Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-5r2nc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-06T09:12:48.605Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Decreased delta sleep ratio and elevated alpha power predict vulnerability to depression during interferon-alpha treatment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 December 2014

Francis E. Lotrich*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinics, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center; Pittsburgh, PA, USA
Anne Germain
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinics, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center; Pittsburgh, PA, USA
*
Dr. Francis E. Lotrich, Department of Psychiatry, Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinics, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, 3811 Ohara Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, USA. Tel: +1 412 246 6267; Fax: +1 412 246 6260; E-mail: lotrichfe@upmc.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Although poor sleep accompanies depression, it is unknown which specific sleep abnormalities precede depression. This is similarly the case for depression developing during interferon-α (IFN-α) therapy. Because vulnerability becomes evident in those who slept poorly before IFN-α, we prospectively determined which specific aspect of sleep could predict subsequent depression.

Methods

Two nights of polysomnography with quantitative electroencephalogram (EEG) were obtained in 24 adult, euthymic subjects – all subsequently treated with IFN-α for hepatitis C. Every 2 weeks, a Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) score was obtained, and the maximal increase in BDI-II from pre-treatment baseline – excluding the sleep question – was determined.

Results

The delta sleep ratio (DSR; an index of early-night restorative delta power) was inversely associated with BDI-II increases (p<0.01), as was elevated alpha power (8–12 Hz; p<0.001). Both delta (0.5–4 Hz) and alpha power exhibited high between-night correlations (r=0.83 and 0.92, respectively). In mixed-effect repeated-measure analyses, there was an interaction between alpha power and DSR (p<0.001) – subjects with low alpha power and elevated DSR were resilient to developing depression. Most other sleep parameters – including total sleep time and percentage of time in slow wave sleep – were not associated with subsequent changes in depression.

Conclusions

Both high DSR and low alpha power may be specific indices of resilience. As most other aspects of sleep were not associated with resilience or vulnerability, sleep interventions to prevent depression may need to specifically target these specific sleep parameters.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
© Scandinavian College of Neuropsychopharmacology 2014 

Significant outcomes

  • In non-depressed people, elevated alpha power during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep and a low delta sleep ratio (DSR) both predict subsequent increases in depression symptoms during interferon-alpha (IFN-α) therapy.

  • These quantitative measures of electroencephalogram (EEG) power are both highly correlated between nights.

Limitations

  • The sample size is fairly small, and serial EEG measures were not obtained during IFN-α therapy.

  • The results may be specific for depression occurring during IFN-α therapy, and generalisation to other types of depression is unknown.

Introduction

The incidence of a major depressive disorder (MDD) episode is usually <2%/year (Reference Patten1,Reference Eaton, Kramer, Anthony, Dryman, Shapiro and Locke2), even in medically ill populations. An MDD episode occurs in <5–10% of people over a several year period (Reference Patten and Lee3,Reference Kruijt, Antypa and Booij4), and in only about 12% the first year after a cancer diagnosis (Reference Boyes, Girgis, D'Este, Zucca, Lecathelinais and Carey5). Treatment with high doses of IFN-α results in an incidence of about 25% in the first few months (Reference Udina, Castellvi and Moreno-Espana6). In each of these situations, most people are resilient – with only a minority exhibiting vulnerability to the development of depression. As depression is a major cause of disability and premature death, replicating this resilience in vulnerable people and thereby preventing depression would be of great consequence.

Because poor sleep frequently precedes the future development of MDD (Reference Breslau, Roth, Rosenthal and Andreski7Reference Roane and Taylor10), it may be possible to prevent depression by correcting indices of sleep. Nonetheless, it is not clear which sleep abnormality might represent a pre-existing vulnerability to subsequent MDD. Cross-sectional studies find a number of objective sleep parameters that are associated with MDD including sleep continuity disturbances (prolonged sleep latency, nocturnal arousals, and early morning awakening), diminished slow wave sleep (SWS), shortened rapid eye movement (REM) latency, decreased DSR, among others (Reference Emslie, Armitage, Weinberg, Rush, Mayes and Hoffmann11Reference Wichniak, Wierzbicka and Jernajczyk17). However, to our knowledge there are no studies that have examined which of these parameters precede or antedate MDD.

To prospectively address which aspect of ‘poor’ sleep precedes depression, we have therefore employed quantitative polysomnography (PSG) in non-depressed, euthymic individuals who are intending to start IFN-α therapy. IFN-α treatment-related MDD has an incidence of about 25% in the first few months (Reference Udina, Castellvi and Moreno-Espana6,Reference Felger and Lotrich18), thereby permitting examination of subjects before they start IFN-α treatment – and before they develop MDD. Consistent with other instances of MDD, poor sleep quality before IFN-α therapy strongly predicts risk for developing MDD during IFN-α treatment (Reference Franzen, Buysse, Rabinovitz, Pollock and Lotrich19), even when controlling for baseline depression and/or history of prior depression. In time-lagged hierarchical regression during IFN-α therapy, subjectively poor sleep is a strong predictor of increased Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) scores 1 month later though not vice versa (Reference Prather, Rabinvitz, Pollock and Lotrich20), further demonstrating that poor self-reported sleep quality precedes depression in this population.

We focused our initial attention on delta power (0.5–4 Hz) during NREM sleep. Delta power is an index of the restorative function of sleep (Reference Campbell, Darchia and Higgins21Reference Dworak, McCarley, Kim, Kalinchuk and Basheer23). Delta waves propagate across the cortex during sleep, and consist of large currents in the medial frontal gyrus, the inferior frontal gyrus, the anterior cingulate, the precuneus, and the posterior cingulate (Reference Murphy, Riedner, Huber, Massimini, Ferrarelli and Tononi24). In positron emission tomography studies, delta power during NREM sleep is highly correlated with decreased metabolic activity in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, and orbitofrontal regions (Reference Dang-Vu, Desseilles and Laureys25,Reference Nofzinger26) – regions implicated in MDD. Conversely, delta power is diminished during periods of elevated stress (Reference Hall, Thayer and Germain27) and the normally reduced metabolic activity during NREM sleep is likewise abnormal during depression (Reference Germain, Nofzinger, Kupfer and Buysse28). It is therefore plausible that disrupting NREM-related restorative processes increases vulnerability to subsequent MDD precipitating insults.

The majority of restorative delta power normally occurs during the first NREM period, with exponentially decreasing delta power in subsequent NREM periods. A basic index of this decrease in delta power over the night is the ratio of delta waves during the first NREM period to the second NREM period – the DSR. A lower DSR is consistently present in MDD, where DSR is about 1.6 in normal subjects compared with 1.1 in depressed subjects (Reference Armitage, Hoffmann, Trivedi and Rush13,Reference Kupfer, Reynolds, Ulrich and Grochocinski29). Importantly, DSR in euthymic individuals is a robust predictor of subsequent MDD relapse after cognitive therapy (Reference Kupfer, Frank, McEachran and Grochocinski16,Reference Spanier, Frank, McEachran, Grochocinski and Kupfer30), after interpersonal psychotherapy (Reference Buysse, Frank, Lowe, Cherry and Kupfer31), and/or during antidepressant maintenance therapy (Reference Reynolds, Buysse and Brunner32). Because low DSR has been replicated as a risk for depression relapse, our primary hypothesis was that low DSR would be associated with risk for inflammation-related depression.

Another aspect of MDD that has been repeatedly observed is shortened REM latency (Reference Steiger and Kimura14,Reference Kupfer, Reynolds, Ulrich and Grochocinski29,Reference Lauer, Schreiber, Holsboer and Krieg33). The pressure for REM to occur earlier may shorten the duration of the initial NREM period (Reference Buysse, Hall and Tu12), and thereby affect DSR. Finally, quantitative EEGs (qEEGs) were used to determine if the per cent of activity in particular frequencies would be associated with depression risk. In addition to delta power, we examined the predictive roles of elevated alpha power (Reference Fenzl, Touma and Romanowski34,Reference Nitschke, Heller, Etienne and Miller35), as well as theta (Reference von Stein and Sarnthein36), sigma, and beta power (Reference Bjorvatn, Fagerland and Ursin37). Elevations in some higher frequencies may reflect, in part, hyperarousal during sleep (Reference Nofzinger, Price and Meltzer38,Reference Krystal and Edinger39). Intrusion of these other frequencies could also adversely affect delta power.

The aspect of sleep that antedates development of MDD has consequences for prevention strategies. Different insomnia treatments typically have different effects on sleep parameters. As examples, benzodiazepines may improve sleep latency but can decrease delta power and increase beta power (Reference Bastien, LeBlanc, Carrier and Morin40,Reference Feinberg, Maloney and Campbell41); serotonin reuptake inhibitors suppress REM sleep (Reference Dumont, de Visser, Cohen and van Gerven42,Reference Silvestri, Pace-Schott, Gersh, Stickgold, Salzman and Hobson43); mindfulness meditation improves sleep continuity but can decrease SWS (Reference Britton, Haynes, Fridel and Bootzin44); zolpidem decreases alpha power with limited effects on SWS while gaboxadol improves delta power (Reference Lundahl, Deacon, Maurice and Staner45); and agomelatine improves sleep latency with minimal effect on REM latency, while escitalopram improves REM latency with minimal effect on sleep latency (Reference Quera-Salva, Hajak and Philip46). There are a large variety of interventions available to improve sleep. Which of these multiple and various sleep interventions affect parameters that precede depression and could therefore be examined for prevention of depression?

Methods

All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008, and as approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board. A total of 24 non-depressed adult subjects with chronic hepatitis C were screened to not have active mood disorders (currently or in the past 6 months), psychotic disorders, or substance abuse, using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV) (Reference First, Spitzer and Williams47). The SCID-IV was performed by a psychiatrist or by a trained research assistant and then reviewed with the psychiatrist. When a urine drug screen was available, this was used to confirm SCID-IV findings. Importantly, use of any hypnotic agents was a reason for exclusion. Exclusion criteria also included known obstructive sleep apnoea, active inflammatory illness requiring corticosteroid treatment, endocrinopathy (such as hypothyroidism), neurologic disorder such as tumour or epilepsy, and pregnancy (or other medical contra-indication to IFN-α therapy). In this highly screened group, subjects had an average cumulate illness rating scale scores <4 (Table 1) consistent with few medical co-morbidities other than hepatitis C virus (HCV).

Table 1 Demographics and polysomnography results include the body mass index (BMI), cumulative illness rating scale for geriatrics (CIRS-G), Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II), Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS), diary self-reports of sleep, and quantitative EEG results

PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index.

The linear correlation coefficient (R) with maximal change in BDI-II scores is given (bold with asterisk indicates p<0.05).

Baseline BDI-II (Reference Beck, Steer and Garbin48) scores were obtained before starting IFN-α therapy, and then every 2 weeks after starting therapy. Depression vulnerability was quantified as the maximal increase in BDI-II score for each individual during treatment. Because subjects could be started on antidepressant treatment if they developed a major depressive episode, the maximal change in BDI-II provides a quantitative measure that is minimally confounded by subsequent alleviation of depression symptoms by medication. Participants also completed the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) (Reference Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman and Kupfer49). The PSQI is a well-validated 18-item self-report measure of global sleep quality. Scores below 5 reflect good sleep quality (Reference Hayashino, Yamazaki, Takegami, Nakayama, Sokejima and Fukuhara50), but a cut-off >7 has been proposed to indicate poor sleep in chronically ill samples (Reference Owen, Parker and McGuire51,Reference Carpenter and Andrykowski52).

A 2-week sleep diary (Reference Monk, Reynolds and Kupfer53), to be completed each morning upon awakening, was requested from subjects before PSG. The diary provided prospective measures of sleep latency, wake time after sleep onset, total sleep duration, and sleep efficiency (i.e. ratio of total sleep duration/total time spent in bed). All subjects then completed a first night of PSG before IFN-α therapy to screen for sleep disorders such as sleep apnoea, and to provide habituation to the PSG environment. PSG was performed at the Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic using GrassTelefactor M15 bipolar Neurodata amplifiers and locally developed collection software (Reference Doman, Detka and Hoffman54). Exclusion criteria included an apnoea-hypopnea index >15 and/or index of periodic leg movements with arousal (PLMA-I) >15. After this initial screening night and after at least one intervening night at home, a second PSG night was conducted. The recording montage consisted of bilateral central EEG leads referenced to A1+A2; right and left electro-oculogram referenced to A1+A2; and bipolar submentalis electromyogram. EEG data were acquired at a rate of 256 Hz and decimated to 128 Hz. High frequency EEG artefacts were excluded in 4-s bins with a previously validated algorithm (Reference Brunner, Vasko, Detka, Monahan, Reynolds and Kupfer55). EEG spectra for each artefact-free 4-s epoch were then aligned with 20-s visually scored sleep stage data to exclude epochs scored as awake or REM sleep. Scoring was performed by trained PSG technicians who maintained a high level of scoring reliability, as indicated by mean κ values of >0.80 for various sleep stages, use standard criteria and Stellate Harmonie software.

qEEG power values from artefact-free 4-s epochs were averaged over NREM periods in 0.5 Hz bins before modelling and analysis. DSR was operationalised as the average number of delta counts per minute in the first NREM period divided by the number of delta counts per minute in the second period (Reference Kupfer, Frank, McEachran and Grochocinski16). Other sleep variables included average and relative power at various frequencies [0.5–4 Hz (delta), 4–8 Hz (theta), 8–12 Hz (alpha), 12–16 Hz (sigma), and 20–32 Hz (beta)], total sleep duration (from diary self-report and from PSG), sleep latency (from diary and from time from beginning of the recording period to the first of 10 consecutive minutes of stage 2 or stages 3–4 sleep interrupted by no more than 2 min of stage 1 or wakefulness), sleep efficiency (time spent asleep/total recording period×100), percentage of time in stages 1, 2, and SWS (sum of sleep stages 3 and 4), REM latency (time between sleep onset and the first REM period with ≥3 consecutive minutes of REM sleep), average REM counts, and REM density.

To avoid confounding, the sleep question was excluded from the BDI-II total scores used in the analyses. Using SPSS 20.0, we used linear regression to examine the correlation of the maximal change in BDI-II with individual sleep variables. To ensure the validity of these regressions, we ensured that the standardised residuals were normally distributed. The role of DSR was our primary hypothesis, with the other sleep variables being exploratory (statistical corrections for repeated testing were not applied). Subsequent repeated-measure exploratory analyses of BDI-II scores employed mixed-effect analyses with unstructured covariances. Results are presented as mean±standard deviation.

Results

Subjects were euthymic (not depressed) before starting IFN-α therapy as evidenced in both BDI-II and MADRS scores (Table 1), although overall self-reported sleep quality was typically poor (average PSQI>7). This was similar to prior studies of hepatitis patients (Reference Franzen, Buysse, Rabinovitz, Pollock and Lotrich19,Reference Prather, Rabinvitz, Pollock and Lotrich20). Sleep efficiency based on diary self-reports was similar to that calculated by PSG observations; and spectral power was remarkably similar across the two nights of testing (Table 1). For subsequent analyses, the two nights were therefore averaged. Typical in this population, 25% had a past history of a major depressive episode in remission; 45% had a history of drug abuse/dependence in remission; and 45% had a history of alcohol abuse/dependence in remission. The average time of abstinent remission was 9.5 months. One subject also had a history of panic disorder in remission, and one had a history of post-traumatic stress disorder in remission. None had a history of bipolar disorder, psychotic disorder, or obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Excluding the sleep question, BDI-II increased during IFN-α therapy with a maximal change that averaged 9.4±6.8 (range: −1 to 23). The maximal increase in BDI-II was not associated with a history of prior MDD (p=0.74), past alcohol abuse/dependence (p=0.34), nor past drug abuse/dependence (p=0.94). Diary self-report measures did not correlate with subsequent maximal change in BDI-II, nor did sleep efficiency or minutes spent in the various stages of sleep (Table 1). REM latency also was not predictive of subsequent depression (Table 1). However, as hypothesised, pre-treatment DSR was negatively correlated with the maximal increase in BDI-II during INF-α treatment (B=−0.59±0.025; t=−2.3; p=0.03), as was lower relative delta power overall (Table 1). Interestingly, sleep latency was also negatively correlated with maximal BDI-II increase, indicating that possibly a greater sleep drive and need for sleep (and therefore falling asleep more quickly) was predictive of future depression. In subsequent exploration of the power spectral results, elevated relative alpha and relative sigma power (but not higher beta frequencies) were also predictive of increased BDI-II (Table 1). In this exploration, Bonferroni correction was not performed, but for 35 independent tests would require p<0.0014.

DSR correlated with several other sleep variables including REM latency (r 2=0.42; p=0.009), alpha power (r 2=0.47; p<0.0001) and sigma power (r 2=0.34; p=0.003). Because of these multiple inter-correlations among sleep parameters, each of the sleep variables that were associated with maximal BDI-II increase was therefore included in stepwise (forward and backward) regression analyses. The only variable that remained associated with maximal change in BDI-II in the stepwise analyses was relative alpha power, where r 2=0.38; p<0.002 (Fig. 1). The pattern was similar for sigma power, but it was no longer significantly associated in stepwise regression. DSR also lost significance when including alpha power in the model.

Fig. 1 Pre-treatment alpha power is predictive of subsequently increased Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) scores.

To further explore the relationship between alpha power and DSR on depression vulnerability, we next dichotomised both sleep parameters (using a median split for each) and used both in repeated measure mixed-effect analysis of BDI-II scores. Both DSR and max-change in BDI-II were normally distributed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test), but alpha power was not. We focused on the first couple months of IFN-α therapy before any antidepressant interventions or treatment interruption could confound the data. Both DSR and alpha power were associated with increasing BDI-II over time (p<0.001 for each). Importantly, there was a significant interaction between time, DSR, and alpha power [F(13,511)=228; p<0.0001]. As seen in Fig. 2, subjects are resilient to depression when both DSR and alpha power are both low. However, when alpha power was high, then subjects developed depression regardless of DSR. Results were similar when using log-transformed alpha power.

Fig. 2 When relative alpha power is low (upper panel), then a high delta sleep ratio (DSR; filled circles) is protective against depression compared with low DSR (open circles). When relative alpha power is high (lower panel), then depression worsens regardless of DSR.

Discussion

Only a few sleep parameters in non-depressed individuals were associated with future risk for increased depression symptoms during IFN-α therapy. Neither sleep efficiency nor REM latency were associated with future risk for depression. Total sleep time or the time spent in any particular stage of sleep was also not predictive. But we confirmed our primary hypothesis that low DSR would predict worsening depression, and additionally observed that relative power in the 8–16 Hz range was also predictive. Thus, standard PSG was not able to predict vulnerability to IFN-α, but qEEG was useful in this regard.

DSR is believed to index the homeostatic drive and restorative function of delta sleep (Reference Campbell, Darchia and Higgins21Reference Dworak, McCarley, Kim, Kalinchuk and Basheer23). Normally, this homeostatic sleep drive should exponentially dissipate through the night, with decreasing delta power in each successive NREM period. Conversely, both alpha and sigma power are considered to be markers of hyperarousal during sleep and can be elevated during depression (Reference Lange56). Alpha activity is associated with arousal-like states that are more easily interrupted by noise – and potentially worse subjective sleep quality (Reference McKinney, Dang-Vu, Buxton, Solet and Ellenbogen57). Highly stress-reactive mice have elevated alpha activity (Reference Fenzl, Touma and Romanowski34), as do children with family histories of alcohol abuse (Reference Dahl, Williamson, Bertocci, Stolz, Ryan and Ehlers58). Consistent with this, patients with gastroesophageal reflux disease have lower delta and higher alpha activity (Reference Budhiraja, Quan, Punjabi, Drake, Dickman and Fass59). Likewise in patients with back pain, more central alpha activity is noted in depressed patients (Reference Harman, Pivik, D'Eon, Wilson, Swenson and Matsunaga60).

Related to the role of hyperarousal, decreased activation of the reticular formation may be necessary for hyperpolarisation of thalamocortical neurons, decreased sensory input to the cortex, and the development of delta waves (Reference Steriade and McCarley61). Stress, potentially contributing to hyperarousal, can increase alpha activity along with decreases in delta power (Reference Hall, Thayer and Germain27). However, the cross-sectional correlation between DSR and alpha power that we observed is unable to prove whether hyperarousal processes could have been causally impairing delta power or vice versa.

Nonetheless, there are known differences in the hyperarousal observed in patients diagnosed with insomnia versus those with MDD (Reference Staner, Cornette and Maurice62). People with simple insomnia may have a primary problem of hyperarousal without evidence for a primary dysfunctional homeostatic drive (i.e. a low DSR). In addition, insomnia tends to also be associated with increases in beta power as well (Reference Hall, Thayer and Germain27,Reference Merica, Blois and Gaillard63). We found no evidence for beta or theta power on depression risk, nor did we find an affect of sleep efficiency or waking after sleep onset (by either diary self-report or PSG). It is therefore unlikely that overt sleep fragmentation with awakening affected depression risk. Our findings appear to be more specific to the 8–16 Hz range during NREM sleep; and do not support the possibility that at-risk people with poor sleep quality simply had a primary insomnia.

Moreover, we observed that subjects with both alpha power <6.5% (below the median) as well as DSR >1.4 (above the median) were resilient to any changes in depression scores during IFN-α therapy, indicating that both aspects of sleep are important for protection from depression risk. When either DSR was below the median (<1.4) or alpha power was above the median (>6.5%), then BDI-II increased during IFN-α therapy. Related to this, an increase in delta power with a reduction in both alpha and sigma is good predictor of antidepressant response (Reference Luthringer, Minot, Toussaint, Calvi-Gries, Schaltenbrand and Macher64). Thus, it may be the combination of both hyperarousal and impaired homeostatic sleep drive that could lead to depression vulnerability.

Notably, variation in sleep architecture, including delta power, appears to have a very strong genetic influence (Reference Linkowski65,Reference Ambrosius, Lietzenmaier and Wehrle66). We observed very strongly correlations between nights for DSR and for quantitative EEG power spectra, consistent with prior studies (Reference Buckelmuller, Landolt, Stassen and Achermann67,Reference Tucker, Dinges and Van Dongen68), indicating that these power spectra may be reliable and stable markers for individuals. Thus, these may be robust physiologic markers with utility in assessing depression vulnerability.

Normal NREM and delta sleep may result in restorative properties that decrease depression risk (Reference Datta and Maclean69). NREM disruptions can adversely influence oxidative stress (Reference Silva, Abilio and Takatsu70), cell proliferation (Reference Hairston, Little and Scanlon71,Reference Roman, Van der Borght and Leemburg72), excitatory/inhibitory balance (Reference Corner, Baker and van Pelt73,Reference Krueger and Obal74), hippocampal plasticity (Reference Guzman-Marin, Ying and Suntsova75), cortical synaptic plasticity (Reference Romcy-Pereira, Leite and Garcia-Cairasco76), and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis function (Reference Leproult, Copinschi and Buxton77,Reference Spath-Schwalbe, Gofferje, Kern, Born and Fehm78). Each of these could feasibly increase depression risk. Decreased delta power and increases in higher frequency power, which can be associated with elevated NREM metabolic activity in depression-related areas such as the reticular formation, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the orbitofrontal cortex (Reference Hofle, Paus, Reutens, Fiset, Gotman and Evans79). It is thus plausible that the low DSR indicates that delta sleep was insufficiently ‘restorative’ in the first NREM period, thereby influencing depression risk.

There are several ways in which poor sleep could vitiate the effects of IFN-α, although any mechanistic inferences are limited by the fact that we did not examine the physiology by which IFN-α induces depression. But because IFN-α can decrease BDNF levels (Reference Lotrich, Albusaysi and Ferrell80), it is feasible that insomnia-induced deficits in cell proliferation (Reference Hairston, Little and Scanlon71,Reference Roman, Van der Borght and Leemburg72) and plasticity (Reference Guzman-Marin, Ying and Suntsova75,Reference Romcy-Pereira, Leite and Garcia-Cairasco76) could have exacerbated the effect of inflammatory cytokines on neurogenesis. Also, IFN-α decreases hippocampal cell proliferation via elevated IL-1β levels (Reference Kaneko, Kudo and Mabuchi81); and social isolation likewise decreases central BDNF and neurogenesis – mediated in part by the inflammatory cytokines like IL-1β (Reference Ben Menachem-Zidon, Goshen and Kreisel82Reference Koo and Duman84). Thus, both stress and inflammation share similar effects on growth factor function (Reference Peng, Chiou, Chen, Chou, Ky and Cheng85); and BDNF is required for the neuroprotective effects of antidepressants against lipopolysaccharide-induced apoptosis (Reference Peng, Chiou, Chen, Chou, Ky and Cheng85). Therefore, it makes sense that a pre-existing deficit in plasticity could be one potential mechanism by which poor sleep might exacerbate inflammatory affects on BDNF. Another possibility is that poor sleep affects the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (Reference Leproult, Copinschi and Buxton77,Reference Spath-Schwalbe, Gofferje, Kern, Born and Fehm78), which is likely to aggravate the response to IFN-α (Reference Raison, Borisov, Woolwine, Massung, Vogt and Miller86). Insufficient delta power also influences the excitatory/inhibitory balance (Reference Corner, Baker and van Pelt73,Reference Krueger and Obal74), which may potentially exacerbate IFN-α effects on serotonin (Reference Raison, Borisov, Majer, Drake, Pagnoni and Miller87), dopamine (Reference Felger, Alagbe and Hu88), and glutamate (Reference Haroon, Woolwine and Chen89) systems. Whether some or all of these physiological interactions are truly involved remains to be determined. Moreover, further exacerbating any pre-existing sleep problems, inflammatory cytokines such as IFN-α can additionally decrease sleep efficiency, sleep continuity, and the total amount of stages 3 and 4 sleep (Reference Raison, Rye and Woolwine90). And of course, a variety of other cytokines can further influence sleep quality. Ultimately, a bi-directional relationship between sleep and inflammation in influencing depression-related physiology is highly plausible. Regardless, the mechanisms underlying these interactions remain speculative.

Despite advances in delineating the pathophysiology of inflammation-related depression (Reference Felger and Lotrich18), a critical clinical question that remains is how to best remediate vulnerability and prevent depression in the minority who are not yet resilient. Our findings indicate that low DSR and high alpha power may be a good physiological target for treatment as these elements of sleep are consistently present in MDD (Reference Armitage, Hoffmann, Trivedi and Rush13,Reference Kupfer, Frank and Ehlers15) and are worsened by stress and glucocorticoids (Reference Antonijevic and Steiger91).

Congruent with this conclusion, patients who already have higher DSR tend to do better with psychotherapies such as cognitive behavioural therapy (Reference Thase, Fasiczka, Berman, Simons and Reynolds92) or sleep deprivation (Reference Nissen, Feige, Konig, Voderholzer, Berger and Riemann93). Conversely, a low DSR in non-depressed patients is a robust predictor of depression relapse after successful psychotherapy (Reference Kupfer, Frank, McEachran and Grochocinski16,Reference Spanier, Frank, McEachran, Grochocinski and Kupfer30,Reference Buysse, Frank, Lowe, Cherry and Kupfer31), during maintenance psychotherapy (Reference Kupfer, Frank, McEachran, Grochocinski and Ehlers94), or during maintenance antidepressant treatment (Reference Reynolds, Buysse and Brunner32). DSR also tends to be a fairly robust trait (Reference Buysse, Hall and Tu12) that does not change over time even with some effective depression psychotherapies (Reference Buysse, Frank, Lowe, Cherry and Kupfer31,Reference Thase, Fasiczka, Berman, Simons and Reynolds92). Although some of these antidepressants interventions may not influence DSR, there is some evidence that insomnia-specific therapies such as cognitive behavioural therapy for insomnia could both improve DSR and decrease alpha power (Reference Krystal and Edinger39,Reference Cervena, Dauvilliers and Espa95). Thus, our results indicate that these types of specific insomnia therapies could be one avenue for preventing depression.

Another avenue for improving sleep is antidepressant medications. Different medications have different effects on various sleep parameters (Reference Wichniak, Wierzbicka and Jernajczyk17,Reference Sonntag, Rothe, Guldner, Yassouridis, Holsboer and Steiger96Reference Argyropoulos and Wilson99). Some SSRIs and tricyclics can improve delta sleep (Reference Reynolds, Buysse and Brunner32,Reference Jindal, Friedman, Berman, Fasiczka, Howland and Thase100Reference Pasternak, Reynolds and Houck103). In fact, SSRIs have been observed to improve DSR in both remitters and non-remitters (Reference Argyropoulos, Hicks and Nash104). The first SSRI observed to prevent depression in patients started on IFN-α was paroxetine (Reference Musselman, Lawson and Gumnick105), and paroxetine also decreases alpha power but increases DSR in non-depressed insomniacs (Reference Nowell, Reynolds, Buysse, Dew and Kupfer106), a plausible mechanism by which paroxetine could improve resilience. Conversely, some SSRIs can exacerbate increased alpha power (Reference Wichniak, Wierzbicka and Jernajczyk17). It will therefore be clearly important to assess sleep parameters if antidepressants are examined for depression prevention efficacy. Other potential depression prevention options include agomelatine, which improves DSR (Reference Salva, Vanier and Laredo107,Reference Quera-Salva, Lemoine and Guilleminault108), as well as ghrelin, which can decrease alpha power and improve the amount of slow wave NREM sleep (Reference Kluge, Gazea and Schussler109).

These speculations regarding medications to prevent depression are testable hypotheses. Our findings would specifically predict that instituting a sleep therapy that improves DSR and decreases alpha power before an inflammatory challenge (such as IFN-α therapy) would prevent depression. This treatment could be specifically targeted to people who are currently sleeping poorly (as defined by a low DSR and elevated alpha power assessed using qEEG). The model would likewise predict that any sleep therapies that worsen DSR and increase alpha power would not prevent depression (even if the intervention improves other sleep parameters).

However, it is important to note that our observations, although prospective, do not necessarily imply causality. Improving DSR and decreasing alpha power may not necessarily improve resilience, particularly if these sleep variables are simply manifestations of a more fundamental neurologic function. A clinical treatment trial would be needed to test this hypothesis. Another caveat to our findings is the need for replication, particularly given the limited sample size. In addition, to focus on the role of sleep, we examined a fairly resilient population screened to not have current psychiatric or substance abuse problems despite having HCV. It remains to be determined whether the findings generalise to more vulnerable HCV populations, to other types of inflammatory cytokine-associated depression (Reference Lotrich110), and/or to depression more broadly.

Nonetheless, it has been years since the initial observations that poor sleep quality antedates depression – and the suggestion that treating sleep could prevent depression (Reference Ford and Kamerow111). Determining potential specific targets for treatment has been the next step. Our results would predict that benzodiazepines, which may improve sleep latency and subjective sleep quality, but can decrease delta power and increase higher frequency power (Reference Bastien, LeBlanc, Carrier and Morin40,Reference Feinberg, Maloney and Campbell41), would be unlikely to prevent depression. Rather, using this specific medical population – patients treated with IFN-α – we find that subjects with a high DSR and lower alpha power are resilient towards developing inflammatory cytokine-associated depression. It will now be critical to determine if normalising these specific sleep parameters can result in depression resilience for other patients.

Acknowledgements

Both Drs. Lotrich and Germain contributed to the conception and design of this study, to the acquisition of data, to the analysis and interpretation of data, to the drafting of the article, and to the final approval of the manuscript submitted.

Financial Support

This study was supported by the National Institutes of Health (MH-R01-090250, UL1-RR-024153, UL1-TR-000005).

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

1.Patten, SB. Long-term medical conditions and major depression in a Canadian population study at waves 1 and 2. J Affective Disord 2001;63:3541.Google Scholar
2.Eaton, WW, Kramer, M, Anthony, JC, Dryman, A, Shapiro, S, Locke, BZ. The incidence of specific DIS/DSM-III mental disorders: data from the NIMH Epidemiologic Catchment Area Program. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1989;79:163178.Google Scholar
3.Patten, SB, Lee, RC. Refining estimates of major depression incidence and episode duration in Canada using a Monte Carlo Markov model. Med Decis Making 2004;24:351358.Google Scholar
4.Kruijt, A-W, Antypa, N, Booij, Let al. Cognitive reactivity, implicit associations, and the incidence of depression: a two-year prospective study. PLoS One 2013;8:e70245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Boyes, AW, Girgis, A, D'Este, CA, Zucca, AC, Lecathelinais, C, Carey, ML. Prevalence and predictors of the short-term trajectory of anxiety and depression in the first year after a cancer diagnosis: a population-based longitudinal study. J Clin Oncol 2013;31:27242729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Udina, M, Castellvi, P, Moreno-Espana, Jet al. Interferon-induced depression in chronic hepatitis C: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Clin Psychiatry 2012;73:11281138.Google Scholar
7.Breslau, N, Roth, T, Rosenthal, L, Andreski, P. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: a longitudinal epidemiological study of young adults. Biol Psychiatry 1996;39:411418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Gregory, AM, Rijsdijk, FV, Lau, JYet al. The direction of longitudinal associations between sleep problems and depression symptoms: a study of twins aged 8 and 10 years. Sleep 2009;32:189199.Google Scholar
9.Buysse, DJ, Angst, J, Gamma, A, Ajdacic, V, Eich, D, Rossler, W. Prevalence, course, and comorbidity of insomnia and depression in young adults. Sleep 2008;31:473480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Roane, BM, Taylor, DJ. Adolescent insomnia as a risk factor for early adult depression and substance abuse. Sleep 2008;31:13511356.Google ScholarPubMed
11.Emslie, GJ, Armitage, R, Weinberg, WA, Rush, AJ, Mayes, TL, Hoffmann, RF. Sleep polysomnography as a predictor of recurrence in children and adolescents with major depressive disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2001;4:159168.Google Scholar
12.Buysse, DJ, Hall, M, Tu, XMet al. Latent structure of EEG sleep variables in depressed and control subjects: descriptions and clinical correlates. Psychiatry Res 1998;79:105122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Armitage, R, Hoffmann, R, Trivedi, M, Rush, AJ. Slow-wave activity in NREM sleep: sex and age effects in depressed outpatients and healthy controls. Psychiatry Res 2000;95:201213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Steiger, A, Kimura, M. Wake and sleep EEG provide biomarkers in depression. J Psychiatr Res 2010;44:242252.Google Scholar
15.Kupfer, DJ, Frank, E, Ehlers, CL. EEG sleep in young depressives: first and second night effects. Biol Psychiatry 1989;25:8797.Google Scholar
16.Kupfer, DJ, Frank, E, McEachran, AB, Grochocinski, VJ. Delta sleep ratio. A biological correlate of early recurrence in unipolar affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1990;47:11001105.Google Scholar
17.Wichniak, A, Wierzbicka, A, Jernajczyk, W. Sleep and antidepressant treatment. Curr Pharm Des 2012;18:58085817.Google Scholar
18.Felger, JC, Lotrich, FE. Inflammatory cytokines in depression: neurobiological mechanisms and therapeutic implications. Neuroscience 2013;246:199229.Google Scholar
19.Franzen, PL, Buysse, DJ, Rabinovitz, M, Pollock, BG, Lotrich, FE. Poor sleep quality predicts onset of either major depression or subsyndromal depression with irritability during interferon-alpha treatment. J Psychiatr Res 2009;177:240245.Google Scholar
20.Prather, A, Rabinvitz, M, Pollock, B, Lotrich, F. Cytokine-induced depression during IFN-a treatment: the role of IL-6 and sleep quality. Brain Behav Immunol 2009;23:11091116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21.Campbell, IG, Darchia, N, Higgins, LMet al. Adolescent changes in homeostatic regulation of EEG activity in the delta and theta frequency bands during NREM sleep. Sleep 2011;34:8391.Google Scholar
22.Csercsa, R, Dombovari, B, Fabo, Det al. Laminar analysis of slow wave activity in humans. Brain 2010;133:28142829.Google Scholar
23.Dworak, M, McCarley, RW, Kim, T, Kalinchuk, AV, Basheer, R. Sleep and brain energy levels: ATP changes during sleep. J Neurosci Res 2010;30:90079016.Google Scholar
24.Murphy, M, Riedner, BA, Huber, R, Massimini, M, Ferrarelli, F, Tononi, G. Source modeling sleep slow waves. PNAS USA 2009;106:16081613.Google Scholar
25.Dang-Vu, TT, Desseilles, M, Laureys, Set al. Cerebral correlates of delta waves during non-REM sleep revisited. Neuroimage 2005;28:1421.Google Scholar
26.Nofzinger, EA. Neuroimaging of sleep and sleep disorders. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2006;6:149155.Google Scholar
27.Hall, M, Thayer, JF, Germain, Aet al. Psychological stress is associated with heightened physiological arousal during NREM sleep in primary insomnia. Behav Sleep Med 2007;5:178193.Google Scholar
28.Germain, A, Nofzinger, EA, Kupfer, DJ, Buysse, DJ. Neurobiology of non-REM sleep in depression: further evidence for hypofrontality and thalamic dysregulation. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:18561863.Google Scholar
29.Kupfer, DJ, Reynolds, CF, Ulrich, RF, Grochocinski, VJ. Comparison of automated REM and slow-wave sleep analyses in young and middle-aged depressed subjects. Biol Psychiatry 1986;21:189200.Google Scholar
30.Spanier, C, Frank, E, McEachran, AB, Grochocinski, VJ, Kupfer, DJ. The prophylaxis of depressive episodes in recurrent depression following discontinuation of drug therapy: integrating psychological and biological factors. Psychol Med 1996;26:461475.Google Scholar
31.Buysse, DJ, Frank, E, Lowe, KK, Cherry, CR, Kupfer, DJ. Electroencephalographic sleep correlates of episode and vulnerability to recurrence in depression. Biol Psychiatry 1997;41:406418.Google Scholar
32.Reynolds, CFr, Buysse, DJ, Brunner, DPet al. Maintenance nortriptyline effects on electroencephalographic sleep in elderly patients with recurrent major depression: double-blind, placebo- and plasma-level-controlled evaluation. Biol Psychiatry 1997;42:560567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33.Lauer, CJ, Schreiber, W, Holsboer, F, Krieg, JC. In quest of identifying vulnerability markers for psychiatric disorders by all-night polysomnography. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995;52:145153.Google Scholar
34.Fenzl, T, Touma, C, Romanowski, CPet al. Sleep disturbances in highly stress reactive mice: modeling endophenotypes of major depression. BMC Neurosci 2011;12:29.Google Scholar
35.Nitschke, JB, Heller, W, Etienne, MA, Miller, GA. Prefrontal cortex activity differentials processes affecting memory in depression. Biol Psychiatry 2004;67:125143.Google Scholar
36.von Stein, A, Sarnthein, J. Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. Int J Psychophysiol 2000;38:301313.Google Scholar
37.Bjorvatn, B, Fagerland, S, Ursin, R. EEG power densities (0.5–20 Hz) in different sleep-wake stages in rats. Physiol Behav 1998;63:413417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
38.Nofzinger, EA, Price, JC, Meltzer, CCet al. Towards a neurobiology of dysfunctional arousal in depression: the relationship between beta EEG power and regional cerebral glucose metabolism during NREM sleep. Psychiatry Res 2000;98:7191.Google Scholar
39.Krystal, AD, Edinger, JD. Sleep EEG predictors and correlates of the response to cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia. Sleep 2010;33:669677.Google Scholar
40.Bastien, CH, LeBlanc, M, Carrier, J, Morin, CM. Sleep EEG power spectra, insomnia, and chronic use of benzodiazepines. Sleep 2003;26:313317.Google Scholar
41.Feinberg, I, Maloney, T, Campbell, IG. Effects of hypnotics on the sleep EEG of healthy young adults: new data and psychopharmacologic implications. J Psychiatr Res 2000;34:423438.Google Scholar
42.Dumont, GJ, de Visser, SJ, Cohen, AF, van Gerven, JM. Biomarkers for the effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) in healthy subjects. Br J Clin Pharmacol 2005;59:495510.Google Scholar
43.Silvestri, R, Pace-Schott, EF, Gersh, T, Stickgold, R, Salzman, C, Hobson, JA. Effects of fluvoxamine and paroxetine on sleep structure in normal subjects: a home-based Nightcap evaluation during drug administration and withdrawal. J Clin Psychiatry 2001;62:642652.Google Scholar
44.Britton, WB, Haynes, PL, Fridel, KW, Bootzin, RR. Polysomnographic and subjective profiles of sleep continuity before and after mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in partially remitted depression. Psychosom Med 2010;72:539548.Google Scholar
45.Lundahl, J, Deacon, S, Maurice, D, Staner, L. EEG spectral power density profiles during NREM sleep for gaboxadol and zolpidem in patients with primary insomnia. J Psychopharmacol 2012;26:10811087.Google Scholar
46.Quera-Salva, MA, Hajak, G, Philip, Pet al. Comparison of agomelatine and escitalopram on nighttime sleep and daytime condition and efficacy in major depressive disorder patients. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 2011;26:252262.Google Scholar
47.First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBWet al. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV (SCID-1) (users guide and interview) research version. New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York Psychiatric Institute, 1995.Google Scholar
48.Beck, A, Steer, R, Garbin, M. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev 1988;8:77100.Google Scholar
49.Buysse, DJ, Reynolds, CF, Monk, TH, Berman, SR, Kupfer, DJ. Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatr Res 1989;28:193213.Google Scholar
50.Hayashino, Y, Yamazaki, S, Takegami, M, Nakayama, T, Sokejima, S, Fukuhara, S. Association between number of comorbid conditions, depression, and sleep quality using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: results from a population-based survey. Sleep Med 2010;11:366371.Google Scholar
51.Owen, DC, Parker, KP, McGuire, DB. Comparison of subjective sleep quality in patients with cancer and healthy subjects. Oncol Nurs Forum 1999;26:16491651.Google Scholar
52.Carpenter, J, Andrykowski, MA. Psychometric evaluation of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. J Psychosom Res 1998;45:513.Google Scholar
53.Monk, TH, Reynolds, CF, Kupfer, DJet al. The Pittsburgh Sleep Diary. J Sleep Res 1994;3:111120.Google Scholar
54.Doman, J, Detka, C, Hoffman, Tet al. Automating the sleep laboratory: implementation and validation of digital recording and analysis. Int J Biomed Comput 1995;38:277290.Google Scholar
55.Brunner, DP, Vasko, RC, Detka, CS, Monahan, JP, Reynolds, CF, Kupfer, DJ. Muscle artifacts in the sleep EEG: automated detection and effect on all-night EEG power spectra. J Sleep Res 1996;5:155164.Google Scholar
56.Lange, H. EEG spectral analysis in vital depression: ultradian cycles. Biol Psychiatry 1982;17:321.Google Scholar
57.McKinney, SM, Dang-Vu, TT, Buxton, OM, Solet, JM, Ellenbogen, JM. Covert waking brain activity reveals instantaneous sleep depth. PLoS One 2011;6:e17351.Google Scholar
58.Dahl, RE, Williamson, DE, Bertocci, MA, Stolz, MV, Ryan, ND, Ehlers, CL. Spectral analyses of sleep EEG in depressed offspring of fathers with or without a positive history of alcohol abuse or dependence: a pilot study. Alcohol Alcohol 2003;30:193200.Google Scholar
59.Budhiraja, R, Quan, SF, Punjabi, NM, Drake, CL, Dickman, R, Fass, R. Power spectral analysis of the sleep electroencephalogram in heartburn patients with or without gastroesophageal reflux disease: a feasibility study. J Clin Gastroenterol 2010;44:9196.Google Scholar
60.Harman, K, Pivik, RT, D'Eon, JL, Wilson, KG, Swenson, JR, Matsunaga, L. Sleep in depressed and nondepressed participants with chronic low back pain: electroencephalographic and behaviour findings. Sleep 2002;25:775783.Google Scholar
61.Steriade, M, McCarley, R. Brainstem control of wakefulness and sleep. New York: Plenum, 1990.Google Scholar
62.Staner, L, Cornette, F, Maurice, Det al. Sleep microstructure around sleep onset differentiates major depressive insomnia from primary insomnia. J Sleep Res 2003;12:319330.Google Scholar
63.Merica, H, Blois, R, Gaillard, JM. Spectral characteristics of sleep EEG in chronic insomnia. Eur J Neurosci 1998;10:18261834.Google Scholar
64.Luthringer, R, Minot, R, Toussaint, M, Calvi-Gries, F, Schaltenbrand, N, Macher, JP. All-night EEG spectral analysis as a tool for the prediction of clinical response to antidepressant treatment. Biol Psychiatry 1995;38:98104.Google Scholar
65.Linkowski, P. EEG sleep patterns in twins. J Sleep Res 1999;8:1113.Google Scholar
66.Ambrosius, U, Lietzenmaier, S, Wehrle, Ret al. Heritability of sleep electroencephalogram. Biol Psychiatry 2008;64:344348.Google Scholar
67.Buckelmuller, J, Landolt, HP, Stassen, HH, Achermann, P. Trait-like individual differences in the human sleep electroencephalogram. Neuroscience 2006;138:351356.Google Scholar
68.Tucker, AM, Dinges, DF, Van Dongen, HPA. Trait interindividual differences in the sleep physiology of healthy young adults. J Sleep Res 2007;16:170180.Google Scholar
69.Datta, S, Maclean, RR. Neurobiological mechanisms for the regulation of mammalian sleep-wake behavior: reinterpretation of historical evidence and inclusion of contemporary cellular and molecular evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2007;31:775824.Google Scholar
70.Silva, RH, Abilio, VC, Takatsu, ALet al. Role of hippocampal oxidative stress in memory deficits induced by sleep deprivation in mice. Neuropharmacol 2004;46:895903.Google Scholar
71.Hairston, IS, Little, MTM, Scanlon, MDet al. Sleep restriction suppresses neurogenesis induced by hippocampus-dependent learning. J Neurophysiol 2005;94:42244233.Google Scholar
72.Roman, V, Van der Borght, K, Leemburg, SAet al. Sleep restriction by forced activity reduces hippocampal cell proliferation. Brain Res 2005;1065:5359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
73.Corner, MA, Baker, RE, van Pelt, J. Physiological consequences of selective suppression of synaptic transmission in developing cerebral cortical networks in vitro: differential effects on intrinsically generated bioelectric discharges in a living ‘model’ system for slow-wave sleep activity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008;32:15691600.Google Scholar
74.Krueger, JM, Obal, F. Sleep function. Front Biosci 2003;8:511519.Google Scholar
75.Guzman-Marin, R, Ying, Z, Suntsova, Net al. Suppression of hippocampal plasticity-related gene expression by sleep deprivation in rats. J Physiol 2006;575:807819.Google Scholar
76.Romcy-Pereira, RN, Leite, JP, Garcia-Cairasco, N. Synaptic plasticity along the sleep-wake cycle: implications for epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav 2009;14:4753.Google Scholar
77.Leproult, R, Copinschi, G, Buxton, Oet al. Sleep loss results in an elevation of cortisol levels the next evening. Sleep 1997;20:865870.Google Scholar
78.Spath-Schwalbe, E, Gofferje, M, Kern, W, Born, J, Fehm, HL. Sleep disruption alters nocturnal ACTH and cortisol secretory patterns. Biol Psychiatry 1991;29:575584.Google Scholar
79.Hofle, N, Paus, T, Reutens, D, Fiset, P, Gotman, J, Evans, A. Regional cerebral blood flow changes as a function of delta and spindle activity during slow wave sleep in humans. J Neurosci 1997;17:48004808.Google Scholar
80.Lotrich, FE, Albusaysi, S, Ferrell, RE. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor serum levels and genotype: association with depression during interferon-alpha treatment. Neuropsychopharmacol 2013;38:989995.Google Scholar
81.Kaneko, N, Kudo, K, Mabuchi, Tet al. Suppression of cell proliferation by interferon-alpha through interleukin-1 production in adult rat dentate gyrus. Neuropsychopharmacol 2006;31:26192626.Google Scholar
82.Ben Menachem-Zidon, O, Goshen, I, Kreisel, Tet al. Intrahippocampal transplantation of transgenic neural precursor cells overexpressing interleukin-1 receptor antagonist blocks chronic isolation-induced impairment in memory and neurogenesis. Neuropsychopharmacol 2008;33:22512262.Google Scholar
83.Barrientos, RM, Sprunger, DB, Campeau, Set al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor mRNA downregulation produced by social isolation is blocked by intrahippocampal interleukin-1 receptor antagonist. Neuroscience 2003;121:847853.Google Scholar
84.Koo, JW, Duman, RS. IL-1beta is an essential mediator of the antineurogenic and anhedondic effects of stress. PNAS USA 2008;105:751756.Google Scholar
85.Peng, CH, Chiou, SH, Chen, SJ, Chou, YC, Ky, HH, Cheng, CK. Neuroprotection by imipramine against lipopolysaccharide-induced apoptosis in hippocampus-derived neural stem cells mediated by activation of BDNF and the MAPK pathway. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2008;18:128140.Google Scholar
86.Raison, CL, Borisov, AS, Woolwine, BJ, Massung, B, Vogt, G, Miller, AH. Interferon-alpha effects on diurnal hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis activity: relationship with proinflammatory cytokines and behavior. Mol Psychiatry 2010;15:535547.Google Scholar
87.Raison, CL, Borisov, AS, Majer, M, Drake, D, Pagnoni, G, Miller, AH. Activation of central nervous system inflammatory pathways by interferon-alpha: relationship to monoamines and depression. Biol Psychiatry 2009;65:296303.Google Scholar
88.Felger, JC, Alagbe, O, Hu, Fet al. Effects of interferon-alpha on rhesus monkeys: a nonhuman primate model of cytokine-induced depression. Biol Psychiatry 2007;62:13241333.Google Scholar
89.Haroon, E, Woolwine, B, Chen, Xet al. IFN-alpha-induced cortical and subcortical glutamate changes assessed by magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Neuropsychopharmacol 2014;39:17771785.Google Scholar
90.Raison, CL, Rye, DB, Woolwine, BJet al. Chronic interferon-alpha administration disrupts sleep continuity and depth in patients with hepatitis C: association with fatigue, motor slowing, and increased evening cortisol. Biol Psychiatry 2010;68:942949.Google Scholar
91.Antonijevic, IA, Steiger, A. Depression-like changes of the sleep-EEG during high dose corticosteroid treatment in patients with multiple sclerosis. Psychoneuroendocrinol 2003;28:780795.Google Scholar
92.Thase, ME, Fasiczka, AL, Berman, SR, Simons, AD, Reynolds, CF. Electroencephalographic sleep profiles before and after cognitive behavior therapy of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998;55:138144.Google Scholar
93.Nissen, C, Feige, B, Konig, A, Voderholzer, U, Berger, M, Riemann, D. Delta sleep ratio as a predictor of sleep deprivation response in major depression. J Psychiatr Res 2001;35:155163.Google Scholar
94.Kupfer, DJ, Frank, E, McEachran, AB, Grochocinski, VJ, Ehlers, CL. EEG sleep correlates of recurrence of depression on active medication. Depression 1993;1:300308.Google Scholar
95.Cervena, K, Dauvilliers, Y, Espa, Fet al. Effect of cognitive behavioural therapy for insomnia on sleep architecture and sleep EEG power spectra in psychophysiological insomnia. J Sleep Res 2004;13:385393.Google Scholar
96.Sonntag, A, Rothe, B, Guldner, J, Yassouridis, A, Holsboer, F, Steiger, A. Trimipramine and imipramine exert different effects on the sleep EEG and on nocturnal hormone secretion during treatment of major depression. Depression 1996;4:113.Google Scholar
97.Chalon, S, Pereira, A, Lainey, Eet al. Comparative effects of duloxetine and desipramine on sleep EEG in healthy subjects. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2005;177:357365.Google Scholar
98.Schittecatte, M, Dumont, F, Machowski, R, Cornil, C, Lavergne, F, Wilmotte, J. Effects of mirtazapine on sleep polygraphic variables in major depression. Neuropsychobiol 2002;46:197201.Google Scholar
99.Argyropoulos, SV, Wilson, SJ. Sleep disturbances in depression and the effects of antidepressants. Int Rev Psychiatry 2005;17:237245.Google Scholar
100.Jindal, RD, Friedman, ES, Berman, SR, Fasiczka, AL, Howland, RH, Thase, ME. Effects of sertraline on sleep architecture in patients with depression. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2003;23:540548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
101.Ehlers, CL, Havstad, JW, Kupfer, DJ. Estimation of the time course of slow-wave sleep over the night in depressed patients: effects of clomipramine and clinical response. Biol Psychiatry 1996;39:171181.Google Scholar
102.Feige, B, Voderholzer, U, Riemann, D, Dittmann, R, Hohagen, F, Berger, M. Fluoxetine and sleep EEG: effects of a single dose, subchronic treatment, and discontinuation in healthy subjects. Neuropsychopharmacol 2002;26:246258.Google Scholar
103.Pasternak, RE, Reynolds, CF 3rd, Houck, PRet al. Sleep in bereavement-related depression during and after pharmacotherapy with nortriptyline. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1994;7:6973.Google Scholar
104.Argyropoulos, SV, Hicks, JA, Nash, JRet al. Redistribution of slow wave activity of sleep during pharmacological treatment of depression with paroxetine but not with nefazodone. J Sleep Res 2009;18:342343.Google Scholar
105.Musselman, DL, Lawson, DH, Gumnick, JFet al. Paroxetine for the prevention of depression induced by high-dose interferon alfa. NEJM 2001;344:961966.Google Scholar
106.Nowell, PD, Reynolds, CFr, Buysse, DJ, Dew, MA, Kupfer, DJ. Paroxetine in the treatment of primary insomnia: preliminary clinical and electroencephalogram sleep data. J Clin Psychiatry 1999;60:8995.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
107.Salva, M-AQ, Vanier, B, Laredo, Jet al. Major depressive disorder, sleep EEG and agomelatine: an open-label study. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2007;10:691696.Google Scholar
108.Quera-Salva, MA, Lemoine, P, Guilleminault, C. Impact of the novel antidepressant agomelatine on disturbed sleep-wake cycles in depressed patients. Hum Psychopharmacol 2010;25:222229.Google Scholar
109.Kluge, M, Gazea, M, Schussler, Pet al. Ghrelin increases slow wave sleep and stage 2 sleep and decreases stage 1 sleep and REM sleep in elderly men but does not affect sleep in elderly women. Psychoneuroendocrinol 2010;35:297304.Google Scholar
110.Lotrich, FE. Inflammatory cytokine-associated depression. Brain Res 2014; epub 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.06.032.Google Scholar
111.Ford, DE, Kamerow, DB. Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders. An opportunity for prevention? JAMA 1989;262:14791484.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Demographics and polysomnography results include the body mass index (BMI), cumulative illness rating scale for geriatrics (CIRS-G), Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II), Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS), diary self-reports of sleep, and quantitative EEG results

Figure 1

Fig. 1 Pre-treatment alpha power is predictive of subsequently increased Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) scores.

Figure 2

Fig. 2 When relative alpha power is low (upper panel), then a high delta sleep ratio (DSR; filled circles) is protective against depression compared with low DSR (open circles). When relative alpha power is high (lower panel), then depression worsens regardless of DSR.