Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T08:09:30.983Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Facial and Bodily Emotion Recognition in Multiple Sclerosis: The Role of Alexithymia and Other Characteristics of the Disease

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 November 2014

Cinzia Cecchetto*
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Sector, SISSA, Trieste, Italy, Udine, Italy
Marilena Aiello
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Sector, SISSA, Trieste, Italy, Udine, Italy
Delia D’Amico
Affiliation:
S.O.C. Neurologia, Azienda Ospedaliero Universitaria “Santa Maria della Misericordia”
Daniela Cutuli
Affiliation:
S.O.C. Neurologia, Azienda Ospedaliero Universitaria “Santa Maria della Misericordia”
Daniela Cargnelutti
Affiliation:
S.O.C. Neurologia, Azienda Ospedaliero Universitaria “Santa Maria della Misericordia”
Roberto Eleopra
Affiliation:
S.O.C. Neurologia, Azienda Ospedaliero Universitaria “Santa Maria della Misericordia”
Raffaella Ida Rumiati
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Sector, SISSA, Trieste, Italy, Udine, Italy
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Cinzia Cecchetto, Neuroscience and Society Lab, SISSA - International School for Advanced Studies, via Bonomea 265, 34100 Trieste Italy. E-mail: ccecchet@sissa.it
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Multiple sclerosis (MS) may be associated with impaired perception of facial emotions. However, emotion recognition mediated by bodily postures has never been examined in these patients. Moreover, several studies have suggested a relation between emotion recognition impairments and alexithymia. This is in line with the idea that the ability to recognize emotions requires the individuals to be able to understand their own emotions. Despite a deficit in emotion recognition has been observed in MS patients, the association between impaired emotion recognition and alexithymia has received little attention. The aim of this study was, first, to investigate MS patient’s abilities to recognize emotions mediated by both facial and bodily expressions and, second, to examine whether any observed deficits in emotions recognition could be explained by the presence of alexithymia. Thirty patients with MS and 30 healthy matched controls performed experimental tasks assessing emotion discrimination and recognition of facial expressions and bodily postures. Moreover, they completed questionnaires evaluating alexithymia, depression, and fatigue. First, facial emotion recognition and, to a lesser extent, bodily emotion recognition can be impaired in MS patients. In particular, patients with higher disability showed an impairment in emotion recognition compared with patients with lower disability and controls. Second, their deficit in emotion recognition was not predicted by alexithymia. Instead, the disease’s characteristics and the performance on some cognitive tasks significantly correlated with emotion recognition. Impaired facial emotion recognition is a cognitive signature of MS that is not dependent on alexithymia. (JINS, 2014, 19, 1–11)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2014 

Introduction

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a chronic inflammatory disease of the central nervous system associated with accumulating multifocal tissue damage, early onset, and variable and unpredictable course (Compston & Coles, Reference Compston and Coles2002). Cognitive and emotional impairments are frequently observed in MS patients, together with neuropsychiatric symptoms such as anxiety and depression (Feinstein, DeLuca, Baune, Filippi, & Lassman, Reference Feinstein, DeLuca, Baune, Filippi and Lassman2013). In particular, as for patients with Parkinson’s disease (Aiello et al., Reference Aiello, Eleopra, Lettieri, Mondani, D’Auria, Belgrado and Rumiati2014; Assogna, Pontieri, Caltagirone, & Spalletta, Reference Assogna, Pontieri, Caltagirone and Spalletta2008; Baggio et al., Reference Baggio, Segura, Ibarretxe-Bilbao, Valldeoriola, Marti, Compta and Junque2012; Narme et al., Reference Narme, Mouras, Roussel, Duru, Krystkowiak and Godefroy2013; Sprengelmeyer et al., Reference Sprengelmeyer, Young, Mahn, Schroeder, Woitalla, Büttner and Przuntek2003), schizophrenia (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Harris, Sprengelmeyer, Sprengelmeyer, Young, Santos and Lawrie2004; Namiki et al., Reference Namiki, Hirao, Yamada, Hanakawa, Fukuyama, Hayashi and Murai2007; Tsui et al., Reference Tsui, Huang, Lui, Au, Leung, Cheung and Chan2013), progressive supranuclear palsy (Ghosh, Rowe, Calder, Hodges, & Bak, Reference Ghosh, Rowe, Calder, Hodges and Bak2009; Pontieri et al., Reference Pontieri, Assogna, Stefani, Pierantozzi, Meco, Benincasa and Spalletta2012), mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease (McCade, Savage, & Naismith, Reference McCade, Savage and Naismith2012; Spoletini et al., Reference Spoletini, Marra, Iulio, Gianni, Sancesario, Giubilei and Spalletta2008) or Huntington’s disease (Robotham, Sauter, Bachoud-Lévi, & Trinkler, Reference Robotham, Sauter, Bachoud-Lévi and Trinkler2011; Sprengelmeyer, Schroeder, Young, & Epplen, Reference Sprengelmeyer, Schroeder, Young and Epplen2006), MS patients experience deficient emotion recognition (Henry et al., Reference Henry, Phillips, Beatty, McDonald, Longley, Joscelyne and Rendell2009, Reference Henry, Tourbah, Chaunu, Rumbach, Montreuil and Bakchine2011; Jehna et al., Reference Jehna, Neuper, Petrovic, Wallner-Blazek, Schmidt, Fuchs and Enzinger2010; Krause et al., Reference Krause, Wendt, Dressel, Berneiser, Kessler, Hamm and Lotze2009; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Henry, Scott, Summers, Whyte and Cook2011; Prochnow et al., Reference Prochnow, Donell, Schäfer, Jörgens, Hartung, Franz and Seitz2011; but see Di Bitonto et al., Reference Di Bitonto, Longato, Jung, Fleury, Marcel, Collongues and Blanc2011; Jehna et al., Reference Jehna, Langkammer, Wallner-Blazek, Neuper, Loitfelder, Ropele and Enzinger2011; Pinto et al., Reference Pinto, Gomes, Moreira, Rosa, Santos, Silva and Cavaco2012). Given its role in non-verbal communication it is, therefore, not surprising that a deficit in emotion recognition is found to interfere with interpersonal relationships and adversely affect family and social life (Schwartz & Frohner, Reference Schwartz and Frohner2005).

Previous studies showed that emotion recognition mediated by facial expressions could be impaired in patients with MS (Di Bitonto et al., Reference Di Bitonto, Longato, Jung, Fleury, Marcel, Collongues and Blanc2011; Henry et al., Reference Henry, Phillips, Beatty, McDonald, Longley, Joscelyne and Rendell2009, Reference Henry, Tourbah, Chaunu, Rumbach, Montreuil and Bakchine2011; Jehna et al., Reference Jehna, Neuper, Petrovic, Wallner-Blazek, Schmidt, Fuchs and Enzinger2010, Reference Jehna, Langkammer, Wallner-Blazek, Neuper, Loitfelder, Ropele and Enzinger2011; Krause et al., Reference Krause, Wendt, Dressel, Berneiser, Kessler, Hamm and Lotze2009; Pinto et al., Reference Pinto, Gomes, Moreira, Rosa, Santos, Silva and Cavaco2012; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Henry, Scott, Summers, Whyte and Cook2011; Prochnow et al., Reference Prochnow, Donell, Schäfer, Jörgens, Hartung, Franz and Seitz2011). However, bodily expressions are also important in conveying information that is essential for social interaction (de Gelder, 2006; de Gelder, Snyder, Greve et al., 2004; de Gelder et al., Reference de Gelder, Van den Stock, Meeren, Sinke, Kret and Tamietto2010; Kret, Stekelenburg, Roelofs, et al., 2013). In healthy individuals, emotions mediated by bodily expressions are recognized as well as those mediated by facial expressions. While the co-occurrence of deficits in recognizing facial expressions and bodily postures has been documented in some pathological populations (de Gelder, Van den Stock, Balaguer et al., 2008; Hadjikhani, Joseph, Manoach et al., 2009; Tamietto, Geminiani, Genero, et al., 2007; Van den Stock, van de Riet, Righart, et al., 2008), to our knowledge, recognition of the bodily emotional expressions in MS patients has never been studied before.

Recently, it has also been suggested that emotion recognition deficit is somewhat related to alexithymia—a cognitive-affective disturbance marked by a reduced ability to identify and describe one’s feelings, together with a difficulty in distinguishing feelings from bodily sensations and a tendency to focus on external events (Nemiah, Freyberger, & Sifneos, Reference Nemiah, Freyberger and Sifneos1976). The prevalence of alexithymia in MS has been suggested to vary between 13.8% and 50% (Bodini et al., Reference Bodini, Mandarelli, Tomassini, Tarsitani, Pestalozza, Gasperini and Pozzilli2008; Chahraoui et al., Reference Chahraoui, Pinoit, Viegas, Adnet, Bonin and Moreau2008), and seems to contribute to the severity of depression and fatigue (Bodini et al., Reference Bodini, Mandarelli, Tomassini, Tarsitani, Pestalozza, Gasperini and Pozzilli2008; Gay, Vrignaud, Garitte, & Meunier, Reference Gay, Vrignaud, Garitte and Meunier2010). Despite the extensive co-occurrence of emotion recognition deficit and alexithymia, to our knowledge only one study directly assessed this association in MS patients (Prochnow et al., Reference Prochnow, Donell, Schäfer, Jörgens, Hartung, Franz and Seitz2011). In this study, the authors investigated patients’ and healthy participants’ ability to recognize six different emotional facial expressions (i.e., happiness, anger, fear, sadness, surprise, and disgust), as well as the presence of alexithymia. Patients obtained higher alexithymia scores and were also less accurate in labeling emotions than controls; however, no significant correlation between alexithymia and emotion recognition was found, even if the impairment in recognizing emotions was associated with alexithymia in the majority of patients. As the patients enrolled suffered from both secondary progressive and relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis and the course of the disease is known to augment cognitive dysfunctions (Chiaravalloti & DeLuca, Reference Chiaravalloti and DeLuca2008), it is difficult to clearly interpret the results presented in this study.

The association of emotion recognition and alexithymia deficits is consistent with the simulationist models of emotion recognition according to which, to understand other people’s emotions, one needs to simulate, replicate, or reproduce in his own mind the same state as the other’s (Barsalou, Reference Barsalou2008; Goldman & Sripada, Reference Goldman and Sripada2005). In line with these models, if the ability to understand one’s own emotions is somewhat impaired, the understanding of other’s people emotions is also expected to be affected. For instance, patients with amygdala lesions are impaired both at experiencing fear as well as recognizing facial expressions of fear (Adolphs, Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio, Reference Adolphs, Tranel, Damasio and Damasio1994; Adolphs et al., Reference Adolphs, Tranel, Hamann, Young, Calder, Phelps and Damasio1999).

The aim of the present study was twofold. First, we tested whether impaired emotion recognition in MS patients can selectively affect faces or whether it can also affect bodily postures. In particular, if recognition of bodily expressions is controlled by the same mechanism that controls perception of facial expressions, we should observe impairment in emotion recognition of both types of stimuli when this mechanism is damaged. On the contrary, if dissociation in recognizing these two types of stimuli is observed, we hypothesized that the two processes are at least partially independent. Second, we aimed at testing whether alleged deficits in emotion recognition observed in patients with MS could be associated with concurrent alexithymia. If the ability to understand our own emotions is necessary to understand other people’s emotions, then an association of deficits should be observed. Alternatively, if no association were to be found, we could conclude that the ability to understand our own emotions is not necessary to understand other people’s emotions. To achieve this aim, we had 30 patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis (RRMS) and 30 healthy participants complete the 20-item revised Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Bressi et al., Reference Bressi, Taylor, Parker, Bressi, Brambilla, Aguglia and Bucca1996) and to perform a series of experimental tasks assessing emotion discrimination and recognition mediated both by facial and bodily expressions.

Methods

Participants

Thirty patients (21 female) with clinically diagnosed MS took part in the study. They were recruited from the neurological unit of the Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria “Santa Maria della Misericordia,” in Udine. Inclusion criteria were a definitive diagnosis of Relapsing-Remitting MS (RRMS) according to the criteria defined by McDonald et al. (Reference McDonald, Compston, Edan, Goodkin, Hartung, Lublin and Wolinsky2001); an Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS; Kurtzke, Reference Kurtzke1983) score ≤4.5; age between 20 and 40 years; no corticosteroid pulse within the past 6 weeks; a minimum estimated IQ of 80; no history of other neurological disorder or major depression or other psychiatric disorders (i.e., anxiety, bipolar disorders, schizophrenia); no severe motor or visual impairment; a score greater than 20 on the Benton Facial Recognition Task (Benton, Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, Reference Benton, Hamsher, Varney and Spreen1983). Thirty age-matched healthy adults (21 females), with no history of neurological disorder or psychiatric disorders, and who present a score greater than 20 on the Benton Facial Recognition Task (Benton et al., Reference Benton, Hamsher, Varney and Spreen1983), took part in the study as controls. The SISSA Ethics Committee approved the study that was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and with its later amendments. Participants signed informed consents before taking part in the study.

Neuropsychological Assessment

Patients performed the Rao’s Brief Repeatable Battery (BRB; Amato et al., Reference Amato, Portaccio, Goretti, Zipoli, Ricchiuti, De Caro and Trojano2006) which assesses short- and long-term spatial and non-spatial memory (Selective Reminding, Spatial Recall), attention and processing speed (Symbol Digit Modalities, Paced Auditory Serial Addition test) and semantic verbal fluency (Word List Generation). Patients also completed the Benton Facial Recognition task (Benton et al., Reference Benton, Hamsher, Varney and Spreen1983), Trail Making test (Giovagnoli et al., Reference Giovagnoli, Del Pesce, Mascheroni, Simoncelli, Laiacona and Capitani1996), Phonemic verbal fluency (Novelli, Papagno, Capitani, & Laiadcona, 1986), and Verbal and Spatial span (Orsini et al., Reference Orsini, Grossi, Capitani, Laiacona, Papagno and Vallar1987).

Tests of Emotion Recognition

Facial Expressions (stimuli and experimental tasks)

One hundred twelve stimuli were used for each experiment. Seventy faces belonged to the Nim Set collection of facial expressions [http://www.macbrain.org/resources.htm; see Tottenham et al. (Reference Tottenham, Tanaka, Leon, McCarry, Nurse, Hare and Nelson2009) and consisted of naturally posed photographs of professional actors (8 female and 8 males) of different nationalities expressing anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise]. The remaining 42 were selected from a dataset composed by similar photographs of six old actors (three female) of different nationalities and collected in our laboratory according to the Nim set criteria. All stimuli were included within a 9 cm×12 cm frame.

Participants performed three tasks involving facial stimuli. The first was an emotion recognition task in which they were requested to choose which of the six labels (i.e., the word “happiness”) correctly matched the facial expression (of an happy face), and then rate their intensity using a continuous scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much). Participants’ responses were recorded by the experimenter who wrote on a score sheet whether a response was correct or incorrect. The task consisted of 30 trials (five for each emotion) and lasted 5 min. The second was an emotion discrimination task in which participants were asked to judge whether two emotional facial stimuli expressed the same or different emotions. The third was an identity discrimination task, in which participants had to judge whether two faces expressed the same or a different identity (i.e., person). In both tasks, participants were asked to respond “as quickly but as accurately as possible” and they answered by pressing yes/no buttons on the computer keyboard. The dependent variables were accuracy and reaction times. During each trial, two faces were presented serially, separated by 1000 ms inter-stimulus lapse; the second task consisted of 72 trials and lasted 10 min while the third task consisted of 36 trials and lasted 5 min.

Bodily expressions (stimuli and experimental tasks)

Photographs of bodily emotional expressions were selected from the BEAST set (http://www.beatricedegelder.com/beast.html; see also de Gelder and Van den Stock, Reference de Gelder and Van den Stock2011). This set consist of 254 black-and-white posed photographs of face-blurred whole body expressions, taken from 46 non-professional actors (31 female and 15 males), instructed in a standardized procedure to display four expressions (i.e., anger, fear, happiness, and sadness) with the whole body. A total of 128 stimuli (6 female and 6 male actors) were used and framed within an 8 cm×12 cm area.

The tasks involving bodily stimuli were the same as those used with facial stimuli. The emotion recognition task consisted of 32 trials and lasted 7 min, the emotion discrimination task of 60 trials and lasted 9 min, and the identity discrimination task of 60 trials and lasted 10 min.

Self-report Questionnaires

Alexithymia

All participants were assessed for alexithymia with the 20-items Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Bressi et al., Reference Bressi, Taylor, Parker, Bressi, Brambilla, Aguglia and Bucca1996). The TAS total score corresponds to the general level of alexithymia. The TAS-20 includes three subscales: Difficulty in Identifying Feelings (F1), Difficulty in Communicating Feelings (F2) and Externally Oriented Thinking (F3). The international cutoff values are the following: 20–50=non-alexithymic subjects; 51–60=borderline alexithymic subjects; 61–100=alexithymic subjects (Bressi et al., Reference Bressi, Taylor, Parker, Bressi, Brambilla, Aguglia and Bucca1996).

Depression

All participants were assessed for depression with the 13-item Beck Depression Inventory (BDI: Beck & Beck, Reference Beck and Beck1972). Each item is scored on a four point Likert scale ranging from 0 to 3; the sum of each item gives the BDI total score (range, 0–39). The 13-item BDI has been used since it has been suggested that the inclusion of somatic and vegetative symptoms (items 14–21 of the BDI-II) in the evaluation of depression in MS could produce an overestimation of its severity (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Tennant, Sharrock, Hodgkinson, Dunn and Pollard2006; Nyenhuis et al., Reference Nyenhuis, Rao, Zajecka, Luchetta, Bernardin and Garron1995; Quaranta et al., Reference Quaranta, Marra, Zinno, Patanella, Messina, Piccininni and Gainotti2012). This version has been already used with MS patients (Besharat, Pourhosein, Rostami, & Bazzazian, Reference Besharat, Pourhosein, Rostami and Bazzazian2011; Pittion-Vouyovitch et al., Reference Pittion-Vouyovitch, Debouverie, Guillemin, Vandenberghe, Anxionnat and Vespignani2006; Solari et al., Reference Solari, Filippini, Mendozzi, Ghezzi, Cifani, Barbieri and Mosconi1999).

Fatigue

MS patients (but 4) were assessed for fatigue using the Fatigue Severity Scale (FSS: Krupp, LaRocca, Muir-Nash, & Steinberg, Reference Krupp, LaRocca, Muir-Nash and Steinberg1989). The FSS is a self-rated questionnaire composed by nine items selected for their ability to describe fatigue in MS (Krupp et al., Reference Krupp, LaRocca, Muir-Nash and Steinberg1989). Patients were asked to express their agreement on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 7 (strongly disagree). The final score is calculated by averaging each item scores. Patients were considered to be fatigued if they obtained a total FSS score of ≥4 (according to Bakshi et al., Reference Bakshi, Shaikh, Miletich, Czarnecki, Dmochowski, Henschel and Kinkel2000; Bodini et al., Reference Bodini, Mandarelli, Tomassini, Tarsitani, Pestalozza, Gasperini and Pozzilli2008).

Procedure

Participants performed emotion recognition task, emotion discrimination task and identity discrimination task with facial expressions and bodily postures as stimuli. They completed the questionnaires for alexithymia (TAS-20: Bressi et al., Reference Bressi, Taylor, Parker, Bressi, Brambilla, Aguglia and Bucca1996), depression (BDI: Beck & Beck, Reference Beck and Beck1972), and fatigue (FSS: Krupp et al., Reference Krupp, LaRocca, Muir-Nash and Steinberg1989). The order of the tests was kept constant whereas the order of the stimulus type was counterbalanced across participants. The stimuli were presented using PowerPoint software (emotion recognition tasks) or the E-prime experimental software package (emotion and identity discrimination tasks) on a LCD screen of a 17.3-inch HP laptop. Participants sat approximately 58 cm from the computer screen.

Statistical Analysis

Between-group comparisons for socio-demographic and clinical scores were performed using t tests for continuous variables. Two analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed separately for faces and for bodily postures. An ANOVA on accuracy with group (MS, HC) as between factor, and task (emotion recognition, emotion discrimination and identity discrimination) as within factor; an ANOVA with group (MS, HC) as between factor, and emotion as within factor were conducted to explore possible group differences in the recognition of single emotions. Accuracy percentages were arcsine-transformed to normalize the distribution of the accuracy data in the emotion tasks. To better characterize their emotion processing, patients were divided into two groups according to the median of the EDSS score, 1.5. Six one-way ANOVAs were performed separately for faces and for bodily postures, with group as categorical predictor and accuracy as dependent variable (for emotion recognition, emotion discrimination and identity discrimination). To investigate the relationship between emotion recognition and alexithymia, Pearson’s correlations were performed for patients, while a multiple linear regression was calculated for the entire group. For MS patients, Pearson’s correlation and a multiple linear regression analysis were carried out to examine the relationship of disease characteristics (disease duration, EDSS) and neuropsychological performance with emotion recognition. Finally, the relationship among alexithymia, fatigue, and depression was tested using Pearson’s correlation. Two additional ANOVAs with two groups (MS, HC) as between factor were performed separately for faces and for bodily postures: an ANOVA on intensity ratings with emotion as within factor, and an ANOVA on reaction times with tasks as within factor. In the RT analysis, trials associated with incorrect responses and those with RTs 2 SD above or below the individual condition mean were discarded. The results of these two analyses can be found in the supplemental materials.

Results

Demographic Information, Screening, and Questionnaires

Groups were matched for gender, age (t(58)=1.14; p=.26), education (t(58)=−0.69; p=.49), intelligent quotient (TIB; t(58)=−1.00; p=.32), and depression (BDI; t(58)=.25; p=.80). Means and standard deviation of demographic variables are detailed in Table 1. Neuropsychological and screening test scores obtained by all MS patients, and the proportion of those who resulted impaired in the different cognitive tests are summarized in Table 2. Table 3 displays questionnaire scores in the patients and control group. No significant differences were found in the TAS-20 score or in its subscale scores between patients and controls.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of patients and controls

Note. Mean values and standard deviations (in brackets) are given for age, education, disease duration, EDSS, TIB, and BDI. F=female; M=male; EDSS=Expanded Disability Status Scale (Kurtzke, Reference Kurtzke1983); TIB=The Short Intelligence Test (Sartori, Lombardi, & Mattiuzzi, Reference Sartori, Lombardi and Mattiuzzi2005); BDI=Beck Depression Inventory (Beck & Beck, Reference Beck and Beck1972).

Table 2. Scores of MS patients on neuropsychological tests

Note. SRT-LTS=Selective Reminding Test-Long Term Storage; SRT-CLTR=Selective Reminding Test-Consistent Long Term Retrieval; SRT-D=Selective Reminding Test-Delayed; SPART=Spatial Recall Test; SPART-D=Spatial Recall Test-Delayed; SDMT=Symbol Digit Modalities Test; PASAT 3=Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 3 seconds; PASAT 2=Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 2 seconds; WLG=Word List Generation; TMT=Trial Making Test; normative data for Digit SPAN (forward and backward) from Monaco, Costa, Caltagirone, and Carlesimo (Reference Monaco, Costa, Caltagirone and Carlesimo2013).

Table 3. Alexithymia in patients and control group

Note: MS=multiple sclerosis; p=p value; BDI=Beck Depression Inventory (Beck & Beck, Reference Beck and Beck1972); FSS=Fatigue Severity Scale (Krupp et al., Reference Krupp, LaRocca, Muir-Nash and Steinberg1989). For each self-report questionnaire, the percentage of participants below the cutoff are reported.

Recognition of Facial Emotions and Emotional Bodily Expressions

Facial expressions

The mixed ANOVA revealed a significant effect of Task, F(2,116)=118.8, p<.001, with higher accuracy for identity discrimination task than for the other tasks, as well as a significant interaction Group×Task, F(2,116)=3.23, p=.04, with patients being less accurate than controls in emotion recognition task (p=.003). The main effect of Group was not significant, F(1,58)=3.55, p=.064 (Figure 1). The mixed ANOVA on accuracy of single emotions revealed a significant effect of Group, F(1,58)=4.16, p=.045, with patients being less accurate than controls, and a significant effect of Emotions, F(5,290)=26.2, p<.001, with some emotions (i.e., happiness, anger, surprise, disgust) being better recognized than others (i.e., fear, sadness); however, the two factors did not interact, F(5,290)=1.05, p=.39. For further information refer to the supplemental materials and Table 4.

Fig. 1 Patients and controls’ accuracy on tasks with facial stimuli.

Table 4. Summary statistics of the tasks involving recognition of facial emotions and emotional bodily expressions

Note. Mean values and standard deviations (in brackets) of experimental tasks.

Bodily expressions

The mixed ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of Task, F(2,116)=84.4, p<.001, with an overall better performance in identity discrimination task than in the other tasks, and a significant effect of Group, F(1,58)=7.46, p=.008, with patients being less accurate than controls. The two factors however did not interact, F(2,116)=.12, p=.88. The mixed ANOVA of single emotions yield a significant main effect of Emotion, F(3,174)=47.8, p<.001, with some emotions (i.e., fear, sadness, and anger) being better recognized than others (happiness), but not a significant main effect of Group, F(1,58)=3.20, p=.08, or interaction, F(3,174)=.83, p=.47. For further information, refer to the supplemental materials and Table 4.

Comparison of patients with lower and higher disability scores

Two groups of patients were identified according to EDSS score: group (a) with a lower disability score (EDSS≤1.5; n=17) and group (b) with a higher disability score (EDSS>1.5; n=13). As represented in Figure 2, an univariate ANOVA showed a significant group effect for both facial emotion recognition, F(2,57)=8.43, p<.005), and facial emotion discrimination, F(2,57)=6.28, p<.005, with patients of group (b) being impaired in both tasks as compared to patients of group (a) (p<.005 and p=.006, respectively) and controls (p<.005 and p=.001, respectively). No differences were detected between patient group (a) and controls (emotion recognition p=.63; emotion discrimination p=.76). As for bodily expressions, the ANOVAs showed a main effect of Group approaching significance for emotion recognition, F(2.57)=2.87, p<.06, and significant for emotion discrimination, F(2,57)=4.36, p<.017. In particular, patients of group (b) were impaired in bodily emotion recognition and discrimination compared to controls (p=.02 and p=.006, respectively), while a difference toward significance is observed compared to patients of group (a) in emotion recognition (p=.07) but not discrimination (p=.24). No difference was observed between patients of group (a) and controls in emotion recognition (p=.70), and emotion discrimination (p=.09). Moreover, differently from facial identity discrimination, in which no differences were observed between the three groups, one way ANOVA F(2,57)=0.59, p=.55, a group effect was observed in bodily identity discrimination, one way ANOVA F(2,57)=3.07, p=.05, with patients of group (b) being more inaccurate than controls (p=.017). None of the remaining differences was significant.

Fig. 2 Accuracy of patients (a – low disability), group (b – high disability) and controls on tasks with facial stimuli (above graph) and with bodily stimuli (below graph).

Relationship between Alexithymia and Emotion Recognition

For patients, Pearson’s correlations indicated that alexithymia did not correlate with facial emotion recognition (r=−0.09; p=.60) or with bodily emotion recognition (r=−0.05; p=.77). A multiple linear regression was calculated to determine whether alexithymia explained the facial and bodily emotion recognition. In the entire group, the proportion of variation in facial emotion recognition accounted for by alexithymia was negligible (2%) and the resulting equation was not significant (F(1,58)=2.24 R2 adjusted=0.02; p=0.14). Similar results were found for facial emotion discrimination (F(1,58)=1.78; R2 adjusted=0.01; p=0.18); bodily emotion recognition (F(1,58)=0.49; R2 adjusted=−0.008; p=.48) and bodily emotion discrimination (F(1,58)=0.68; R2 adjusted=−0.005; p=.41).

Relationship between Emotion Recognition, Disease, and Neuropsychological Performance

Patients’ performance in facial emotion recognition task correlated significantly with age, education, disease duration, EDSS score, and with the following tests of the RAO’s BRB: SRT-LTS, SRT-CLTR, SRT-D, and SDMT, and with TIB test. Facial emotion discrimination task correlated significantly with age, disease duration, EDSS score, and with the following tests of the RAO’s BRB: SRT-D, SPART, and SPART-D. Regarding bodily stimuli, patients’ performance in bodily emotion recognition task correlated significantly with EDSS score and with the following tests of the RAO’s BRB: SDMT, PASAT 3, and PASAT 2. Bodily emotion discrimination task correlated significantly with disease duration and with the following tests of the RAO’s BRB: SRT-D, SPART, SPART-D, and SDMT. Results of Pearson’s correlations extensively reported in Table 5. A multiple linear regression was carried out to determine if deficit in facial emotion recognition was explained by different characteristics of the disease such as disease duration, EDSS score and SDMT, one of the most sensitive tasks to cognitive impairment in MS (see Parmenter, Weinstock-Guttman, Garg, Munschauer, & Benedict, Reference Parmenter, Weinstock-Guttman, Garg, Munschauer and Benedict2007). The variables were entered simultaneously into the analysis. Overall the three variables significantly accounted for the 36.9% of the adjusted facial emotion recognition variance (F=6.66; p=.001) but the effect was due only to SDMT (slope=0.36; B=.23; p=.04). The same analysis conducted on bodily emotion recognition showed that the same three variables accounted for the 24.09% of the adjusted bodily emotion recognition variance (F=4.06; p=.017), but that the effect was due only to EDSS (slope=−0.52; B=−6.16; p=.03).

Table 5 Correlational analysis between the performance on facial emotion recognition task, facial emotion discrimination task, neuropsychological tests and disease characteristics (EDSS, disease duration, BDI, and FSS)

Note. EDSS=Expanded Disability Status Scale (Kurtzke, Reference Kurtzke1983); BDI=Beck Depression Inventory (Beck & Beck, Reference Beck and Beck1972); FSS=Fatigue Severity Scale (Krupp et al., Reference Krupp, LaRocca, Muir-Nash and Steinberg1989); SRT-LTS=Selective Reminding Test-Long Term Storage; SRT-CLTR=Selective Reminding Test-Consistent Long Term Retrieval; SRT-D=Selective Reminding Test-Delayed; SPART=Spatial Recall Test; SPART-D=Spatial Recall Test-Delayed; SDMT=Symbol Digit Modalities Test; Pasat 3=Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 3 seconds; PASAT 2=Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 2 seconds; WLG=Word List Generation (Amato et al., Reference Amato, Portaccio, Goretti, Zipoli, Ricchiuti, De Caro and Trojano2006); Benton=Benton Facial Recognition Task (Benton et al., Reference Benton, Hamsher, Varney and Spreen1983); TIB=The Short Intelligent Test (Sartori et al. Reference Sartori, Lombardi and Mattiuzzi2005); TMT=Trial Making Test (Monaco et al., Reference Monaco, Costa, Caltagirone and Carlesimo2013).

Relationship between Depression, Fatigue, and Alexithymic Features

Alexithymia TAS-20 score correlated significantly with depression measured using the BDI (r=0.51; p=.004). No significant correlations were found between alexithymia and fatigue (r=0.21; p=.305).

Discussion

In the present study, we investigated the ability of patients with MS to recognize emotional facial and bodily expressions controlling for the presence of concurrent alexithymia.

Consistently with previous reports (Henry et al., Reference Henry, Phillips, Beatty, McDonald, Longley, Joscelyne and Rendell2009, Reference Henry, Tourbah, Chaunu, Rumbach, Montreuil and Bakchine2011; Jehna et al., Reference Jehna, Neuper, Petrovic, Wallner-Blazek, Schmidt, Fuchs and Enzinger2010; Krause et al., Reference Krause, Wendt, Dressel, Berneiser, Kessler, Hamm and Lotze2009; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Henry, Scott, Summers, Whyte and Cook2011; Prochnow et al., Reference Prochnow, Donell, Schäfer, Jörgens, Hartung, Franz and Seitz2011), patients with MS recognized significantly fewer facial emotional expressions than controls. However, in contrast with studies that reported a selective impairment for unpleasant emotions such as anger, fear, and sadness in MS (Henry et al., Reference Henry, Phillips, Beatty, McDonald, Longley, Joscelyne and Rendell2009, Reference Henry, Tourbah, Chaunu, Rumbach, Montreuil and Bakchine2011; Prochnow et al., Reference Prochnow, Donell, Schäfer, Jörgens, Hartung, Franz and Seitz2011), the patients in our study were worse at recognizing all facial emotions. These inconsistent results could be accounted by methodological differences across studies. For instance, while in Prochnow et al. (Reference Prochnow, Donell, Schäfer, Jörgens, Hartung, Franz and Seitz2011) the selective impairment for fear, surprise, anger and sadness was found with morph sequences of emotional faces, here we used static emotional facial images in a recognition task.

Moreover, no significant differences between patients with MS and controls were found in tasks involving bodily postures when patients were considered as a group. This result is at variance with the impaired recognition of emotional bodily postures observed in patients with schizophrenia (Van den Stock et al., Reference Van den Stock, de Jong, Hodiamont and de Gelder2011), and Huntington disease (de Gelder et al., 2008) who were often found to be impaired also at recognizing facial emotional expressions. In these studies, the authors proposed that emotion perception mediated by bodies and faces shares common areas, possibly in the amygdala, fusiform gyrus, specific parts of STS, and the parietal lobe (de Gelder et al., Reference de Gelder, Van den Stock, Meeren, Sinke, Kret and Tamietto2010; Hadjikhani et al., 2009). Differently from the abovementioned studies our study assessed both facial and bodily emotion recognition in the same sample of patients thus allowing us to directly test whether facial and bodily emotion recognition share a common mechanism that could be damaged due to the neurological disorder. Of interest, after dividing the patients according to their disability scores, the analysis showed that not only the emotions conveyed by facial expressions but also those conveyed by bodily expressions were more difficult to be recognized by patients with a higher disability score. Moreover, as for faces, correlation analysis revealed that bodily emotion recognition and discrimination significantly correlated with disease duration, disability scores and with some neuropsychological scores. This result suggests that a common mechanism to both facial and bodily expressions may actually be damaged in patients with MS. However, patients with higher disability exhibited difficulties even in the discrimination of bodily identities, and it is not clear whether these difficulties are responsible for the emotion recognition impairment. We speculate that recognizing bodies may be, in general, more difficult than recognizing faces, especially with black/white stimuli. However, future studies should examine this issue more in detail.

Furthermore, the design we used allowed us to clarify whether the integrity of our ability to understand our own emotions is necessary to understand other people’s emotions. Indeed, Pearson’s correlation and multiple regression analysis showed that alexithymia did not explain a lower ability to recognize emotions. Our findings do not support the hypothesis that alexithymia might be a better predictor of the ability to recognize emotional facial expressions (Grynberg et al., Reference Grynberg, Chang, Corneille, Maurage, Vermeulen, Berthoz and Luminet2012). More importantly, they suggest that the ability to understand our mental state is not necessary to understand mental states of others in contrast with what the simulationist models of emotion recognition predict (Barsalou, Reference Barsalou2008; Goldman & Sripada, Reference Goldman and Sripada2005). Our study is not the first to have failed to find an association between alexithymia and emotion recognition in clinical disorders. For instance, Kessler, Schwarze, Filipic, Traue, and von Wietersheim (Reference Kessler, Schwarze, Filipic, Traue and von Wietersheim2006) reported patients with eating disorders who were significantly more alexithymic than controls but showed no emotion recognition deficits. Mann, Wise, Trinidad, and Kohansky (Reference Mann, Wise, Trinidad and Kohansky1995) reported the same finding in substance abusers. Both studies support our hypothesis that alexithymia is not a good predictor of the ability to recognize emotional facial expressions.

There are two possible alternative explanations as to why we failed to observe a clear relation between emotion recognition performance and alexithymia. First, our negative findings might be due to the fact that the emotion recognition tasks we administered were too easy. However, as Prochnow et al. (Reference Prochnow, Donell, Schäfer, Jörgens, Hartung, Franz and Seitz2011) failed to find a significant correlation between alexithymia scored and emotion recognition even though they used ambiguous items such as morphed faces. The second alternative explanation suggests that our negative findings might be due to the characteristics of the patients’ sample, in which a low proportion of patients exhibited alexithymia.

Of interest, we observed that the characteristics specifically associated to the disease such as duration and disability score were significantly associated with bodily and facial emotion recognition and discrimination tasks and that the performance in SDMT, a measure of information processing speed, significantly accounted for facial emotion cognition performance. Moreover, we also showed that emotion recognition and discrimination tasks (both with facial and bodily expressions) significantly correlated with a subset of neuropsychological tests. These results are in line with previous studies on patients with neurodegenerative disorders like Parkinson’s disease. For example, Assogna et al. (Reference Assogna, Pontieri, Cravello, Peppe, Pierantozzi, Stefani and Spalletta2010) observed that emotion recognition correlates with long-term memory, verbal memory, attention, and complex cognitive functions suggesting that recognizing emotions is a complex cognitive process that requires the integrity of several neural circuits. Our results support this view and suggest that impaired facial emotion recognition and, to lesser extent, bodily facial recognition might be a cognitive marker of MS. Accordingly, functional imaging studies have provided clear evidence that patients with MS showed altered brain activation of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPC) in response to emotional stimuli (Jehna et al., Reference Jehna, Langkammer, Wallner-Blazek, Neuper, Loitfelder, Ropele and Enzinger2011; Krause et al., Reference Krause, Wendt, Dressel, Berneiser, Kessler, Hamm and Lotze2009; Passamonti et al., Reference Passamonti, Cerasa, Liguori, Gioia, Valentino, Nisticò and Fera2009). Recently, impaired emotion recognition and theory of mind abilities have been attributed to white matter disconnection and regional cortical atrophy in temporal lobe (Mike et al., Reference Mike, Strammer, Aradi, Orsi, Perlaki, Hajnal and Illes2013).

Consistently with the association between mood and alexithymia observed in studies with MS patients (Bodini et al., Reference Bodini, Mandarelli, Tomassini, Tarsitani, Pestalozza, Gasperini and Pozzilli2008; Gay et al., Reference Gay, Vrignaud, Garitte and Meunier2010), our study shows that alexithymia was associated with depression. In contrast with previous studies (Bodini et al., Reference Bodini, Mandarelli, Tomassini, Tarsitani, Pestalozza, Gasperini and Pozzilli2008), we failed to observe a significant association between alexithymia and fatigue. However, the small sample size of our study could be responsible for negative result.

Our study has some limitations. First, as in most studies, we have used the TAS-20 as the measure of alexithymia. Several concerns have been raised regarding the use of self-report measures to the assessment of alexithymia since alexithymic people could not be aware of their emotional difficulties. Second, our sample size is mainly composed by females (70%) consistently with the disease being twice more common in females than males (Milo & Kahana, Reference Milo and Kahana2010). As several studies suggested that men score higher than woman on alexithymia questionnaires (Levant et al., Reference Levant, Good, Cook, O’Neil, Smalley, Owen and Richmond2006; Levant, Hall, Williams, & Hasan, Reference Levant, Hall, Williams and Hasan2009), our results can reflect the composition of the patients’ sample. Third, our results cannot possibly be extended to all patients with MS. In fact, the population of the present study was characterized by a mild level of disability and a mild level of depression.

In conclusion, the results of this study provide evidence that emotion recognition mediated by facial stimuli and, to lesser extent, by bodily expressions can be impaired in patients with MS. In particular, patients with higher disability showed impairment in emotion recognition as compared to patients with lower disability and matched controls. This deficit seems to be related to the disease’s characteristics and also to some deficit in processing speed and attention, suggesting that impaired emotion recognition might be a general feature of multiple sclerosis. Moreover, our study does not support the hypothesis that alexithymia might be a good predictor of the ability to recognize emotional facial expressions (Grynberg et al., Reference Grynberg, Chang, Corneille, Maurage, Vermeulen, Berthoz and Luminet2012) and suggests that the ability to understand our mental state is not necessary to understand mental states of others in contrast with simulationist models.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all patients who participated in this research. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. There are no conflicts of interest.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617714000939

References

Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1994). Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature, 372(6507), 669672.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Hamann, S., Young, A.W., Calder, A.J., Phelps, E.A., … Damasio, A.R. (1999). Recognition of facial emotion in nine individuals with bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 37(10), 11111117.Google Scholar
Aiello, M., Eleopra, R., Lettieri, C., Mondani, M., D’Auria, S., Belgrado, E., … Rumiati, R. (2014). Emotion recognition in Parkinson’s disease after subthalamic deep brain stimulation: Differential effects of microlesion and STN stimulation. Cortex, 51, 3545.Google Scholar
Amato, M., Portaccio, E., Goretti, B., Zipoli, V., Ricchiuti, L., De Caro, M., … Trojano, M. (2006). The Rao’s Brief Repeatable Battery and Stroop Test: Normative values with age, education and gender corrections in an Italian population. Multiple Sclerosis, 12(6), 787793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assogna, F., Pontieri, F.E., Caltagirone, C., & Spalletta, G. (2008). The recognition of facial emotion expressions in Parkinson's disease. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 18(11), 835848.Google Scholar
Assogna, F., Pontieri, F.E., Cravello, L., Peppe, A., Pierantozzi, M., Stefani, A., … Spalletta, G. (2010). Intensity-dependent facial emotion recognition and cognitive functions in Parkinson's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(5), 867876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baggio, H., Segura, B., Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N., Valldeoriola, F., Marti, M., Compta, Y., … Junque, C. (2012). Structural correlates of facial emotion recognition deficits in Parkinson's disease patients. Neuropsychologia, 50(8), 21212128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakshi, R., Shaikh, Z., Miletich, R., Czarnecki, D., Dmochowski, J., Henschel, K., … Kinkel, P. (2000). Fatigue in multiple sclerosis and its relationship to depression and neurologic disability. Multiple Sclerosis, 6(3), 181185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617645.Google Scholar
Beck, A.T., & Beck, R. W. (1972). Screening depressed patients in a family practive: A rapid technique. Postgraduate Medicine, 52, 4.Google Scholar
Benton, A., Hamsher, K., Varney, N., & Spreen, O. (1983). Contributions to neuropsychology assessment: A clinical manual. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Besharat, M.A., Pourhosein, R., Rostami, R., & Bazzazian, S. (2011). Perfectionism and fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Psychology & Health, 26(4), 419432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodini, B., Mandarelli, G., Tomassini, V., Tarsitani, L., Pestalozza, I., Gasperini, C., … Pozzilli, C. (2008). Alexithymia in multiple sclerosis: Relationship with fatigue and depression. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 118(1), 1823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bressi, C., Taylor, G., Parker, J., Bressi, S., Brambilla, V., Aguglia, E., … Bucca, M. (1996). Cross validation of the factor structure of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: an Italian multicenter study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41(6), 551559.Google Scholar
Brown, R., Tennant, C., Sharrock, M., Hodgkinson, S., Dunn, S., & Pollard, J. (2006). Relationship between stress and relapse in multiple sclerosis: Part II. Direct and indirect relationships. Multiple Sclerosis, 12(4), 465475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chahraoui, K., Pinoit, J.-M., Viegas, N., Adnet, J., Bonin, B., & Moreau, T. (2008). Alexithymie et liens avec la dépression et l’anxiété dans la sclérose en plaques. Revue Neurologique, 164(3), 242245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiaravalloti, N., & DeLuca, J. (2008). Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurology, 7(12), 11391151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compston, A., & Coles, A. (2002). Multiple sclerosis. Lancet, 359(9313), 12211231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Gelder, B. (2006). Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language. Nature reviews, 7(3), 242249.Google Scholar
de Gelder, B., Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., & Hadjikhani, N. (2004). Fear fosters flight: a mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(47), 1670116706.Google Scholar
de Gelder, B., Van den Stock, J., Balaguer, R. d. D., & Bachoud-Lévi, A.C. (2008). Huntington’s disease impairs recognition of angry and instrumental body language. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 369373.Google Scholar
de Gelder, B., & Van den Stock, J. (2011). The Bodily Expressive Action Stimulus Test (BEAST). Construction and validation of a stimulus basis for measuring perception of whole body expression of emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 181.Google Scholar
de Gelder, B., Van den Stock, J., Meeren, H., Sinke, C., Kret, M., & Tamietto, M. (2010). Standing up for the body. Recent progress in uncovering the networks involved in the perception of bodies and bodily expressions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(4), 513527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Bitonto, L., Longato, N., Jung, B., Fleury, M., Marcel, C., Collongues, N., … Blanc, F. (2011). [Reduced emotional reactivity to negative stimuli in multiple sclerosis, preliminary results]. Revue Neurologique, 167(11), 820826.Google Scholar
Feinstein, A., DeLuca, J., Baune, B.T., Filippi, M., & Lassman, H. (2013). Cognitive and neuropsychiatric disease manifestations in MS. Multiple Sclerosis and Related Disorders, 2(1), 412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gay, M.C., Vrignaud, P., Garitte, C., & Meunier, C. (2010). Predictors of depression in multiple sclerosis patients. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 121(3), 161170.Google Scholar
Ghosh, B., Rowe, J., Calder, A., Hodges, J., & Bak, T. (2009). Emotion recognition in progressive supranuclear palsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 80(10), 11431145.Google Scholar
Giovagnoli, A.R., Del Pesce, M., Mascheroni, S., Simoncelli, M., Laiacona, M., & Capitani, E. (1996). Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls. The Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 17(4), 305309.Google Scholar
Goldman, A., & Sripada, C. (2005). Simulationist models of face-based emotion recognition. Cognition, 94(3), 193213.Google Scholar
Grynberg, D., Chang, B., Corneille, O., Maurage, P., Vermeulen, N., Berthoz, S., & Luminet, O. (2012). Alexithymia and the processing of emotional facial expressions (EFEs): systematic review, unanswered questions and further perspectives. PloS One, 7(8), e42429.Google Scholar
Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R.M., Manoach, D.S., Naik, P., Young, A., Snyder, J., Dominick, K., Hoge, R., Van den Stock, J., … de Gelder, B. (2009). Body expressions of emotion do not trigger fear contagion in autism spectrum disorder. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 4(1), 7078.Google Scholar
Hall, J., Harris, J., Sprengelmeyer, R., Sprengelmeyer, A., Young, A., Santos, I., … Lawrie, S. (2004). Social cognition and face processing in schizophrenia. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 169170.Google Scholar
Henry, A., Tourbah, A., Chaunu, M.-P., Rumbach, L., Montreuil, M., & Bakchine, S. (2011). Social cognition impairments in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(6), 11221131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J., Phillips, L., Beatty, W., McDonald, S., Longley, W., Joscelyne, A., & Rendell, P. (2009). Evidence for deficits in facial affect recognition and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(2), 277285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jehna, M., Langkammer, C., Wallner-Blazek, M., Neuper, C., Loitfelder, M., Ropele, S., … Enzinger, C. (2011). Cognitively preserved MS patients demonstrate functional differences in processing neutral and emotional faces. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 5(4), 241251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jehna, M., Neuper, C., Petrovic, K., Wallner-Blazek, M., Schmidt, R., Fuchs, S., … Enzinger, C. (2010). An exploratory study on emotion recognition in patients with a clinically isolated syndrome and multiple sclerosis. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 112(6), 482484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, H., Schwarze, M., Filipic, S., Traue, H.C., & von Wietersheim, J. (2006). Alexithymia and facial emotion recognition in patients with eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 39(3), 245251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krause, M., Wendt, J., Dressel, A., Berneiser, J., Kessler, C., Hamm, A., & Lotze, M. (2009). Prefrontal function associated with impaired emotion recognition in patients with multiple sclerosis. Behavioural Brain Research, 205(1), 280285.Google Scholar
Kret, M., Stekelenburg, J., Roelofs, K., & de Gelder, B. (2013). Perception of face and body expressions using electromyography, pupillometry and gaze measures. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krupp, L.B., LaRocca, N.G., Muir-Nash, J., & Steinberg, A.D. (1989). The fatigue severity scale: application to patients with multiple sclerosis and systemic lupus erythematosus. Archives of Neurology, 46(10), 11211123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurtzke, J. (1983). Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: An expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology, 33(11), 14441452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levant, R.F., Good, G.E., Cook, S.W., O’Neil, J.M., Smalley, K.B., Owen, K., & Richmond, K. (2006). The normative Male Alexithymia Scale: Measurement of a gender-linked syndrome. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 7(4), 212.Google Scholar
Levant, R.F., Hall, R.J., Williams, C.M., & Hasan, N.T. (2009). Gender differences in alexithymia. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 10(3), 190.Google Scholar
Mann, L.S., Wise, T.N., Trinidad, A., & Kohansky, R. (1995). Alexithymia, affect recognition, and five factors of personality in substance abusers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81(1), 3540.Google Scholar
McCade, D., Savage, G., & Naismith, S.L. (2012). Review of emotion recognition in mild cognitive impairment. Dementia and geriatric cognitive disorders, 32(4), 257266.Google Scholar
McDonald, W.I., Compston, A., Edan, G., Goodkin, D., Hartung, H., Lublin, F.D., … Wolinsky, J.S. (2001). Recommended diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: guidelines from the International Panel on the diagnosis of multiple sclerosis. Annals of Neurology, 50(1), 121127.Google Scholar
Mike, A., Strammer, E., Aradi, M., Orsi, G., Perlaki, G., Hajnal, A., … Illes, Z. (2013). Disconnection mechanism and regional cortical atrophy contribute to impaired processing of facial expressions and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis: a structural MRI study. PloS One, 8(12), e82422.Google Scholar
Milo, R., & Kahana, E. (2010). Multiple sclerosis: geoepidemiology, genetics and the environment. Autoimmunity Reviews, 9(5), A387A394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monaco, M., Costa, A., Caltagirone, C., & Carlesimo, G. (2013). Forward and backward span for verbal and visuo-spatial data: standardization and normative data from an Italian adult population. Neurological Sciences, 34(5), 749754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Namiki, C., Hirao, K., Yamada, M., Hanakawa, T., Fukuyama, H., Hayashi, T., & Murai, T. (2007). Impaired facial emotion recognition and reduced amygdalar volume in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 156(1), 2332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narme, P., Mouras, H., Roussel, M., Duru, C., Krystkowiak, P., & Godefroy, O. (2013). Emotional and cognitive social processes are impaired in Parkinson's disease and are related to behavioral disorders. Neuropsychology, 27(2), 182192.Google Scholar
Nemiah, J., Freyberger, H., & Sifneos, P. (1976). Alexithymia: a view of the psychosomatic process. Modern Trends in Psychosomatic Medicine, 3, 430439.Google Scholar
Novelli, G., Papagno, C., Capitani, E., & Laiacona, M. (1986). Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale. Taratura su sogetti normali. Archivio di Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria, 47(4), 477506.Google Scholar
Nyenhuis, D.L., Rao, S.M., Zajecka, J.M., Luchetta, T., Bernardin, L., & Garron, D.C. (1995). Mood disturbance versus other sympthoms of depression in multiple sclerosis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 1(3), 291296.Google Scholar
Orsini, A., Grossi, D., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1987). Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: normative data from 1355 adults and 1112 children. The Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 8(6), 537548.Google Scholar
Parmenter, B., Weinstock-Guttman, B., Garg, N., Munschauer, F., & Benedict, R.H. (2007). Screening for cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis using the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Multiple Sclerosis, 13(1), 5257.Google Scholar
Passamonti, L., Cerasa, A., Liguori, M., Gioia, M., Valentino, P., Nisticò, R., … Fera, F. (2009). Neurobiological mechanisms underlying emotional processing in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Brain, 132(Pt 12), 33803391.Google Scholar
Phillips, L., Henry, J., Scott, C., Summers, F., Whyte, M., & Cook, M. (2011). Specific impairments of emotion perception in multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychology, 25(1), 131136.Google Scholar
Pinto, C., Gomes, F., Moreira, I., Rosa, B., Santos, E., Silva, A.M., & Cavaco, S. (2012). Emotion recognition in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Eye Tracking, Visual Cognition and Emotion, 2(1), 7681.Google Scholar
Pittion-Vouyovitch, S., Debouverie, M., Guillemin, F., Vandenberghe, N., Anxionnat, R., & Vespignani, H. (2006). Fatigue in multiple sclerosis is related to disability, depression and quality of life. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 243(1--2), 3945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pontieri, F.E., Assogna, F., Stefani, A., Pierantozzi, M., Meco, G., Benincasa, D., & Spalletta, G. (2012). Sad and happy facial emotion recognition impairment in progressive supranuclear palsy in comparison with Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 18(7), 871875.Google Scholar
Prochnow, D., Donell, J., Schäfer, R., Jörgens, S., Hartung, H., Franz, M., & Seitz, R. (2011). Alexithymia and impaired facial affect recognition in multiple sclerosis. Journal of Neurology, 258(9), 16831688.Google Scholar
Quaranta, D., Marra, C., Zinno, M., Patanella, A.K., Messina, M.J., Piccininni, C., & Gainotti, G. (2012). Presentation and validation of the multiple sclerosis depression rating scale: a test specifically devised to investigate affective disorders in multiple sclerosis patients. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26(4), 571587.Google Scholar
Robotham, L., Sauter, D.A., Bachoud-Lévi, A.-C., & Trinkler, I. (2011). The impairment of emotion recognition in Huntington’s disease extends to positive emotions. Cortex, 47(7), 880884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sartori, G., Lombardi, L., & Mattiuzzi, L. (2005). Semantic relevance best predicts normal and abnormal name retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 43(5), 754770.Google Scholar
Schwartz, C., & Frohner, R. (2005). Contribution of demographic, medical, and social support variables in predicting the mental health dimension of quality of life among people with multiple sclerosis. Health & Social Work, 30(3), 203212.Google Scholar
Solari, A., Filippini, G., Mendozzi, L., Ghezzi, A., Cifani, S., Barbieri, E., … Mosconi, P. (1999). Validation of Italian multiple sclerosis quality of life 54 questionnaire. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry, 67(2), 158162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spoletini, I., Marra, C., Iulio, F.D., Gianni, W., Sancesario, G., Giubilei, F., … Spalletta, G. (2008). Facial emotion recognition deficit in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16(5), 389398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sprengelmeyer, R., Schroeder, U., Young, A., & Epplen, J. (2006). Disgust in pre-clinical Huntington's disease: A longitudinal study. Neuropsychologia, 44(4), 518533.Google Scholar
Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A., Mahn, K., Schroeder, U., Woitalla, D., Büttner, T., & Przuntek, H. (2003). Facial expression recognition in people with medicated and unmedicated Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 41(8), 10471057.Google Scholar
Tamietto, M., Geminiani, G., Genero, R., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Seeing fearful body language overcomes attentional deficits in patients with neglect. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 19(3), 445454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J., Leon, A., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T., … Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242249.Google Scholar
Tsui, C.F., Huang, J., Lui, S.S.Y., Au, A.C.W., Leung, M.M.W., Cheung, E.F.C., & Chan, R.C.K. (2013). Facial emotion perception abnormality in patients with early schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 147(2), 230235.Google Scholar
Van den Stock, J., van de Riet, W., Righart, R., & de Gelder, B. (2008). Neural correlates of perceiving emotional faces and bodies in developmental prosopagnosia: an event-related fMRI-study. PloS one, 3(9), e3195.Google Scholar
Van den Stock, J., de Jong, S. J., Hodiamont, P.P., & de Gelder, B. (2011). Perceiving emotions from bodily expressions and multisensory integration of emotion cues in schizophrenia. Social Neuroscience, 6(5--6), 537547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of patients and controls

Figure 1

Table 2. Scores of MS patients on neuropsychological tests

Figure 2

Table 3. Alexithymia in patients and control group

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Patients and controls’ accuracy on tasks with facial stimuli.

Figure 4

Table 4. Summary statistics of the tasks involving recognition of facial emotions and emotional bodily expressions

Figure 5

Fig. 2 Accuracy of patients (a – low disability), group (b – high disability) and controls on tasks with facial stimuli (above graph) and with bodily stimuli (below graph).

Figure 6

Table 5 Correlational analysis between the performance on facial emotion recognition task, facial emotion discrimination task, neuropsychological tests and disease characteristics (EDSS, disease duration, BDI, and FSS)

Supplementary material: File

Cecchetto Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material

Download Cecchetto Supplementary Material(File)
File 14.9 KB