Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-686fd747b7-hzbls Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-13T12:09:45.979Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 5 - The Role of Parental Emotion Regulation in Parental Neglect and Violence

from Part II - Influence of Parents’ Emotion Regulation on Parenting

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2024

Isabelle Roskam
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
James J. Gross
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Moïra Mikolajczak
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium

Summary

This chapter aims to explore the emotional processes associated with parenting stress and competence, as specific risk factors in child maltreatment. We consider two key aspects of the relationship between emotion regulation (ER) and child maltreatment: (1) ER as an important antecedent of child maltreatment and (2) violent parenting behaviors as an ER strategy. The theoretical framework of the social information processing model guides our treatment of the subject. Gender differences and clinical implications are discussed.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

5.1 Parenting Stress: The Key Concept between Emotion Regulation and Child Maltreatment

In the literature, parenting stress encompasses the psychological/emotional reactions arising from attempts to adapt to the demands of parenthood (Deater-Deckard, Reference Deater-Deckard1998, Reference Deater-Deckard2004) and is associated with parenting outcomes and the quality of dyadic parent–child interactions (Camisasca et al., Reference Camisasca, Miragoli and Di Blasio2014; Crnic & Low, Reference Crnic, Low and Boernstein2002; Miragoli et al., Reference Miragoli, Balzarotti, Camisasca and Di Blasio2018). Specifically, parenting stress develops from parents’ evaluations that the demands of the parenting role are currently exceeding their coping abilities. It has been theorized as a multifactor process, which includes both individual and parenting-related sources of distress, ranging from objective life events (e.g. the death of a family member) to the parent’s evaluation of the child’s behavior and to the parent’s subjective feeling of failing in their own parental responsibility (Abidin, Reference Abidin1992, Reference Abidin1995; Deater-Deckard, Reference Deater-Deckard1998). High levels of parenting stress interfere with the caregiver’s ability to effectively cope with parenting-related difficulties (Jackson & Huang, Reference Jackson and Huang2000; Scheel & Rieckmann, Reference Scheel and Rieckmann1998), increasing negative emotions and interactions with children (Coyl et al., Reference Coyl, Roggman and Newland2002), and the use of ineffective disciplinary strategies (Cain & Combs-Orme, Reference Cain and Combs-Orme2005; Crouch & Behl, Reference Crouch and Behl2001). In the field of child maltreatment, several studies have documented that maltreating parents report significantly higher levels of parenting stress and negative affect than non-maltreating parents and that parenting stress is related to increased child maltreatment (e,g., Ethier et al., Reference Ethier, Lacharite and Couture1995; Krahé et al., Reference Krahé, Bondü, Höse and Esser2015; McPherson et al., Reference McPherson, Lewis, Lynn, Haskett and Behrend2009; Whipple & Webster-Stratton, Reference Whipple and Webster-Stratton1991).

In the social information processing (SIP) model (Milner, Reference Milner1993, Reference Milner and Hansen2000, Reference Milner2003), child maltreatment is regarded exclusively as an extreme consequence of parenting problems, resulting from bad cognitive processing of social information and from high levels of emotional distress (Azar, Reference Azar1998, Reference Azar and Bornstein2002; Bugental et al., Reference Bugental, Ellerson, Lin, Rainey, Kokotovic and O’Hara2002). The SIP model (Milner, Reference Milner1993, Reference Milner and Hansen2000, Reference Milner2003) assumes that parenting behaviors are marked by three cognitive processing stages (perceptions of the child’s behavior; interpretations and evaluations of the child’s behavior; and information integration and disciplinary response selection) and a fourth cognitive/behavioral stage consisting of the monitoring process and response implementation. These different processes are theory driven (based on preexisting cognitive schemata and beliefs about children and child-rearing, expectations concerning the child and concerning themselves as parents, including emotional components originating from emotions experienced during previous events involving attachment and parenting) and context driven (affected by situational factors, such as level of stress).

According to this cognitive-behavioral approach, maltreating (or at-risk) parents fail at several steps of the parenting process, leading to a negative characterization of their relationship with the child and increasing levels of stress (Dadds et al., Reference Dadds, Mullens, McAllister and Atkinson2003; Francis & Wolfe, Reference Francis and Wolfe2008; Montes et al., Reference Montes, de Paul and Milner2001). Specifically, they fail to objectively interpret the child’s behavior, which is viewed as more problematic than it actually is (Stage 1 of SIP model; e.g. Crouch et al., Reference Crouch, Skowronski, Milner and Harris2008; Lau et al., Reference Lau, Valeri, McCarty and Weisz2006; Miragoli et al., Reference Miragoli, Balzarotti, Camisasca and Di Blasio2018). Moreover, several studies have linked these negative views of children’s behavior to parental attributions of child-related stable, hostile, and provocative intent, due to automatic accessibility of developmental expectancy biases (Stage 2; e.g. Farc et al., Reference Farc, Crouch, Skowronski and Milner2008; Haskett et al., Reference Haskett, Scott, Grant, Ward and Robinson2003; Mammen et al., Reference Mammen, Kolko and Pilkonis2002). Finally, the use of harsh parenting behaviors, including acts of physical maltreatment (Timmer et al., Reference Timmer, Borrego and Urquiza2002), arises from some marked difficulties with assuming the child’s perspective, which interfere with a parent’s processing of mitigating information in discipline situations (Stage 3; e.g. de Paúl et al., Reference de Paúl, Asla, Pérez-Albéniz and De Cádiz2006; McElroy & Rodriguez, Reference McElroy and Rodriguez2008; Perez-Albeniz & de Paúl, Reference Perez-Albeniz and de Paúl2003). For these reasons, in daily interactions, maltreating (or at-risk) parents experience a lack of efficacy in their own parental skills (in terms of self-efficacy) and their parent–child relationship is impaired by negative mental representations (expectations about their parenting and about their children’s abilities and intent) and feelings of frustration and dissatisfaction, with high levels of parenting stress and emotional reactivity (in terms of sadness, anger, hostility, and fear; Berryhill, Reference Berryhill2016; Crnic & Ross, Reference Crnic, Ross, Deater-Deckard and Panneton2017).

Parenting stress and reactivity to negative emotions are further amplified by the embedded emotional components of the preexisting cognitive schemata (mainly due to previous childhood experiences; Lavi et al., Reference Lavi, Ozer, Katz and Gross2021; Milner, Reference Milner2003), which severely hinders parents’ ability to attend to the child’s needs and increases the risk of child maltreatment (Milner, Reference Milner1993, Reference Milner and Hansen2000, Reference Milner2003). Parents who maltreat their children, or are at risk of doing so, have more inaccurate and biased cognitive preexisting schemata than other parents, involving beliefs and values that influence the way they perceive, evaluate, integrate, and respond to information related to children (e.g. Dadds et al., Reference Dadds, Mullens, McAllister and Atkinson2003; Francis & Wolfe, Reference Francis and Wolfe2008; Montes et al., Reference Montes, de Paul and Milner2001). In addition to ideational components, preexisting schemata include affective/emotional components (e.g. sadness, anger, hostility, anxiety, etc.) that were experienced during previous relational events and that influence how new information is perceived and processed. Maltreating (or at-risk) parents are more likely to use preexisting cognitive schemata if they are experiencing negative affect and/or high levels of emotional distress connected to parenting practice (e.g. Crouch et al., Reference Crouch, Risser, Skowronski, Milner, Farc and Irwin2010; Dadds et al., Reference Dadds, Mullens, McAllister and Atkinson2003; de Paúl et al., Reference de Paúl, Asla, Pérez-Albéniz and De Cádiz2006; Haskett et al., Reference Haskett, Scott, Grant, Ward and Robinson2003).

Although evidence suggests a substantial link between parenting stress and child maltreatment, the underlying emotional mechanisms that could potentially moderate this relationship have been less investigated. Internal appraisals of parenting (perceptions and attributions deriving from preexisting cognitive schemata) and emotional distress may interact and spill over into daily caregiver–child relationships, promoting maltreatment behaviors.

5.2 Emotion Regulation in Child Maltreatment

Emotion regulation is defined as the “processes by which individuals influence which emotions they have, when they have them, and how they experience and express these emotions” (Gross & Thompson, Reference Gross, Thompson and Gross2007, p. 275). Parental emotion regulation serves several important goals in parenting (see Chapter 2), and, in disciplinary encounters with the child, involves the ability for parents to control and inhibit their avoidant, intrusive, and aggressive actions, when experiencing negative and stressful emotions (Leith & Baumeister, Reference Leith and Baumeister1996).

In contrast to the many investigations of emotions as reliable correlates of parenting behaviors (e.g. Crandall et al., Reference Crandall, Deater-Deckard and Riley2015; Rueger et al., Reference Rueger, Katz, Risser and Lovejoy2011), work on parents’ emotion regulation (ER) and child maltreatment has lagged behind. In the examination of the predictors of child maltreatment, one important focus has been parents’ emotion reactivity and regulation (Lavi et al., Reference Lavi, Ozer, Katz and Gross2021). To date, studies have shown great variability in the magnitude of the relationships between parental emotional processes and the risk of child maltreatment.

In this perspective, Dix’s model (Reference Dix1991) suggests that dysfunctional and ineffective parenting is characterized by inadequate ER, in terms of hyporeactivity (to positive emotions) or hyperreactivity (to negative emotions) or a mismatch of emotions between parent and child (e.g. child expressing happiness and parent expressing disappointment), with deleterious effects on the parent–child relationship. Therefore, according to the model, guided by cognitive preexisting and biased schemata (Milner, Reference Milner1993, Reference Milner and Hansen2000, Reference Milner2003), when faced with urgent negative emotions, maltreating (or at-risk) parents potentially tend to respond to the child with aversive control, avoidance, and emotional cutoff behaviorsFootnote 1 (Bowen, Reference Bowen1993; Smith, Reference Smith and Titelman2003), in order to find a way to reduce the burden of emotional distress (Mence et al., Reference Mence, Hawes, Wedgwood, Morgan, Barnett, Kohlhoff and Hunt2014). For example, anger is conceptualized in the literature as an emotion leading the parents to react to the child with intrusiveness or disengagement (Crittenden, Reference Crittenden1993), narrowing their attention to the anger-provoking stimuli alone (Gibb et al., Reference Gibb, Johnson, Benas, Uhrlass, Knopik and McGeary2011). This narrowing affects the parents’ cognitive processes and leads them to express anger via impulsive and aggressive actions, with an elevated propensity to child maltreatment (Rodriguez, Reference Rodriguez2018; Rodriquez & Green, Reference Rodriguez and Green1997; Rodriguez & Richardson, Reference Rodriguez and Richardson2007). According to the SIP model (Milner, Reference Milner1993, Reference Milner and Hansen2000, Reference Milner2003), maltreating (or at-risk) parents could be prone to respond aggressively to the perceived provocations of the child, because they feel that aggressive actions will enable them to let off steam and feel less upset and angry (Francis & Wolfe, Reference Francis and Wolfe2008).

Overall, the literature suggests that aggressive and violent behaviors (e.g. child maltreatment, domestic and intimate partner violence) may function to regulate stress and negative emotions (Jakupcak et al., Reference Jakupcak, Lisak and Roemer2002). Aggressive people believe that violence can be a good way of getting rid of their emotional distress and that aggressive behaviors can be undertaken as a good strategy of emotion regulation (Bushman et al., Reference Bushman, Baumeister and Phillips2001). Therefore, during daily caregiver–child interactions, for maltreating (or at-risk) parents, aggressive and violent behaviors could represent not only the inability to manage negative and stressful emotions but also a strategy to cope with this emotional dysregulation (Marziali et al., Reference Marziali, Damianakis and Trocmé2003).

In summary, parental reactivity to negative emotions could become a significant risk factor for child maltreatment, via the path of parental intrusiveness, disengagement, and cognitive narrowing of attention (Bowen, Reference Bowen1993; Crittenden, Reference Crittenden1993; Dix, Reference Dix1991), and this emotion dysregulation could be intended as an outcome of the activation of preexisting cognitive schemata that include previous unresolved relational experiences and unrealistic expectations of self and child (Milner, Reference Milner1993, Reference Milner and Hansen2000, Reference Milner2003).

5.3 Gender Differences

Currently one of the most interesting aspects of the field of child maltreatment is the analysis of possible gender differences to explain dissimilar trajectories of maltreating parenting and also to provide valuable information for intervention and prevention plans. Some studies have investigated general constructs (parenting stress, ER, negative affect, emphatic skills, etc.) and have shown comparable risk profiles for maltreating (or at-risk) fathers and mothers (Asla et al., Reference Asla, de Paúl and Perez-Albeniz2011; Perez-Albeniz & de Paul, Reference Perez-Albeniz and de Paul2004; Smith Slep & O’Leary, Reference Smith Slep and O’Leary2007), although some specific differences have emerged in other studies (e.g. Miragoli et al., Reference Miragoli, Balzarotti, Camisasca and Di Blasio2018; Pittman & Buckley, Reference Pittman and Buckley2006; Schaeffer et al., Reference Schaeffer, Alexander, Bethke and Kretz2005).

Overall, in terms of emotional reactivity and regulation, parents show some significant gender differences, which are attributable to biological factors, in interaction with social roles and ecological conditions (Wood & Eagly, Reference Wood and Eagly2002). In parenthood, social roles and sex-typed goals help define relationships, direct behaviors, and guide decision-making processes (Grusec & Davidov, Reference Grusec, Davidov, Grusec and Hastings2014). Culturally shared beliefs may drive parenting practices of knowledge, expression, and regulation of emotions: mothers are expected to be more emotionally expressive through prosocial and caring parenting actions, whereas fathers are expected to be more pragmatic, detached, and oriented toward achievement (Goodnow & Collins, Reference Goodnow and Collins1990; Sigel et al., Reference Sigel, McGillicuddy-DeLisi and Goodnow1992). Moreover, parenting stress appears to have a greater impact on a mother’s self-assessment of her role as a mother (Berryhill, Reference Berryhill2016; Pearlin et al., Reference Pearlin, Menaghan, Lieberman and Mullan1981), further contaminating the quality of the caregiver-child relationship more easily.

With a view to identifying possible gender differences in maltreating (or at-risk) parenting, Miragoli and colleagues (Reference Miragoli, Milani, Di Blasio and Camisasca2020) analyzed the ER processes in fathers and mothers. Whereas previous studies had illustrated the role of ER in the risk of child maltreatment, in terms of a unitary construct or the effects of single dimensions (e.g. impulsivity, emotional distress, or anger; e.g. Bushman et al., Reference Bushman, Baumeister and Phillips2001; Dadds et al., Reference Dadds, Mullens, McAllister and Atkinson2003; Francis & Wolfe, Reference Francis and Wolfe2008; Rodriguez & Green, Reference Rodriguez and Green1997), in this study some individual components of ER were evaluated comprehensively (acceptance of emotional responses, ability in distracting and performing alternative behaviors when experiencing negative emotions, confidence in the emotional regulation skills, ability in controlling impulsive behaviors when distressed, recognition of emotions, and emotional awareness). The findings confirmed the important role of ER in the risk of child maltreatment and provided support for gender differences in at-risk fathers and mothers. When at-risk fathers experienced negative and stressful emotions, they showed a significant lack of emotional awareness and difficulties in distracting themselves and using alternative behaviors. By contrast, when distressed about an unsatisfactory relationship with the child, at-risk mothers experienced more nonacceptance of negative emotions, difficulties in distracting themselves and using alternative behaviors, and subsequent difficulties in controlling impulsive/aggressive behaviors toward the child.

Both at-risk fathers and mothers, regardless of gender, when feeling emotional parenting stress, showed difficulty in distracting themselves and using alternative behaviors, resulting in an inability to concentrate or effectively complete their activities, because of emotional arousal and the consequent tendency to focus all attention resources on the negative emotional experience. More precisely, these parents failed to pursue goal-directed behaviors, in which distraction could be a protective factor for stress and predictive of adaptive coping (oriented to problem-solving and emotional acceptance; Reynolds & Wells, Reference Reynolds and Wells1999).

A significant gender difference concerns at-risk fathers, who are more characterized by a lack of awareness in the management of negative emotions. A good level of emotional awareness allows individuals to know what they are feeling and to identify useful coping strategies to cope with the emotions and the demands of the context (Clore et al., Reference Clore, Schwarz and Conway1994). Attention and emotional awareness are specific elements of mindful parenting (Kabat-Zinn & Kabat-Zinn, Reference Kabat-Zinn and Kabat-Zinn1997) and allow parents to communicate acceptance, compassion, and kindness in interactions with their child (Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Coatsworth and Greenberg2009; Turpyn & Chaplin, Reference Turpyn and Chaplin2016). According to the SIP model (Milner, Reference Milner1993, Reference Milner and Hansen2000, Reference Milner2003), in maltreating (or at-risk) parents a lack of emotional awareness interferes with all the cognitive stages of discipline response processing (perception and interpretation of child behavior, and integration of available information), causing a failure to choose an effective parenting behavior and leading to episodes of maltreatment to release the emotional anxiety. For these reasons, maltreating (or at-risk) fathers often show authoritarian and intrusive parenting, lack of anger and hostility control, and ineffective and coercive discipline (Rodriguez, Reference Rodriguez2010).

By contrast, nonacceptance of emotional responses and difficulties in controlling impulsive behaviors (when distressed) were specific deficits of at-risk mothers. In social contexts, individuals who do not accept their negative emotions appear avoidant and constantly absorbed in suppressing this emotional distress, failing to achieve the social information needed to respond appropriately to others (Brockman et al., Reference Brockman, Ciarrochi, Parker and Kashdan2017). Specifically, nonacceptance and repeated efforts to suppress negative emotions deplete cognitive resources that could otherwise be used for optimal performance in the social context (for example, for parents, for effective behavior in interactions with their children). In the literature, these individuals are described as avoidant and distracted, with negative feelings about the self and a sense of alienation from others, which often impede the development of emotionally satisfying relationships (Purnamaningsih, Reference Purnamaningsih2017). Clinical research has shown that these individuals may not always be healthy or effective: nonacceptance and emotion suppression could have paradoxical effects on adjustment, increasing the severity and frequency of stressful and unwanted internal experiences (Gross & John, Reference Gross and John2003; Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda and Lillis2006). Therefore, for at-risk mothers, in discipline encounters with their child, non-acceptance and suppression of negative emotions decrease the behavioral expression of those emotions but not the subjective experience (Bailey et al., Reference Bailey, Moran and Pederson2007; Jacobvitz et al., Reference Jacobvitz, Leon and Hazen2006). Thus this emotional strategy does not reduce the subjective experience but contributes to the maintenance and accumulation of unresolved emotions (Gross, Reference Gross1998; Richards & Gross, Reference Richards and Gross1999). In the literature, maltreating mothers show atypical caregiving and are basically unable to accept, monitor, and contain the emotional negative experiences of interaction with their child (e.g. Lyons-Ruth et al., Reference Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Zoll and Stahl1987; Savage et al., Reference Savage, Tarabulsy, Pearson, Collin-Vézina and Gagné2019). At the base of these dysfunctional caregiving behaviors, many authors have identified different traumatic childhood experiences that break into the mother’s mental state and prevent her from being responsive and sensitive with her child (e.g. Hesse & Main, Reference Hesse and Main2006; Lyons-Ruth & Block, Reference Lyons-Ruth and Block1996). Accordingly, in discipline encounters, nonacceptance and suppression of negative emotions could be positively associated with poorer parental functioning (e.g. poorer parental adjustment and compromised discipline practices; Lorber, Reference Lorber2012) and more aggressive parental behaviors, as the capacity to accept negative emotions is a prerequisite for the development of the consequent capacity to maintain control over behavior even in the presence of emotional distress (Gratz & Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004).

5.4 Clinical Implications

This chapter shows that ER processes must be considered when training programs for maltreating or at-risk parents are designed, to promote, specific adaptive regulative skills and adequate parenting behaviors in stressful conditions (Gratz & Gunderson, Reference Gratz and Gunderson2006).

Overall, treatments that focus on avoidance or control of negative and undesirable emotions may not be useful with maltreating or at-risk parents and may inadvertently reinforce a damaging nonacceptance of negative emotions. Instead, treatments based on learning alternative ways of coping and responding to emotional distress could be more productive in mitigating aggressive parenting behaviors. Emotion coaching and mindfulness may be valuable skills for maltreating or at-risk parents, as they promote the emotional modulation of the arousal caused by negative experiences (rather than emotional detachment) resulting from the relationship with the child (Gratz & Tull, Reference Gratz, Tull and Baer2010). Specifically, for maltreating or at-risk fathers, interventions could focus more on the promotion of emotional awareness, by encouraging fathers to observe and label the negative emotions, facilitating contact with these emotions and differentiating between emotional states. By contrast, for maltreating or at-risk mothers, interventions could be more focused on letting go of evaluations (such as “bad” or “wrong”) and taking a nonjudgmental opinion toward the unwanted emotions, facilitating the acceptance and decreasing the development of secondary emotional responses (e.g. fear, anger, guilt or shame). Therefore, clinical interventions with maltreating or at-risk parents should involve reappraisal and modulation of arousal of the negative experience of emotions (rather than eliminating emotions). For at-risk fathers and mothers, it is important to learn to manage the emotional distress deriving from the relationship with the child, understanding that negative emotions can be tolerated (without necessarily being acted on) and facilitating the ability to control impulsive and aggressive behaviors.

5.5 Conclusions and Future Directions

In conclusion, this chapter set out to illustrate the essential role of emotional processes in underlying a cluster of maltreating behaviors and conditions, with significant practical implications for primary and secondary interventions.

First, it is important to highlight that the relation between ER and child maltreatment is bidirectional (Figure 5.1): (1) previous experiences of child maltreatment lead to parental dysfunctional emotion reactivity/regulation and (2) parental dysfunctional emotion reactivity/regulation leads to a higher propensity to child maltreatment (see Chapter 5).

Figure 5.1 Bidirectional relation between emotion regulation (ER) and child maltreatment

Second, child maltreatment can be understood as the result of a dysfunctional ER strategy, where aggressive, violent, and avoidant parenting behaviors are a way to relieve the pressure of stress and negative emotions associated with interacting with the child.

Finally, although much remains to be done at the intersection of ER and child maltreatment, the most pressing requirements appear to be to explore the possible role of ER as a moderator between parenting stress and child maltreatment (in particular, the question whether stressed parents with good levels of ER are less likely to maltreat or be violent with their children), and to identify precise risk profiles for at-risk fathers and mothers to improve understanding of the emotional processes that guide the intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment.

Footnotes

1 Emotional cutoff behavior can be considered a process of distancing and cutting oneself off from emotions or affects that lead, most often unconsciously, to a feared loss of control and/or suffering (Titelman, Reference Titelman2014). People who are emotionally cut off find intimacy profoundly threatening and, therefore, tend to display an exaggerated independence and to isolate themselves from others, denying the importance of attachment and family. In family systems theory of Bowen (Reference Bowen1993), emotional cutoff behavior is a mechanism for managing relational anxiety and parenting stress, and in caregiver–child interactions is manifested in physical distance, lack of contact, and avoidance toward the child.

References

Abidin, R. (1992). The determinants of parenting behavior. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 407412. https://doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2104_12Google Scholar
Abidin, R. (1995). The Parenting Stress Index. Pediatric Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Asla, N., de Paúl, J., & Perez-Albeniz, A. (2011). Emotion recognition in fathers and mothers at high-risk for child physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 35, 712721. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2011.05.010Google Scholar
Azar, S. (1998). A framework for understanding child maltreatment: An integration of cognitive behavioral and developmental perspectives. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 18, 340355.Google Scholar
Azar, S. (2002). Parenting and child maltreatment. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Social conditions and applied parenting (pp. 361388). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bailey, H. N., Moran, G., & Pederson, D. R. (2007). Childhood maltreatment, complex trauma symptoms, and unresolved attachment in an at-risk sample of adolescent mothers. Attachment & Human Development, 9(2), 139161. https://doi:10.1080/14616730701349721Google Scholar
Berryhill, M. (2016). Mothers’ parenting stress and engagement: Mediating role of parental competence. Marriage & Family Review, 52(5), 461480. https://doi:10.1080/01494929.2015.1113600Google Scholar
Bowen, M. (1993). Family therapy in clinical practice. Jason Aronson.Google Scholar
Brockman, R., Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P., & Kashdan, T. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies in daily life: Mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 46(2), 91113. https://doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.30CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bugental, D. B., Ellerson, P. C., Lin, E. K., Rainey, B., Kokotovic, A., & O’Hara, N. (2002). A cognitive approach to child abuse prevention. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 243258. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bushman, B., Baumeister, R., & Phillips, C. (2001). Do people aggress to improve their mood? Catharsis beliefs, affect regulation opportunity, and aggressive responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 1732. https://doi:10.1037//0022-3514.81.1.17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cain, D., & Combs-Orme, T. (2005). Family structure effects on parenting stress and practices in the African American family. Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare, 32, 1940.Google Scholar
Camisasca, E., Miragoli, S., & Di Blasio, P. (2014). Is the relationship between marital adjustment and parenting stress mediated or moderated by parenting alliance? Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 10(2), 235254. https://doi:10.5964/ejop.v10i2.724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clore, G., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Cognitive causes and consequences of emotion. Handbook of Social Cognition, 1, 323417.Google Scholar
Coyl, D., Roggman, L., & Newland, L. (2002). Stress, maternal depression, and negative mother-infant interactions in relation to infant attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23, 145163. https://doi:10.1002/imhj.10009Google Scholar
Crandall, A., Deater-Deckard, K., & Riley, A. (2015). Maternal emotion and cognitive control capacities and parenting: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review, 36, 105126. https://doi:10.1016/j.dr.2015.01.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crittenden, P. (1993). An information-processing perspective on the behavior of neglectful parents. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 20, 2748. https://doi:10.1177/0093854893020001004Google Scholar
Crnic, K., & Low, C. (2002). Everyday stresses and parenting. In Boernstein, M. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Practical issues in parenting (pp. 243267). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crnic, K., & Ross, E. (2017). Parenting stress and parental efficacy. In Deater-Deckard, K and Panneton, R. (Eds.), Parental stress and early child development (pp. 263284). Springer.Google Scholar
Crouch, J., & Behl, L. (2001). Relationships among parental beliefs in corporal punishment, reported stress, and physical child abuse potential. Child Abuse & Neglect, 25, 413419. https://doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(00)00256-8Google Scholar
Crouch, J., Skowronski, J., Milner, J., & Harris, B. (2008). Parental responses to infant crying: The influence of child physical abuse risk and hostile priming. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32(7), 702710. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crouch, J. L., Risser, H. J., Skowronski, J. J., Milner, J. S., Farc, M. M., & Irwin, L. M. (2010). Does accessibility of positive and negative schema vary by child physical abuse risk? Child Abuse & Neglect, 34, 886895. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2010.05.005Google Scholar
Dadds, M., Mullens, M., McAllister, R., & Atkinson, E. (2003). Attributions, affect, and behavior in abuse-risk mothers: A laboratory study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27, 2145. https://doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00510-0Google Scholar
de Paúl, J., Asla, N., Pérez-Albéniz, A., & De Cádiz, B. (2006). Impact of stress and mitigating information on evaluations, attributions, affect, disciplinary choices, and expectations of compliance in mothers at high and low risk for child physical abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 10181045. https://doi:10.1177/0886260506290411CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deater-Deckard, K. (1998). Parenting stress and child adjustment: Some old hypotheses and new questions. Clinical Psychology Science and Practice, 5, 314332. https://doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.1998.tb00152.xGoogle Scholar
Deater-Deckard, K. (2004). Parenting stress. Yale University.Google Scholar
Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and maladaptative processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 325. https://doi:10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.3Google Scholar
Duncan, L., Coatsworth, J., & Greenberg, M. (2009). A model of mindful parenting: Implications for parent-child relationships and prevention research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 12, 255270. https://doi:10.1007/s10567-009-0046-3Google Scholar
Ethier, L., Lacharite, C., & Couture, G. (1995). Childhood adversity, parental stress, and depression of negligent mothers. Child Abuse & Neglect, 19(5), 619632. https://doi:10.1016/0145-2134(95)00020-9Google Scholar
Farc, M., Crouch, J., Skowronski, J., & Milner, J. (2008). Hostility ratings by parents at risk for child abuse: Impact of chronic and temporary schema activation. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 177193. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.06.001Google Scholar
Francis, K., & Wolfe, D. (2008). Cognitive and emotional differences between abusive and non-abusive fathers. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 11271137. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.05.007Google Scholar
Gibb, B., Johnson, A., Benas, J., Uhrlass, D., Knopik, V., & McGeary, J. (2011). Children’s 5-HTTLPR genotype moderates the link between maternal criticism and attentional biases specifically for facial displays of anger. Cognition & Emotion, 25(6), 11041120. https://doi:10.1080/02699931.2010.508267CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodnow, J., & Collins, W. (1990). Development according to parents: The nature, sources, and consequences of parents’ ideas. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gratz, K. L., & Gunderson, J. G. (2006). Preliminary data on an acceptance-based emotion regulation group intervention for deliberate self-harm among women with borderline personality disorder. Behavior Therapy, 37(1), 2535. https://doi:10.1016/j.beth.2005.03.002Google Scholar
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(1), 4154. https://doi:10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gratz, K. L., & Tull, M. T. (2010). Emotion regulation as a mechanism of change in acceptance-and mindfulness-based treatments. In Baer, R. A. (Ed.), Assessing mindfulness and acceptance: Illuminating the processes of change (pp. 107133). New Harbinger Publications.Google Scholar
Gross, J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271299. https://doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271Google Scholar
Gross, J., & John, O. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348362. https://doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J., & Thompson, R. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 324). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Grusec, J., & Davidov, M. (2014). Socialization in the family: The roles of parents. In Grusec, J. E. & Hastings, P. D. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 284308). Guilford.Google Scholar
Haskett, M., Scott, S., Grant, R., Ward, C., & Robinson, C. (2003). Child-related cognitions and affective functioning of physically abusive and comparison parents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27, 663686. https://doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(03)00103-0Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(1), 125. https://doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006Google Scholar
Hesse, E., & Main, M. (2006). Frightened, threatening, and dissociative parental behavior in low-risk samples: Description, discussion, and interpretations. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 309343. https://doi:10.1017/S0954579406060172Google Scholar
Jackson, A., & Huang, C. (2000). Parenting stress and behavior among single mothers of preschoolers: The mediating role of self-efficacy. Journal of Social Service Research, 26, 2942. https://doi:10.1080/01488370009511335Google Scholar
Jacobvitz, D., Leon, K., & Hazen, N. (2006). Does expectant mothers’ unresolved trauma predict frightened/frightening maternal behavior? Risk and protective factors. Development and Psychopathology, 18(2), 363379. https://doi:10.1017/S0954579406060196Google Scholar
Jakupcak, M., Lisak, D., & Roemer, L. (2002). The role of masculine ideology and masculine gender role stress in men’s perpetration of relationship violence. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 3(2), 97106. https://doi:10.1037/1524-9220.3.2.97Google Scholar
Kabat-Zinn, M., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (1997). Everyday blessings. The inner work of mindful parenting. Hachette Books.Google Scholar
Krahé, B., Bondü, R., Höse, A., & Esser, G. (2015). Child aggression as a source and a consequence of parenting stress: A three-wave longitudinal study. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 25, 328339. https://doi:10.1111/jora.12115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lau, A., Valeri, S., McCarty, C., & Weisz, J. (2006). Abusive parents’ reports of child behavior problems: Relationship to observed parent-child interactions. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30, 639655. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.11.009Google Scholar
Lavi, I., Ozer, E., Katz, L., & Gross, J. (2021). The role of parental emotion reactivity and regulation in child maltreatment and maltreatment risk: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 90, 102099. https://doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102099Google Scholar
Leith, K., & Baumeister, R. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risk tasking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(6), 12501267. https://doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.6.1250Google Scholar
Lorber, M. (2012). The role of maternal emotion regulation in overreactive and lax discipline. Journal of Family Psychology, 26, 642647. https://doi:10.1037/a0029109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons-Ruth, K., & Block, D. (1996). The disturbed caregiving system: Relations among childhood trauma, maternal caregiving, and infant affect and attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 17, 257275. https://doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199623)17:3<257::AID-IMHJ5>3.0.CO;2-LGoogle Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D., Zoll, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). Infants at social risk: Relations among infant maltreatment, maternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. Developmental Psychology, 23(2), 223232. https://doi:10.1037/0012-1649.23.2.223Google Scholar
Mammen, O., Kolko, D., & Pilkonis, P. (2002). Negative affect and parental aggression in child physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 407424. https://doi:10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00316-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marziali, E., Damianakis, T., & Trocmé, N. (2003). Nature and consequences of personality problems in maltreating caregivers. Families in Society, 84(4), 530538. https://doi:10.1606/1044-3894.141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McElroy, E., & Rodriguez, C. (2008). Mothers of children with externalizing behavior problems: Cognitive risk factors for abuse potential and discipline style and practices. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 774784. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.01.002Google Scholar
McPherson, A., Lewis, K., Lynn, A., Haskett, M., & Behrend, T. (2009). Predictors of parenting stress for abusive and non-abusive mothers. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18, 6169. https://doi:10.1007/s10826-008-9207-0Google Scholar
Mence, M., Hawes, D., Wedgwood, L., Morgan, S., Barnett, B., Kohlhoff, J., & Hunt, C. (2014). Emotional flooding and hostile discipline in the families of toddlers with disruptive behavior problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 28, 1221. https://doi:10.1037/a0035352.Google Scholar
Milner, J. (1993). Social information processing and physical child abuse. Clinical Psychology Review, 13, 275294. https://doi:10.1016/0272-7358(93)90024-GCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, J. (2000). Social information processing and physical child abuse. Theory and research. In Hansen, D. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Motivation and child maltreatment (pp. 3984). University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Milner, J. (2003). Social information processing in high-risk and physically abusive parents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27, 720. https://doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00506-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miragoli, S., Balzarotti, S., Camisasca, E., & Di Blasio, P. (2018). Parents’ perception of child behavior, parenting stress, and child abuse potential: Individual and partner influences. Child Abuse & Neglect, 84C, 146156. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.07.034Google Scholar
Miragoli, S., Milani, L., Di Blasio, P., & Camisasca, E. (2020). Difficulties in emotion regulation in child abuse potential: Gender differences in parents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 106, 104529. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104529CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montes, P., de Paul, J., & Milner, J. (2001). Evaluations, attributions, affect, and disciplinary choices in mothers at high and low risk for child physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 25, 10151036. https://doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(01)00254-XGoogle Scholar
Perez-Albeniz, A., & de Paúl, J. (2003). Dispositional empathy in high- and low-risk parents for child physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27, 769780. https://doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(03)00111-XGoogle Scholar
Perez-Albeniz, A., & de Paul, J. (2004). Gender differences in empathy in parents at high and low risk of child physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28, 289300. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.11.01Google Scholar
Pearlin, L., Menaghan, E., Lieberman, M., & Mullan, J. (1981). The stress process. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22, 337356.Google Scholar
Pittman, J., & Buckley, R. R. (2006). Comparing maltreating fathers and mothers in terms of personal distress, interpersonal functioning, and perceptions of family climate. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30, 481496. https://doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.10.017Google Scholar
Purnamaningsih, E. (2017). Personality and emotion regulation strategies. International Journal of Psychological Research, 10(1), 5360. https://doi:10.21500/20112084.2040.Google Scholar
Reynolds, M., & Wells, A. (1999). The Thought Control Questionnaire – psychometric properties in a clinical sample, and relationships with PTSD and depression. Psychological Medicine, 29(5), 10891099. https://doi:10.1017/S003329179900104XGoogle Scholar
Richards, J., & Gross, J. (1999). Composure at any cost? The cognitive consequences of emotion suppression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(8), 10331044. https://doi:10.1177/01461672992511010Google Scholar
Rodriguez, C. (2010). Parent–child aggression: Association with child abuse potential and parenting styles. Violence and Victims, 25, 728741. https://doi:10.1891/0886-6708.25.6.728CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez, C. (2018). Predicting parent–child aggression risk: Cognitive factors and their interaction with anger. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 33, 359378. https://doi:10.1177/0886260516629386Google Scholar
Rodriguez, C., & Green, A. (1997). Parenting stress and anger expression as predictors of child abuse potential. Child Abuse & Neglect, 21(4), 367377. https://doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(96)00177-9Google Scholar
Rodriguez, C., & Richardson, M. (2007). Stress and anger as contextual factors and preexisting cognitive schemas: Predicting parental child maltreatment risk. Child Maltreatment, 12(4), 325337. https://doi:10.1177/1077559507305993Google Scholar
Rueger, S., Katz, R., Risser, H., & Lovejoy, M. (2011). Relations between parental affect and parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Parenting: Science and Practice, 11, 133. https://doi:10.1080/15295192.2011.539503.Google Scholar
Savage, L., Tarabulsy, G., Pearson, J., Collin-Vézina, D., & Gagné, L. (2019). Maternal history of childhood maltreatmen and later parenting behavior: A meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 921. https://doi:10.1017/S0954579418001542Google Scholar
Schaeffer, C., Alexander, P., Bethke, K., & Kretz, L. (2005). Predictors of child abuse potential among military parents: Comparing mothers and fathers. Journal of Family Violence, 20, 123129. https://doi:10.1007/s10896-005-3175-6Google Scholar
Scheel, M., & Rieckmann, T. (1998). An empirically derived description of self-efficacy and empowerment for parents of children identified as psychologically disordered. American Journal of Family Therapy, 26, 1527. https://doi:10.1080/01926189808251083Google Scholar
Sigel, I., McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A., & Goodnow, J. (1992). Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children. Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, W. (2003). Emotional cutoff and family stability: Child abuse in family emotional process. In Titelman, P. (Ed.), Emotional cutoff: Bowen family systems theory perspectives (pp. 351378). Haworth.Google Scholar
Smith Slep, A., & O’Leary, S. (2007). Multivariate models of mothers’ and fathers’ aggression toward their children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 739751. https://doi:10.1037/0022-006X.75.5.739Google Scholar
Titelman, P. (2014). Emotional cutoff: Bowen family systems theory perspectives. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timmer, S., Borrego, J., & Urquiza, A. (2002). Antecedents of coercive interactions in physically abusive mother-child dyads. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 17, 836853. https://doi:10.1177/0886260502017008003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turpyn, C., & Chaplin, T. (2016). Mindful parenting and parents’ emotion expression: Effects on adolescent risk behaviors. Mindfulness, 7, 246254. https://doi:10.1007/s12671-015-0440-5Google Scholar
Whipple, E., & Webster-Stratton, C. (1991). The role of parental stress in physically abusive families. Child Abuse & Neglect, 15, 279291. https://doi:10.1016/0145-2134(91)90072-LGoogle Scholar
Wood, W., & Eagly, A. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins of sex differences. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 699727. https://doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.699Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 5.1 Bidirectional relation between emotion regulation (ER) and child maltreatment

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×