Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-12T07:57:34.079Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Incorporating coordination dynamics into an evolutionarily grounded science of intentional change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2014

Viviane Kostrubiec
Affiliation:
PRISSMH EA EA 4561, University of Toulouse, 31062 Toulouse Cedex 9, France.viviane.kostrubiec@univ-tlse3.fr
J. A. Scott Kelso
Affiliation:
Center for Complex Systems and Brain Sciences, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL 33421. kelso@ccs.fau.edu Intelligent Systems Research Centre, University of Ulster, Magee Campus, Derry BT487JL, Northern Ireland.

Abstract

We suggest the authors' endeavor toward a science of intentional change may benefit from recent advances in informationally meaningful self-organizing dynamical systems. Coordination Dynamics, having contributed to an understanding of behavior on several time scales – adaptation, learning, and development – and on different levels of analysis, from the neural to the social, may complement, if not enhance, the authors' insights.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Inspired by the notion of a “Darwin machine,” Wilson et al. aim to reconcile diametrically distinct evolutionary processes, such as innate versus adaptive and domain-general versus task specific, in a move toward a science of behavioral and cultural change. We applaud this step, though we think that the authors' rapprochement between Darwin machines and “multi-agent cooperative systems” requires some elaboration. What seems to be missing are the concepts, methods, and tools of self-organizing dynamical systems tailored specifically to the coordinated activities of living things – how they move, adapt, learn, develop, and so on (Beek et al. Reference Beek, Peper and Stegman1995; Calvin & Jirsa Reference Calvin, Jirsa, Huys and Jirsa2010; Haken et al. Reference Haken, Kelso and Bunz1985; Kelso Reference Kelso1995; Kelso & Haken Reference Kelso, Haken, Murphy and O'Neill1995; Schöner & Kelso Reference Schöner and Kelso1988; Turvey & Carello Reference Turvey and Carello2012; Warren Reference Warren2006; Zanone & Kelso Reference Zanone and Kelso1992). Among others, Coordination Dynamics (CD) has long been inspired by the works of Howard Pattee, who understood the significance of biological coordination, particularly the complementary nature of symbolic and dynamic descriptions (Kelso & Engstrøm, Reference Kelso and Engstrøm2006; Pattee & Raczaszek-Leonardi Reference Pattee and Raczaszek-Leonardi2012).

Instead of opposing genetically fixed and adaptive processes, Coordination Dynamics sees them as dual processes evolving on different time scales. Apparently “fixed” processes are not immutable; they are stable or slowly evolving. In complex systems, processes evolving on slower time scales have been shown to constrain faster ones (Haken Reference Haken1983). This opens the possibility to inquire under which conditions fast-evolving processes escape such slowly evolving (viz. inherited) constraints and reorganize the entire behavioral repertoire. Evidence shows that by scrutinizing how behavioral stability is lost or increases, it is possible to address the fundamental nature of change on several time scales: behavior (Schöner & Kelso Reference Schöner and Kelso1988), development (Sporns & Edelman Reference Sporns and Edelman1993; Thelen & Smith Reference Thelen and Smith1994; Thelen et al. Reference Thelen, Kelso and Fogel1987), learning (Zanone & Kelso Reference Zanone and Kelso1992), and adaptation (Warren Reference Warren2006).

Intentional change acts not on a blank slate but on an initial behavioral repertoire that favors or alters intentional action (Johnston Reference Johnston1981). A basic aspect of self-organizing CD is that elements of any nature tend to coordinate spontaneously when information is exchanged (usually bidirectionally) with the environment, creating “for free” intrinsic coordination tendencies. Such tendencies are not fixed but constitute a dynamic potential – the initial behavioral repertoire – that inhabits the same information space as intention (Kelso Reference Kelso1995; Reference Kelso2002). This is a reciprocal interaction: The repertoire shapes, while being shaped by, intentional forcing. Evidence on both behavioral and brain levels shows that an intentional change of behavior is determined by the relative stability of pre-existing patterns in the repertoire (DeLuca et al. Reference DeLuca, Jantzen, Comani, Bertollo and Kelso2010). Such tendencies specify the nature of change-driving parameters, the competitive or cooperative mechanisms involved, the gradual or abrupt pathways of behavioral evolution, and the transfer of acquired changes (Kostrubiec et al. Reference Kostrubiec, Zanone, Fuchs and Kelso2012; Zanone & Kelso Reference Zanone and Kelso1992; Reference Zanone and Kelso1997). In line with a science of intentional change, CD suggests that the paths and outcomes of behavioral evolution are predictable. Prediction is possible, however, on the condition that the relevant variables capturing coordination tendencies are identified and the initial behavioral repertoire assessed before applying change-driving factors.

If, like the authors, CD rejects the blank-slate tradition, the question arises of how the initial behavioral repertoire prior to learning specifies criteria for selection mechanisms to operate. Although useful, Skinner's (Reference Skinner1984) “selection by consequences” leaves open this issue (Timberlake Reference Timberlake and Dewsbury1990).

In our work on learning dynamics (e.g., Kostrubiec et al. Reference Kostrubiec, Zanone, Fuchs and Kelso2012), careful measurement of the initial repertoire allowed the discovery of “selection via instability” and “selection via matching” principles (Kelso Reference Kelso, Bergman, Cairns, Nilsson and Nystedt2000). The former predicts that when the initial repertoire contains only a few stable patterns, such that they and environmental requirements are far apart in pattern space, competition arises, leading to instability and sudden, abrupt phase transitions, at which a wide range of unstable, transient patterns are generated. Once one of them succeeds in dominating the system, it tends to persist as a new stable pattern in the repertoire. Conversely, when the required and initial patterns are close to each other in pattern space cooperation between them ensues. This entails a smooth, gradual shift of one of the initial patterns in the repertoire that matches the environmentally required behavior but does not add to the number of patterns in the repertoire itself. Hence, the contingent mechanism for change is “selection via matching.”

Empirical data indicate that selection via instability leads to long-term persistent behavior, whereas the outcomes of selection via matching are rapidly forgotten (Kostrubiec et al. Reference Kostrubiec, Tallet and Zanone2006).

In the last analysis, cooperation and competition dictate the path of change (viz. smooth or abrupt) and Darwinian-like manifestations (viz. generation, selection, retention) appear as observable a posteriori outcomes. A key aspect the authors may consider is that the initial behavioral repertoire and its essentially nonlinear dynamics influence not only how new behaviors are formed but also their sustainability.

Viewing selection as a competitive process, the authors argue that an account of coordination between distinct processes around a given task is required. CD may help in clarifying what cooperation means. Evidence suggests that due to the tremendous degeneracy of living systems, where the same outcome may be produced by different combinations of elements and via different pathways, the significant functional units are context-dependent coordinative structures (Kelso Reference Kelso and Meyers2009). Coordinative structures are softly assembled; all the parts are weakly interacting. Perturbing one part may produce a remote effect somewhere else without disrupting – indeed preserving – integrity of function. Coordinative structures are collective states whose spatiotemporal dynamics prove to be quite rich, including interesting transient regimes that are neither fully ordered nor disordered in space and time. For present purposes, a system may be termed “cooperative” if it is open to information exchange and hosts numerous context-dependent elements whose nonlinear interaction leads them to coordinate. Viewed in this light, a Darwin machine is a cooperative system in which the slowly and rapidly adapting parts remain separated while transiently interacting. Such dual coexisting tendencies would mean that the coordination dynamics of a Darwin machine are metastable, thereby providing a number of evolutionary advantages (Kelso Reference Kelso2012).

References

Beek, P. J., Peper, C. E. & Stegman, D. F. (1995) Dynamical models of movement coordination. Human Movement Science 14:573608.Google Scholar
Calvin, S., & Jirsa, V.K. (2010) Perspectives on the dynamic nature of coupling in human coordination. In: Nonlinear dynamics of human behaviour, ed. Huys, R. & Jirsa, V. K.. Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
DeLuca, C., Jantzen, K. J., Comani, S., Bertollo, M. & Kelso, J. A. S. (2010) Striatal activity during intentional switching depends on pattern stability. Journal of Neuroscience 30:3167–74.Google Scholar
Haken, H. (1983) Synergetics: An introduction. Nonequilibrium phase transitions and self-organization in physics, chemistry and biology, 3rd rev. enl. ed. Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Haken, H., Kelso, J. A. S. & Bunz, H. (1985) A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics 51:347–56.Google Scholar
Johnston, T. D. (1981) Contrasting approaches to a theory of learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4:125–73.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. (1995) Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. (2000) Principles of dynamic pattern formation and change for a science of human behavior. In: Developmental science and the holistic approach, ed. Bergman, L. R., Cairns, R. B., Nilsson, L.-G. & Nystedt, L., pp. 6383. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. (2002) The complementary nature of coordination dynamics: Self-organization and the origins of agency. Journal of Nonlinear Phenomena in Complex Systems 5:364–71.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. (2009) Coordination dynamics. In: Encyclopedia of complexity and system science, ed. Meyers, R. A., pp. 1537–64. Springer.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. (2012) Multistability and metastability: Understanding dynamic coordination in the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367:906–18.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. & Engstrøm, D. A. (2006) The complementary nature. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. & Haken, H. (1995) New laws to be expected in the organism: Synergetics of brain and behavior. In: What is life? The next 50 years, ed. Murphy, M. & O'Neill, L., pp. 137–60. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kostrubiec, V., Tallet, J. & Zanone, P. G. (2006) How a new behavioral pattern is stabilized with learning determines its persistence and flexibility in memory. Experimental Brain Research 170:238–44.Google Scholar
Kostrubiec, V., Zanone, P. G., Fuchs, A. & Kelso, J. A. S. (2012) Beyond the blank slate: Routes to learning new coordination patterns depend on the intrinsic dynamics of the learner – experimental evidence and theoretical model. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pattee, H. H. & Raczaszek-Leonardi, J. (2012) Laws, language and life. Springer.Google Scholar
Schöner, G. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1988) Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science 239:1513–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1984) Selection by consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7:477510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sporns, O. & Edelman, G. M. (1993) Solving Bernstein's problem: A proposal for the development of coordinated movement by selection. Child Development 64:960–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1994) A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., Kelso, J. A. S. & Fogel, A. (1987) Self organizing systems and infant motor development. Developmental Review 7:3965.Google Scholar
Timberlake, W. (1990) Natural learning in laboratory paradigms. In: Contemporary issues in comparative psychology, ed. Dewsbury, D. A., pp. 3154, Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Turvey, M. T. & Carello, C. (2012) On intelligence from first principles: Guidelines for inquiry into the hypothesis of physical intelligence (PI). Ecological Psychology 24:332.Google Scholar
Warren, W. H. (2006) The dynamics of perception and action. Psychological Review 113:358–89.Google Scholar
Zanone, P. G. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1997) The coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: Collective and component levels. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 23(5):1454–80.Google ScholarPubMed
Zanone, P. G. & Kelso, J. A. S. (1992) Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: Nonequilibrium phase transitions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18(2):403–21.Google Scholar