while the interaction term was entered at Block 2 to ascertain R 2 change. Standard errors were estimated with the Davidson–McKinnon Heteroskedasticity consistent inference. Five-thousand bootstrap samples were drawn with replacement from the original sample to calculate the 95% bias-corrected (BCa) confidence intervals (Cl) for the unstandardized regression (b) coefficients for each model; the effects were considered statistically significant if the 95% BCa CI did not overlap zero.

Results

Descriptives

There were minimal age differences between patients and controls, but patients had a slightly increased intracranial volume and were overrepresented by males (Table 1a). Patients with average CR were slightly older than those with below-average CR. They also had longer illness durations and less severe negative symptomsFootnote 4. There were no CR subgroup differences in gender distribution, diagnostic categorization, onset age, positive symptoms or medication usage (Table 1b).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

CR, cognitive reserve.

Diagnostic differences in age-related cognitive and brain structural decline (Steps 1 and 2)

Diagnostic differences in age-related cognitive and brain structural decline are shown in Table 2. As expected, no significant age×diagnosis interaction effects were evident for either of the cognitive tests of interest. Significant interactions effects were evident for global frontal, caudal middle frontal, pars orbitalis and pars triangularis volume, such that patients showed greater age-related volume loss in these regions compared to controls (Step 1). Subsequent analyses (Step 2) indicated significant age-related contraction of the cortical area in these regions in patients but not controls, with no significant age×diagnosis interaction effects evident for cortical thickness. Online Supplementary Fig. S2 presents the regions in which there were significant differences in age-related brain structural change in patients relative to controls.

Table 2. Diagnostic differences in age-related cognitive and brain structural change

Dx, diagnosis; HC, healthy control; Sz, schizophrenia.

Note that values for covariates are not displayed for brevity. Covariates, age and Dx were entered at block 1, and the interaction term was entered at block 2. Conditional effects of age on the DV for each group are only reported for those interactions surviving False Discovery Rate (FDR) correction. Confidence intervals for all but the conditional effects of age for each group are bias corrected.

a Controlling for site, gender.

b Controlling for site, intracranial volume.

Moderation of pathological brain morphology on cognition by CR in patients (Step 3)

Of the brain measures showing a pathological age-related change in patients at Step 1 or 2, no main or interaction effects of caudal middle frontal volume or area on Matrix Reasoning or LNS scores were evident, nor were effects of global frontal volume and area on LNS. However, the effect of global frontal volume and area, pars triangularis and pars orbitalis volume and area on Matrix Reasoning scores differed between patients with average and low CR, as did the effect of pars orbitalis and triangularis volume and area on LNS scores (Fig. 1). In patients with low CR, significant brain–cognition relationships of moderate effect were evident, such that lower brain volume or area predicted worse cognitive performance. Those with average CR either showed much weaker, or non-significant relationships (online Supplementary Table S1).

Fig. 1. Correlations between brain volume or surface area and cognitive performance for schizophrenia-spectrum patients with average or below average (low) cognitive reserve (CR). Panel A = fluid reasoning; panel B = working memory. Letter Number Sequencing = LNS. Volume is in mm3, surface area in mm2. Graphs depict cognitive tests for which brain region×CR interactions survived FDR correction.

Moderation of age-related change in cognition and brain structure by CR in patients (Step 4)

No main effects or age×CR interactions were evident for any of the brain measures whose association with cognition was moderated by CR at Step 3. However, age-related change in Matrix Reasoning performance did differ significantly between CR subgroups, with a much sharper age-related decline in performance evident in those with low CR (Table 3; online Supplementary Fig. S3a). While the LNS interaction term only trended towards significance (p = 0.08 uncorrected), post-hoc conditional effects analysis (produced automatically in PROCESS) showed age-related decline in the performance in only the patients with low CR (online Supplementary Table S2 and online Supplementary Fig. S3b). Footnote 5

Table 3. Moderation of age-related change in cognition fluid reasoning by CR in schizophrenia-spectrum patients

CR, cognitive reserve; HC, healthy controls.

Note that values for covariates are not displayed for brevity. Confidence intervals for all but the conditional effects of age for each group are bias corrected.

aControlling for site.

Diagnostic differences in age-related cognitive decline as a function of CR subgroup (Step 5)

Figure 2 shows age-related cognitive decline in Matrix Reasoning performance in controls and patients with either low or average CR. Relative to controls, a significant exaggeration of age-related change in Matrix Reasoning scores was evident for only the patients with low CR (online Supplementary Table S3a). CR subgroup – control differences are not reported for the LNS given the interaction term only trended towards significance.

Fig. 2. Age-related decline in fluid reasoning in schizophrenia-spectrum (Sz) subgroups with low or average cognitive reserve (CR) v. healthy controls (HC). Age is reported in years.

Discussion

We aimed to reconcile the inconsistencies regarding brain–cognition relationships in a large sample of schizophrenia-spectrum patients and healthy controls. Consistent with the accelerated brain ageing hypothesis of schizophrenia (Harvey and Rosenthal, Reference Harvey and Rosenthal2018; Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Eyler and Jeste2018), our results showed greater frontal cortex volume reductions in patients with increasing age, particularly in lateral middle and rostral segments of the inferior frontal gyrus. This pattern was reminiscent of a declining structural brain trajectory, did not vary as a function of CR, and was largely explained by contraction of the cortical surface with age.

As predicted, an absence of age-related changes in fluid reasoning and working memory were inconsistent with these results. While this superficially suggests a lack of direct association between brain structure and cognition, further analysis revealed that this was only the case for those characterized by CR in a range equivalent to most controls. Patients with below-average CR, however, showed significant and/or stronger negative relationships between these cognitive functions and frontal brain structure, likely owing to more pronounced putative age-related decline in performance than for patients with average CR. Indeed, only patients with low CR showed putative age-related fluid reasoning decline that mirrored the pervasive age-related frontal volume and surface area changes evident in all patients both globally and regionally in the ventral inferior frontal gyrus. Thus, the burden of frontal brain pathology on fluid cognition varied as a function of CR.

This is the first study to integrate measures of both cognition and brain imaging in the context of age, to explicitly determine whether patients with below-average CR are less cognitively resilient to pathological brain change. Although existing studies explicitly focussed on CR in schizophrenia-spectrum samples have shown that patients with higher CR have better behavioural outcomes (Holthausen et al., Reference Holthausen, Wiersma, Sitskoorn, Hijman, Dingemans, Schene and van den Bosch2002; Leeson et al., Reference Leeson, Sharma, Harrison, Ron, Barnes and Joyce2011; Wells et al., Reference Wells, Swaminathan, Sundram, Weinberg, Bruggemann, Jacomb, Cropley, Lenroot, Pereira and Zalesky2015), the mechanism by which this occurs remained unknown in the absence of concurrent analysis of brain pathology or age-related change. That is, in past studies it was not clear whether more positive patient outcomes in those with higher CR reflected (1) a neuroprotective effect on both the brain and behaviour regardless of ones point in the lifespan/illness course, where a larger gap needed to be crossed to reach the threshold of significant impairment relative to those with lower CR, or (2) manifestation of a greater tolerance of age/illness-related pathology of the brain than those with lower CR. Our findings are supportive of the latter, where schizophrenia-spectrum patients showed an equivalent level of brain pathology irrespective of CR, but their cognitive outcomes varied by CR in the context of putative age-related decline. These findings are consistent with the effects of CR seen in neurological illnesses such as multiple sclerosis, where CR appears to protect against cognitive decline that is secondary to illness effects rather than confer gains to cognition itself (Sumowski et al., Reference Sumowski, Chiaravalloti and DeLuca2009; Sumowski et al., Reference Sumowski, Wylie, DeLuca and Chiaravalloti2010b).

Relevantly, despite the average CR patient subgroup being older and having been exposed to the deleterious effects of the illness for longer, they exhibited less age-related cognitive deficits and less severe negative symptoms than the low CR patients. This further supports our hypothesis of a resilience effect of CR. Crucially, these findings shed light on seemingly discrepant results in past schizophrenia research showing a pathological change in the brain, but not cognition, as a function of age and illness progression. They also point to CR as an important modifier that could explain the inconsistent brain structure – cognition correlations that are seen across schizophrenia studies (Karantonis et al., Reference Karantonis, Hughes, Rossell, Wannan, Pantelis, Cropley and RheenenIn preparation).

Our finding suggesting an absence of exaggerated frontal thickness reductions alongside exaggerated age-related frontal volume reductions in the whole patient group is also of interest, particularly in the context of marked frontal areal contraction with age that was entirely absent in healthy controls. This is contrary to the work in healthy individuals showing that exaggerated age-related volume loss of frontal regions is explained by cortical thinning, while age-related surface area changes in these regions are minimal (Lemaitre et al., Reference Lemaitre, Goldman, Sambataro, Verchinski, Meyer-Lindenberg, Weinberger and Mattay2012; Storsve et al., Reference Storsve, Fjell, Tamnes, Westlye, Overbye, Aasland and Walhovd2014). In our data, the main effect of surface area was absent while a pattern of increased surface area in younger patients and decreased surface area in older patients was present (online Supplementary Fig. S1). This suggests that absolute diagnostic differences in surface area are age-dependent in schizophrenia-spectrum illness and that the trajectory of surface area is highly relevant to its neuroanatomical and cognitive characterization.

Our findings should be considered in the context of the strengths of the study, which include the large sample of individuals diagnosed with a schizophrenia-spectrum illness with both cognitive and neuroimaging data; and the multi-site nature of the sample that speaks to geographic generalizability. Several limitations should also be considered, including the use of a cross-sectional experimental design to infer age-related change. Thus, it is possible that these findings may be partially attributable to the factors including cohort effects, or psychotropic medication use in the case of the schizophrenia-control comparisons. While longitudinal experimental designs are undoubtedly preferable in the exploration of this research question, they are also economically unfeasible and impractical owing to high attrition rates in psychiatric samples. In order to explore our hypotheses, the benefits of a large cross-sectional sample spanning key periods of adulthood was weighted against this and considered in the context of evidence showing that cross-sectional trends provide reliable estimates for longitudinally assessed age-related change within the frontal cortex specifically (Raz et al., Reference Raz, Lindenberger, Rodrigue, Kennedy, Head, Williamson, Dahle, Gerstorf and Acker2005; Raz and Lindenberger, Reference Raz and Lindenberger2011).

Other limitations include (1) the use of bilateral composite brain measures, such that CR moderation effects of left or right frontal regions were not explored. While this was done for conceptual and statistical reasons, it is possible that different effects for each hemisphere exist; (2) the use of different medications in the sample. The absence of distribution differences in the percentage of patients using different medication classes between CR subgroups suggests that medication may not have a key role in our findings; however, no dosing information was available which impeded our ability to clearly tease apart medication effects; (3) restriction of fluid cognition measures to the only two tests available in the ASRB that met our criteria, making it unclear whether different effects occur with other fluid tests sensitive to age-related decline (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Sattler and Lopez2000); (4) use of data collected on a 1.5 Tesla MR scanner, which may have affected the signal to noise ratio and subsequent analysis outcomes; and (5) analysis of CR effects in only the schizophrenia-spectrum diagnosed individuals, leaving questions open about whether different CR effects would be evident in patients v. controls. Finally, CR is a broad construct that was operationalized by a composite proxy measure of crystallized intellectual functioning in this study. While this approach is justified and well-recognized in the literature, it is possible that different moderation effects may be seen with other proxy measures of CR that were not considered here, such as education or occupational functioning. Future research will do well to build on our work using several indices of CR and by following participants over the lifespan.

In sum, our findings indicate that associations between fluid cognition and brain volume and area are moderated by CR in schizophrenia-spectrum illness. As CR does not moderate pathological age-related increases in the magnitude of structural brain abnormalities as it does age-related increases in fluid reasoning deficits, it appears to confer resilience to the latter by negating the influence of the former through some form of compensation. While not tested in these data, it is possible that this compensation involves adaptive engagement of alternative neural regions and/or networks to maintain fluid cognitive performance when the usual structural neural resources are deteriorated (Stern, Reference Stern2009).

Our findings thus suggest that CR, as proxied by crystallized intelligence, is a key factor in explaining individual differences in ageing effects on fluid reasoning in schizophrenia-spectrum illness. While genetic and neurodevelopmental influences on schizophrenia may affect the accumulation of CR in terms of such intelligence (Barnett et al., Reference Barnett, Salmond, Jones and Sahakian2006), evidence also shows that intellectual enrichment through education and early life reading engagement can boost later intelligence even after controlling for underlying genetic influences (Ramsden et al., Reference Ramsden, Richardson, Josse, Shakeshaft, Seghier and Price2013; Ritchie et al., Reference Ritchie, Bates and Plomin2015; Ritchie and Tucker-Drob, Reference Ritchie and Tucker-Drob2018). Thus, CR may represent a clinically important target that is amenable to change.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719001417.

Author ORCIDs

Tamsyn E. Van Rheenen, 0000-0003-3339-6665.

Financial support

Dr Van Rheenen was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Early Career Fellowship (1088785). Dr Cropley was supported by a Brain and Behavior Research Foundation (NARSAD) Young Investigator Award (21660), and a University of Melbourne Faculty of Medicine, Dentistry, and Health Sciences Research Fellowship. Dr Zalesky was supported by an NHMRC Fellowship (1047648). Dr Bousman was supported by an NHMRC Career Development Fellowship (1127700). Prof Shannon Weickert is funded by the NSW Ministry of Health, Office of Health and Medical Research. CSW is a recipient of a National Health and Medical Research Council (Australia) Principal Research Fellowship (PRF) (#1117079). Professor Pantelis was supported by an NHMRC Senior Principal Research Fellowship (628386 and 1105825) and by a NARSAD Distinguished Investigator Award. Data for this study were provided by the ASRB, which is supported by the NHMRC (enabling grant 386500), the Pratt Foundation, Ramsay Health Care, the Viertel Charitable Foundation and the Schizophrenia Research Institute. The authors thank the chief investigators and manager of the ASRB: Carr V, Schall U, Scott R, Jablensky A, Mowry B, Michie P, Catts S, Henskens F, Pantelis C and Loughland C. None of the funding sources played any role in the study design; in the collection, analysis or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication

Conflict of interest

Dr Van Rheenen has received grant funding (unrelated to the current paper) from Club Melbourne, the Henry Freeman Trust, Jack Brockhoff Foundation, University of Melbourne, Barbara Dicker Brain Sciences Foundation, Rebecca L Cooper Foundation and the Society of Mental Health Research. Professor Sundram has received consulting fees, advisory board fees, research support, speakers honoraria or travel support from AstraZeneca, the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council, the Australian Department of Immigration and Border Protection, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, the Flack Trust, GlaxoSmithKline, Lundbeck, the One-in-Five Association, Otsuka, Pfizer, Roche and the United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. Cynthia Shannon Weickert is on an advisory board for Lundbeck, Australia Pty Ltd and in collaboration with Astellas Pharma Inc., Japan. Over the last 4 years, Professor Pantelis has been on advisory boards for AstraZeneca, Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck and Servier; and he has received honoraria for talks presented at educational meetings organized by AstraZeneca, Eli Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck, Pfizer and Shire. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Footnotes

1 Available ASRB cognitive data included the Repeatable Battery for Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS), LNS, Matrix Reasoning Test and Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) (Loughland et al., Reference Loughland, Draganic, McCabe, Richards, Nasir, Allen, Catts, Jablensky, Henskens and Michie2010). Note that although the COWAT is an executive measure associated with frontal brain functioning, evidence suggests that age-related performance decline on this measure is evident in late life, at ages beyond those captured in the ASRB (Rodríguez-Aranda and Martinussen, Reference Rodríguez-Aranda and Martinussen2006). Thus, it was not selected as a measure of interest in the current study.

2 To avoid overcorrecting, gender was not used as a covariate for brain structure analyses since it was highly correlated with intracranial volume.

3 Brain reserve and CR are not consistently related in schizophrenia and may not be synonymous (Van Rheenen et al., Reference Van Rheenen, Cropley, Wells, Bruggemann, Swaminathan, Sundram, Weinberg, Jacomb, Lenroot, Pereira, Zalesky, Bousman, Shannon Weickert, Weickert and Pantelis2018), hence we aimed to remove the effects of the former given our focus on the latter.

4 Subsequent within schizophrenia-spectrum subgroup analyses were conducted with and without negative symptoms as a covariate. As findings remained unchanged, for brevity the results without negative symptoms as a covariate are presented.

5 As a secondary check of the significant findings, we re-analysed the data using the CR variable as a continuous measure. The general pattern of interaction effects was the same, where significant and/or stronger relationships between brain measures and the cognitive tests; and between age and the cognitive measures were evident when CR was at 1SD below the mean, and sometimes at the mean, v. at 1SD above the mean. Similar to the dichotomous variable analysis, the relationship between age and the brain measures did not differ by CR. Given the similarity in the interaction effects across the two methods, for brevity these findings are not reported, although examples of the outcomes of some analyses are presented in online Supplementary Fig. S4 for demonstrative purposes.

The notes appear after the main text.

References

Andreasen, NA (1983) Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. Iowa City, Iowa: University of Iowa.Google Scholar
Barbey, AK, Colom, R, Paul, EJ and Grafman, J (2014) Architecture of fluid intelligence and working memory revealed by lesion mapping. Brain Structure and Function 219, 485494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, J, Salmond, C, Jones, P and Sahakian, B (2006) Cognitive reserve in neuropsychiatry. Psychological Medicine 36, 10531064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartrés-Faz, D and Arenaza-Urquijo, EM (2011) Structural and functional imaging correlates of cognitive and brain reserve hypotheses in healthy and pathological aging. Brain Topography 24, 340357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-David, BM, Erel, H, Goy, H and Schneider, BA (2015) ‘Older is always better’: age-related differences in vocabulary scores across 16 years. Psychology and Aging 30, 856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, BL, Sherman, EM, Iverson, GL, Slick, DJ and Strauss, E (2011) Psychometric foundations for the interpretation of neuropsychological test results. In Smelser, NJ and Baltes, PB (eds), The Little Black Book of Neuropsychology. New York: Springer, pp. 893922.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castle, DJ, Jablensky, A, McGrath, JJ, Carr, V, Morgan, V, Watereus, A, Valuri, G, Stain, H, McGuffin, P and Farmer, A (2006) The diagnostic interview for psychoses (DIP): development, reliability and applications. Psychological Medicine 36, 6980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, H, Anstey, KJ, Parslow, RA, Maller, J, Mackinnon, A and Sachdev, P (2007) The brain reserve hypothesis, brain atrophy and aging. Gerontology 53, 8295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christoff, K, Prabhakaran, V, Dorfman, J, Zhao, Z, Kroger, JK, Holyoak, KJ and Gabrieli, JD (2001) Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in relational integration during reasoning. Neuroimage 14, 11361149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, JR and Garthwaite, PH (2009) Percentiles please: the case for expressing neuropsychological test scores and accompanying confidence limits as percentile ranks. The Clinical Neuropsychologist 23, 193204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cropley, VL, Klauser, P, Lenroot, R, Bruggemann, J, Sundram, S, Bousman, C, Pereira, A, Di Biase, M, Weickert, TW, Shannon Weickert, C, Pantelis, C and Zalesky, A (2017) Accelerated gray and white matter deterioration with age in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry 174, 286295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Czepielewski, LS, Wang, L, Gama, CS and Barch, DM (2016) The relationship of intellectual functioning and cognitive performance to brain structure in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 43, 355364.Google Scholar
Dale, AM, Fischl, B and Sereno, MI (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9, 179194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Zeeuw, P, Weusten, J, van Dijk, S, van Belle, J and Durston, S (2012) Deficits in cognitive control, timing and reward sensitivity appear to be dissociable in ADHD. PLoS ONE 7, e51416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desikan, RS, Ségonne, F, Fischl, B, Quinn, BT, Dickerson, BC, Blacker, D, Buckner, RL, Dale, AM, Maguire, RP and Hyman, BT (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31, 968980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B and Dale, AM (2000) Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97, 1105011055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B, Sereno, MI and Dale, AM (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis: II: inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9, 195207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischl, B, Salat, DH, Busa, E, Albert, M, Dieterich, M, Haselgrove, C, Van Der Kouwe, A, Killiany, R, Kennedy, D and Klaveness, S (2002) Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiszdon, J, Choi, J, Bryson, G and Bell, M (2006) Impact of intellectual status on response to cognitive task training in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 87, 261269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giorgio, A, Santelli, L, Tomassini, V, Bosnell, R, Smith, S, De Stefano, N and Johansen-Berg, H (2010) Age-related changes in grey and white matter structure throughout adulthood. NeuroImage 51, 943951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, PD and Rosenthal, JB (2018) Cognitive and functional deficits in people with schizophrenia: evidence for accelerated or exaggerated aging? Schizophrenia Research 196, 1421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R, Paulsen, JS, McAdams, LA, Kuck, J, Zisook, S, Braff, D, Harris, MJ and Jeste, DV (1994) Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenics: relationship to age, chronicity, and dementia. Archives of General Psychiatry 51, 469476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, RK, Gladsjo, JA, Palmer, BW, Kuck, J, Marcotte, TD and Jeste, DV (2001) Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 2432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinrichs, RW, Pinnock, F, Parlar, M, Hawco, C, Hanford, L and Hall, GB (2017) Cortical thinning in network-associated regions in cognitively normal and below-normal range schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research and Treatment 2017, 9760905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holdnack, JA (2001) Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Holthausen, EAE, Wiersma, D, Sitskoorn, MM, Hijman, R, Dingemans, PM, Schene, AH and van den Bosch, RJ (2002) Schizophrenic patients without neuropsychological deficits: subgroup, disease severity or cognitive compensation? Psychiatry Research 112, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulshoff Pol, HE, Schnack, HG, Bertens, MG, van Haren, NE, van der Tweel, I, Staal, WG, Baaré, WF and Kahn, RS (2002) Volume changes in gray matter in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 244250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutton, C, Draganski, B, Ashburner, J and Weiskopf, N (2009) A comparison between voxel-based cortical thickness and voxel-based morphometry in normal aging. NeuroImage 48, 371380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karantonis, J, Hughes, M, Rossell, SL, Wannan, C, Pantelis, C, Cropley, V and Rheenen, TEV (In preparation) A review of brain morphology-cognition relationships on the schizophrenia-bipolar disorder spectrum.Google Scholar
Kievit, RA, Davis, SW, Mitchell, DJ, Taylor, JR, Duncan, J, Tyler, LK, Brayne, C, Bullmore, E, Calder, A and Cusack, R (2014) Distinct aspects of frontal lobe structure mediate age-related differences in fluid intelligence and multitasking. Nature Communications 5, 5658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kontis, D, Huddy, V, Reeder, C, Landau, S and Wykes, T (2013) Effects of age and cognitive reserve on cognitive remediation therapy outcome in patients with schizophrenia. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 21, 218230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leeson, VC, Sharma, P, Harrison, M, Ron, MA, Barnes, TRE and Joyce, EM (2011) IQ trajectory, cognitive reserve, and clinical outcome following a first episode of psychosis: a 3-year longitudinal study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 37, 768777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemaitre, H, Goldman, AL, Sambataro, F, Verchinski, BA, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Weinberger, DR and Mattay, VS (2012) Normal age-related brain morphometric changes: nonuniformity across cortical thickness, surface area and gray matter volume? Neurobiology of Aging 33, 617, e1-617. e9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewandowski, KE, McCarthy, JM, Öngür, D, Norris, LA, Liu, GZ, Juelich, RJ and Baker, JT (2019) Functional connectivity in distinct cognitive subtypes in psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 204, 120126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindenberger, U (2001) Lifespan theories of cognitive development. In Smelser, NJ and Baltes, PB (eds), International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. Elsevier Science, pp. 88488854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, DA, Czaja, SJ, Bowie, CR and Harvey, PD (2012) Age-associated differences in cognitive performance in older patients with schizophrenia: a comparison with healthy older adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 20, 2940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loughland, C, Draganic, D, McCabe, K, Richards, J, Nasir, A, Allen, J, Catts, S, Jablensky, A, Henskens, F and Michie, P (2010) Australian Schizophrenia Research Bank: a database of comprehensive clinical, endophenotypic and genetic data for aetiological studies of schizophrenia. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 44, 10291035.Google Scholar
Nelson, HE, Pantelis, C, Carruthers, K, Speller, J, Baxendale, S and Barnes, TR (1990) Cognitive functioning and symptomatology in chronic schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 20, 357365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, TT, Eyler, LT and Jeste, DV (2018) Systemic biomarkers of accelerated aging in schizophrenia: a critical review and future directions. Schizophrenia Bulletin 44, 398408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opdebeeck, C, Martyr, A and Clare, L (2016) Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition 23, 4060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pantelis, C, Barnes, T, Nelson, HE, Tanner, S, Weatherley, L, Owen, AM and Robbins, TW (1997) Frontal-striatal cognitive deficits in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Brain: A Journal of Neurology 120, 18231843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrides, M, Alivisatos, B, Meyer, E and Evans, AC (1993) Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 90, 878882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R and Deary, IJ (2015) Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Molecular Psychiatry 20, 98108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pol, HEH and Kahn, RS (2008) What happens after the first episode? A review of progressive brain changes in chronically ill patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 34, 354366.Google Scholar
Prabhakaran, V, Smith, JA, Desmond, JE, Glover, GH and Gabrieli, JD (1997) Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven's Progressive Matrices Test. Cognitive Psychology 33, 4363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsden, S, Richardson, FM, Josse, G, Shakeshaft, C, Seghier, ML and Price, CJ (2013) The influence of reading ability on subsequent changes in verbal IQ in the teenage years. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 6, 3039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Randolph, C, Tierney, MC, Mohr, E and Chase, TN (1998) The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS): preliminary clinical validity. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 20, 310319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, N and Rodrigue, KM (2006) Differential aging of the brain: patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 30, 730748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raz, N and Lindenberger, U (2011) Only time will tell: cross-sectional studies offer no solution to the age–brain–cognition triangle: Comment on Salthouse (2011). Psychological Bulletin 137, 790795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raz, N, Lindenberger, U, Rodrigue, KM, Kennedy, KM, Head, D, Williamson, A, Dahle, C, Gerstorf, D and Acker, JD (2005) Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cerebral Cortex 15, 16761689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchie, SJ and Tucker-Drob, EM (2018) How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychological Science 29, 13581369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchie, SJ, Bates, TC and Plomin, R (2015) Does learning to read improve intelligence? A longitudinal multivariate analysis in identical twins from age 7 to 16. Child Development 86, 2336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez-Aranda, C and Martinussen, M (2006) Age-related differences in performance of phonemic verbal fluency measured by Controlled Oral Word Association Task (COWAT): a meta-analytic study. Developmental Neuropsychology 30, 697717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, JJ, Sattler, JM and Lopez, SJ (2000) Age effects on Wechsler adult intelligence scale-III subtests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 15, 311317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnack, HG, van Haren, NE, Nieuwenhuis, M, Pol, HEH, Cahn, W and Kahn, RS (2016) Accelerated brain aging in schizophrenia: a longitudinal pattern recognition study. American Journal of Psychiatry 173, 607616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solé-Padullés, C, Bartrés-Faz, D, Junqué, C, Vendrell, P, Rami, L, Clemente, IC, Bosch, B, Villar, A, Bargalló, N and Jurado, MA (2009) Brain structure and function related to cognitive reserve variables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiology of Aging 30, 11141124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y (2002) What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 8, 448460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y (2009) Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47, 20152028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y (2012) Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. The Lancet Neurology 11, 10061012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stine-Morrow, EA, Hussey, EK and Ng, S (2015) The potential for literacy to shape lifelong cognitive health. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2, 92100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storsve, AB, Fjell, AM, Tamnes, CK, Westlye, LT, Overbye, K, Aasland, HW and Walhovd, KB (2014) Differential longitudinal changes in cortical thickness, surface area and volume across the adult life span: regions of accelerating and decelerating change. The Journal of Neuroscience 34, 84888498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sumowski, JF, Chiaravalloti, N and DeLuca, J (2009) Cognitive reserve protects against cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 31, 913926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sumowski, JF, Wylie, GR, Chiaravalloti, N and DeLuca, J (2010 a) Intellectual enrichment lessens the effect of brain atrophy on learning and memory in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 74, 19421945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sumowski, JF, Wylie, GR, DeLuca, J and Chiaravalloti, N (2010 b) Intellectual enrichment is linked to cerebral efficiency in multiple sclerosis: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for cognitive reserve. Brain 133, 362374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Rheenen, TE, Lewandowski, KE, Ongur, D, Tan, EJ, Neill, E, Gurvich, C, Pantelis, C, Malhotra, A, Rossell, SL and Burdick, KE (2017) Characterizing cognitive heterogeneity on the schizophrenia – bipolar disorder spectrum. Psychological Medicine 47, 18481864.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Rheenen, TE, Cropley, V, Wells, R, Bruggemann, J, Swaminathan, V, Sundram, S, Weinberg, W, Jacomb, I, Lenroot, R, Pereira, AM, Zalesky, A, Bousman, C, Shannon Weickert, C, Weickert, TW and Pantelis, C (2018) Widespread volumetric reductions in schizophrenia and schizoaffective patients displaying compromised cognitive abilities. Schizophrenia Bulletin 44, 560574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuoksimaa, E, Panizzon, MS, Chen, C-H, Eyler, LT, Fennema-Notestine, C, Fiecas, M, Fischl, B, Franz, CE, Grant, MD, Jak, A, Lyons, MJ, Neale, MC, Thompson, WK, Tsuang, MT, Xian, H, Dale, AM and Kremen, WS (2013) Cognitive reserve moderates the association between hippocampal volume and episodic memory in middle Age. Neuropsychologia 51, 11241131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D (1997 a) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised: Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1997 b) Wechsler Memory Scale. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1999) Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weickert, TW, Goldberg, TE, Gold, JM, Bigelow, LB, Egan, MF and Weinberger, DR (2000) Cognitive impairments in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 907913.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinberg, D, Lenroot, R, Jacomb, I, Allen, K, Bruggemann, J, Wells, R, Balzan, R, Liu, D, Galletly, C, Catts, S, Shannon Weickert, C and Weickert, T (2016) Cognitive subtypes of schizophrenia characterized by differential brain volumetric reductions and cognitive decline. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 12511259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, R, Swaminathan, V, Sundram, S, Weinberg, D, Bruggemann, J, Jacomb, I, Cropley, V, Lenroot, R, Pereira, AM and Zalesky, A (2015) The impact of premorbid and current intellect in schizophrenia: cognitive, symptom, and functional outcomes. npj Schizophrenia 1, 15043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodberry, KA, Giuliano, AJ and Seidman, LJ (2008) Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 579587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, ND and Heckers, S (2015) Brain structure in neuropsychologically defined subgroups of schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin 41, 13491359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Diagnostic differences in age-related cognitive and brain structural change

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Correlations between brain volume or surface area and cognitive performance for schizophrenia-spectrum patients with average or below average (low) cognitive reserve (CR). Panel A = fluid reasoning; panel B = working memory. Letter Number Sequencing = LNS. Volume is in mm3, surface area in mm2. Graphs depict cognitive tests for which brain region×CR interactions survived FDR correction.

Figure 3

Table 3. Moderation of age-related change in cognition fluid reasoning by CR in schizophrenia-spectrum patients

Figure 4

Fig. 2. Age-related decline in fluid reasoning in schizophrenia-spectrum (Sz) subgroups with low or average cognitive reserve (CR) v. healthy controls (HC). Age is reported in years.

Supplementary material: File

Van Rheenen et al. supplementary material

Van Rheenen et al. supplementary material 1

Download Van Rheenen et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.9 MB
Supplementary material: File

Van Rheenen et al. supplementary material

Van Rheenen et al. supplementary material 2

Download Van Rheenen et al.  supplementary material(File)
File 1.9 MB