Introduction
Visuospatial working memory (VSWM) is the process by which a limited amount of visuospatial information is maintained and manipulated for a short time. This ability is crucial to plan future behaviour but because of its limited capacity, the selection of relevant information is critical. It has been proposed that VSWM can regulate oculomotor processes in order to select or recognise relevant information in the visual field (Reference Pierrot-Deseilligny, Milea and Muri1–Reference Pierrot-Deseilligny, Muri, Ploner, Gaymard, Demeret and Rivaud-Pechoux3).
Working memory dysfunctions in schizophrenia are well documented Reference Lee and Park(4) and deficits in its visuospatial component have been proposed as an illness trait marker Reference Wood, Pantelis and Proffitt(5). They are accompanied by scanpath abnormalities during the exploration of geometrical figures Reference Green, Williams and Hemsley(6) or images with socially salient content [e.g. faces; see (Reference Phillips and David7–Reference Loughland, Williams and Gordon10)]. Patients show shorter scanpath lengths and fewer fixations on informative parts of the scene (Reference Williams, Loughland, Gordon and Davidson9–Reference Manor, Gordon and Williams16). This particular oculomotor behaviour in schizophrenia might be related to a general cognitive deficit in the selection, integration and use of relevant information, commonly detected in VSWM deficits. However, the spatial distribution of fixations during the encoding of complex visual stimuli in schizophrenia seems independent of stimulus type (e.g. natural scene vs. images with socially salient content). This suggests that basic oculomotor function can contribute to the abnormal ocular behaviour and eventually disturb subsequent global information processing Reference Bestelmeyer, Tatler, Phillips, Fraser, Benson and St Clair(17).
The relationship between VSWM and oculomotor functions has been investigated in a number of studies (Reference Hutton, Huddy and Barnes18–Reference Snitz, Curtis, Zald, Katsanis and Iacono21). In spite of incongruent results, recent findings argue that basic anomalies in smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) are insufficient by themselves to explain VSWM deficits in patients with schizophrenia Reference Hutton, Huddy and Barnes(18,Reference Cocchi, Bosisio, Carter, Wood, Berchtold, Conus, Orita, Debanné and Schenk42). During the encoding of dynamic stimuli, SPEM involve complex processes Reference Thaker, Avila, Hong, Medoff, Ross and Adami(22), not directly comparable to those of scanpath generation (i.e. saccades and fixations). Therefore, the relationship between oculomotor and VSWM processes during a complex VSWM task presenting static stimuli in patients with schizophrenia requires further investigation.
Recently, we investigated VSWM in patients with schizophrenia using a new task Reference Cocchi, Schenk, Volken, Bovet, Parnas and Vianin(23). In this paradigm, subjects were asked to encode stimuli composed of single multi-segment line patterns for later recognition. After a short delay, they had to discriminate the original stimulus from five incorrect patterns that had some properties in common with the target. Recognition performance was reduced in participants with schizophrenia as compared to healthy controls. They expressed a preference for incorrect patterns with fragmentary encoding concentrated on the left or right part of the stimulus, or approximating its global shape. We interpreted these results as a general impairment in global encoding abilities, combining early stage visual processes (Reference Uhlhaas and Silverstein24–Reference Kurylo, Pasternak, Silipo, Javitt and Butler27) and VSWM dysfunctions Reference Lee and Park(4). Early stage visual processes are needed to produce a fast, but imprecise, internal representation of the stimulus. VSWM functions assist encoding by orienting attention Reference de Fockert, Rees, Frith and Lavie(28,Reference Gazzaley, Clapp, Kelley, McEvoy, Knight and D'Esposito29) (and the eyes) on critical regions of the stimulus in order to complete the representation.
Behavioural results obtained in our previous study did not allow a clear assessment of the contribution of VSWM and oculomotor processes to the patients’ impaired performances. In order to do this, we reproduced our initial investigation using the VSWM task and combined it with an analysis of the participants’ exploratory eye movements during the encoding and the recognition phases. The present study involves an independent group of outpatients meeting diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia. It emphasised the following points: (a) comparison of the scanpath patterns of patients and control subjects during stimulus encoding in relation with subsequent choice efficacy, (b) detailed analysis of the scanpaths during stimuli discrimination in order to qualify the ability to build a coherent internal stimulus representation online and (c) assessment of a possible interaction between these two processes.
Method
Subjects
Sixteen individuals meeting diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia (4th ed., DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association) and 16 controls entered this study. Patients with schizophrenia were recruited from the outpatient population of the Psychiatric Hospital of Cery, Lausanne (Switzerland). As shown in Table 1, patients and controls were carefully matched for sex, handedness and age. Individuals with a history of neurological or medical illness likely to affect the nervous system, traumatic brain injury and reduced, uncorrected, visual acuity were excluded. All subjects in the control group reported no history of substance or alcohol abuse and were free from psychiatric illness or symptoms, as investigated by a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10 [Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview, see Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier and Sheehan(30)].
Table 1 Sociodemographic information of 16 patients with schizophrenia and 16 healthy comparison subjects
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160629091457-50714-mediumThumb-S0924270800000946_tab1.jpg?pub-status=live)
SD = standard deviation.
Participants were recruited in the region of Lausanne and provided with a complete description of the proposed study and given oral and written information. The University of Lausanne (School of Medicine) ethical commission approved the protocol and consent procedures. Participation to the study was voluntary and participants were rewarded 25 CHF after the study session.
All the patients were treated with antipsychotic drugs at the time of testing. Fourteen patients received atypical antipsychotic drugs [chlorpromazine equivalents 325.71 mg/day ± 204.50, see Reference Woods(31,Reference Leucht, Wahlbeck, Hamann and Kissling32)] and one received typical (chlorpromazine equivalent: 500 mg/day) medication. Data were not available for one patient.
Materials and procedure
Subjects (control and patients) performed a task designed to investigate VSWM processes involved in the encoding of neutral static stimuli composed of eight linear segments [for a detailed description, see Reference Cocchi, Schenk, Volken, Bovet, Parnas and Vianin(23)]. Stimuli were presented for 2 s on a screen (27.5°×20.8°, visual angle). They were composed of eight static line patterns (see Fig. 1). A blank delay lasting 2 s (white screen) at the end of the stimulus exposure was followed by the recognition slide displaying six patterns (the target pattern and five incorrect patterns) in a 3 × 2 grid. Upon the presentation of the discrimination slide, participants were asked to verbalise the number (from 1 to 6) corresponding to the target path. This slide was available as long as required. The task session was composed of 15 stimuli and the subjects did control the start of each new trial.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160629090726-10145-mediumThumb-S0924270800000946_fig1g.jpg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 1 Illustration of the working memory task design showing the time sequence for each trial. On the left panel are the three stimulus regions of interest (ROIs) (limits not visible during the presentation). On the right is the choice grid with six discrimination ROIs (see 1 to 6 in the discrimination slide, each rectangle represents an ROI). The exploration of the valid path was also analysed from fixations in three specific ROIs (reproducing the stimulus ROIs).
Five different incorrect patterns were designed to partially match each stimulus. The specific distortions leading to the five incorrect patterns allowed these to be distributed in two different categories: (a) fragmentary: patterns conserving two segments (first two: start patterns; last two: end patterns) or four segments (first and last two: start–end patterns) identical to those of the target trajectory; (b) global: patterns with either the same nine coordinates on the x axis (patterns named rhythm) or on the y axis (patterns named melody) as in the target stimulus. The incorrect patterns (melody and rhythm) in this group approximate the global structure of the target trajectory but they do not have identical segments.
A practice session was performed by all the subjects before beginning the test. Subjects performed trial examples until they demonstrated a clear understanding of the paradigm. Online visual analysis of eye behaviour during the practice and the task sessions was used to ensure that participants actively performed the task. One patient was excluded because of this control analysis.
Oculomotor data acquisition and analysis
Eye movements’ data were collected using an infrared technique (Applied Science Laboratory, H6 model, pupil-corneal reflection tracking, sampling rate 120 Hz, accuracy 0.5° and resolution of 0.1°). Visual stimuli were presented with a projector (resolution of 1280 × 1024, with refresh rate of 70 Hz). A 9-point calibration procedure was performed after the example, and at the end of the test, to ensure consistency of data collection. The head was not fixed during the tasks, but its position in space was recorded by a magnetic sensor integrated into the helmet (sampling rate of 103 Hz). Furthermore, eye movements’ data were automatically corrected for head movements.
Eye movement analyses were performed during two specific test phases (see Fig. 1): (a) the stimulus presentation (i.e. encoding) and (b) the discrimination of the correct pattern between the six possibilities. Visual scanning variables included the number of fixations (FX) and the scanning time (ST) in specific regions of interest (ROIs). Eye movement analyses were performed offline with ILAB Reference Gitelman(33). A fixation was defined as a result of consecutive x and y coordinates that remained within a diameter of 1.5° of visual angle for at least 150 ms Reference Phillips and David(7,Reference Kojima, Matsushima and Ando11). Blinks were identified by a loss of corneal reflection and were excluded from subsequent data analysis, as were gazes directed outside the screen.
Three ROIs were defined to analyse gaze distribution during the stimulus encoding (see Fig. 1). The first three stimuli segments composed of ROI 1, the two central segments, ROI 2, and the three last segments, ROI 3. For the discrimination phase, we defined six ROIs (discrimination ROI: 1 to 6) corresponding to the six grid cases containing the correct and the five incorrect patterns. Scanning time and FX distribution on the six specific ROIs as well as the distribution of FX on the correct pattern were investigated. The distribution of FX on the correct pattern, during the discrimination phase, was estimated by the same criteria used to define stimulus phase ROIs.
Statistical analysis
Memory performance
T-test analyses supported group comparisons in task performance. Mixed between- and within-subjects two-way ANOVAs were used to analyse between-group (patients vs. controls) difference in incorrect patterns choices. One-way repeated measure ANOVAs were used to investigate within-group differences. Data sphericity was tested using Mauchly's approach. When sphericity was not respected, we employed a Huynh–Feldt correction to rectify the ANOVA results. Chi-squared tests of the distribution of correct choices for the 15 stimuli were performed in each group to assess if all the stimuli were equally difficult. Cohen's d and partial [eta]2 were used to estimate effect size.
Eye and head movements
Stimulus phase. Mann–Whitney (MW) and Wilcoxon tests were used to test the number of fixations between and within the groups. These nonparametric approaches were used because data normality was not confirmed. Horizontal and vertical head movements were assessed from the difference found in each subject for each stimulus between the eye–head movement correlations (MW). The distribution of fixations in the three ROIs of the stimuli was analysed using a mixed two-way ANOVA (groups × three ROIs as repeated factor). Independent sample t-tests were used as post hoc analysis. Cohen's d and partial [eta]2 were used to estimate effect size.
Discrimination phase. Mixed between- and within-subjects two-way ANOVAs on FX and ST were used for the following: (a) to compare group visual exploratory patterns in the discrimination ROIs and (b) to compare the exploration of the correct patterns in patients and controls. Specific post hoc comparisons (one-way repeated measure ANOVAs and t-tests) were performed to investigate significant global differences in mixed two-way ANOVAs. Cohen's d and partial [eta]2 were used to estimate effect size.
Results
Memory performance
The distribution of correct choices in each of the 15 trajectories was homogenous in both groups (X 2 = 8.23, df = 14, p > 0.75 for controls; X 2 = 2.51, df = 14, p > 0.99 for patients). Hence, all the 15 stimuli offered a similar difficulty. Overall, patients made significantly fewer correct choices [average (SD) 10.6± 2.5] than did controls (13.0± 1.4). This difference was statistically significant [t(30) = 3.281, d = 1.16,p = 0.003].
A mixed two-way ANOVA of error categories (two groups × five categories as repeated factor) showed a significant group effect [i.e. more wrong choices overall in the patients F(1, 30) = 10.765, partial [eta]2 = 0.264,p = 0.003] but no significant error category effect [F(4, 120) = 1.652, partial [eta]2 = 0.05,p = 0.166] or group × category interaction [F(4, 120) = 1.252, partial [eta]2 = 0.04,p = 0.293].
Oculomotor behaviour
Stimulus phase (stimulus encoding)
Controls and patients showed a similar number of fixations during the presentation of the subsequently wrongly identified stimuli (MW Z = −1.638,p = 0.101). When exploring the correctly recognised stimuli, however, the patients made more fixations in comparison to controls (MW Z = −2.507;p = 0.012). Nevertheless, no group exhibited significant differences in number of fixations between correct and incorrect trials (Wilcoxon test: controls Z = −0.853;p = 0.394; patients Z = −0.894,p = 0.371). There was also no significant correlation between the number of fixations and the number of correct responses (controls: Spearman's rho = 0.195, p = 0.469; patients: Spearman's rho = −0.101, p = 0.709).
Figure 2 shows the number of fixations in each of the three ROIs during the presentation of the subsequently correctly (Fig. 2a) and incorrectly (Fig. 2b) recognised stimuli. The variability appears more important during the exploration of the incorrectly recognised stimuli in both patients and controls. The control subjects showed decreased exploration from the left to the right ROIs in the correctly recognised stimuli. This strategy was not evident in the patient group.
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160629090954-63748-mediumThumb-S0924270800000946_fig2g.jpg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 2 (a) Mean and error bar (confidence intervals, 95%) of the number of fixations performed by controls and patients with schizophrenia during the encoding of the correctly recognised stimulus. Patients made less correct recognitions (maximum 240, grand total N = 170) compared to controls (N = 209). (b) Same representation for the visual exploration of the incorrectly recognised stimuli (grand total, patients N = 70; controls N = 31).
The distribution of fixations in the three ROIs of the correctly recognised stimuli was analysed using a mixed two-way ANOVA (groups × ROIs). This analysis revealed an interaction between group and ROI [F(2, 754) = 5.143; partial [eta]2 = 0.013,p = 0.006]. A similar interaction was found for scantime [F(1.943, 732.576) = 3.655, partial [eta]2 = 0.01,p = 0.028]. Post hoc independent sample t-tests showed that patients explored the second (central) stimulus ROI significantly more than did the controls [FX t(377) = −4.168, d = 0.82,p< 0.001; ST t(377) = −2.257, d = 0.23,p = 0.025; see Fig. 2a]. In contrast, the two groups showed no marked difference in their exploration of the incorrectly recognised stimuli (FX and ST, see Fig. 2b).
Because of a technical problem in head movement output file generation, head movements could only be analysed for 11 patients and 10 controls. This problem had no influence on the correction of eye movements for head movements, automatically performed during the eye movements recording. The average number of positive correlations between eye and head movements during stimuli encoding were similar in the two groups (horizontal axis: Z = −0.424;p = 0.672; vertical axis: Z = −0.166;p = 0.868).
Discrimination phase
Given that response grid durations were not equivalent for all participants, the original data of each subject were related to its total number of fixations and scanning time (%) in the six ROIs. Visual explorations of the six patterns were analysed separately for the trials with a correct or an incorrect choice. In addition, we analysed whether visual exploration of the correct pattern in correct and incorrect trials was different in the two groups.
Correct trials. A group (control vs. patients) by ROIs (six discrimination ROIs) mixed two-way ANOVA showed a large ROI effect [FX: F(1, 3.734) = 272.305, partial [eta]2 = 0.440,p< 0.001; ST: F(5.696, 1456.260) = 451.277, partial [eta]2 = 0.566,p< 0.001], suggesting the preferential exploration of the correct patterns. There was an incongruent group effect for each of the two visual scanning variables [FX: F(1, 347)= 13.830, partial [eta]2 = 0.038,p< 0.001; ST: F(1, 346) = 0.001, partial [eta]2< 0.001, p = 0.99] and no interaction between group and ROI for either visual scanning variables [FX: F(3.734, 1295.798) = 1.947, partial [eta]2 = 0.006,p = 0.105; ST: F(4.209, 1456.260) = 1.078, partial [eta]2 = 0.003,p = 0.367; see Fig. 3a].
![](https://static.cambridge.org/binary/version/id/urn:cambridge.org:id:binary-alt:20160629091225-11439-mediumThumb-S0924270800000946_fig3g.jpg?pub-status=live)
Fig. 3 Mean and confidence intervals (95%) of the relative number of fixations performed by controls and patients throughout the discrimination phase. N = total number of data for mean computation. ‘+’ indicates significant within-group differences; ‘*’ indicates significant between-group differences.
Within-group one-way repeated measure ANOVAs (six discrimination ROIs as repeated factor) and subsequent post hoc analyses revealed differential exploration of the six discrimination ROIs in the two groups. Indeed, patients and controls explore more the ROI containing the correct pattern in comparison to the other five patterns (paired sample t-test: p< 0.01 for all the five correct vs. incorrect patterns of ROI comparison, FX and ST; see Fig. 3a).
A group (control vs. patients) by ROIs (five ‘incorrect’ discrimination ROIs) mixed two-way ANOVA revealed a significant interaction between group and ROI factors for the number of fixations [FX: F(3.933, 1364.622) = 3.223, partial [eta]2 = 0.009,p = 0.013]. As shown in Fig. 3a, the patients made significantly more fixations on Rhythm (t(347) = −3.644, d = 0.38,p< 0.001) and Start (t(347) = −3.147, d = 0.33,p< 0.002) incorrect patterns.
Incorrect trials. Figure 3b suggests that the patient group paid more attention to the correct pattern even though it was not selected.
A group (control vs. patients) by ROIs (six discrimination ROIs as repeated factor) mixed two-way ANOVA revealed the expected ROI effect [FX: F(4.623, 596.403) = 6.322, partial [eta]2 = 0.047, p< 0.001; ST: F(4.817, 621.380) = 3.664, partial [eta]2 = 0.028,p = 0.003] but failed to show a significant interaction between ROI and group [FX: F(4.623, 596.403) = 1.165, partial [eta]2 = 0.009,p = 0.315; ST: F(4.817, 621.380) = 2.048, partial [eta]2 = 0.016,p = 0.073 see Fig. 3b]. However, independent samples t-test and effect size exploratory analyses suggest that patients tended to spend more time in scanning the correct patterns than did controls [t(129) = −2.390, d = 0.52,p = 0.018]. This is consistent with the within-groups analysis showing that control subjects explored the six discrimination ROIs equally [FX: F(5, 150) = 1.450, partial [eta]2 = 0.042,p = 0.210; ST F(5, 150) = 0.315, partial [eta]2 = 0.01,p = 0.903; see Fig. 3b], whereas patients’ exploration was unequally distributed [FX: F(4.628, 458.194) = 11.045, partial [eta]2 = 0.1,p< 0.001; ST F(4.576, 453.061) = 11.206, partial [eta]2 = 0.102,p< 0.001]. Post hoc analyses of the patient group confirmed that the correct pattern was more explored than the five incorrect patterns (paired sample t-test: p< 0.001 for all comparisons of FX and ST; see Fig. 3b for FX).
Visual exploration of the correct pattern during correct and incorrect trials. Mixed two-way ANOVAs (group and three ROIs as repeated factor) revealed no significant effect of the group or ROI and no interaction between these factors for correct and incorrect trials. These results indicate that patients’ and controls’ explorations of the correct pattern during the discrimination period was similar.
Discussion
Consistent with our previous research Reference Cocchi, Schenk, Volken, Bovet, Parnas and Vianin(23), we found that patients made a higher number of incorrect choices than did the control subjects. Detailed analysis of eye movements revealed that, for correct recognitions, control subjects spent most of the encoding phase fixating the leftmost part of the stimulus and very little time on its rightmost part. Because all subjects in this study were native French speakers, this linear exploration from the left to the right side of the stimulus can be related to occidental reading skills Reference Liversedge and Findlay(34). During the discrimination phase, controls were mainly fixating on the correct rather than the five incorrect patterns. However, when an incorrect pattern was chosen, the gaze distribution was roughly the same for all six patterns, but there was a high degree of variability suggesting poor stimulus representation.
Analysis of the patients’ fixations during the encoding phase showed that, unlike controls, they concentrated particularly on the central ROI when they subsequently recognised the pattern. In contrast, this fixation pattern was absent during the encoding phase of the incorrect trials, suggesting a displacement in attentional focus during encoding of the correct trials. Like controls, however, the patients spent a minimal time in the rightmost ROI during the encoding of all stimuli. During the discrimination phase, the patients differed from the controls in two aspects. Firstly, when selecting the correct patterns, they were particularly attracted by the rhythm and start incorrect patterns. Secondly, they also spent more time inspecting the correct pattern, when subsequently selecting an incorrect one. This specific oculomotor behaviour suggests that the accuracy of the global target representation within VSWM is reduced in patients. Though the patients’ visual exploratory strategy differed from that of control subjects, their encoding was sufficient to maintain a relatively high—though diminished—level of accuracy, 70% compared to a chance level of 1 out of 6.
In our view, the selective scanning of the central ROI shown by patients during the correct stimuli encoding reveals a specific VSWM encoding strategy. Three main findings suggest that this specific encoding style of the patients is not a direct consequence of a basic oculomotor dysfunction. Firstly, when patients performed a scanning pattern similar to that of the control subjects during correct trials, they made an incorrect response. Secondly, patients explored the three stimulus ROIs at least as controls, in both correct and incorrect trials. Finally, the total number of fixations during stimulus encoding was not associated with the task score, nor was it associated with accuracy in either group.
The majority of experimental designs fix the head during eye movements recording. This artificial constraint prevents the detection of possible compensation of impaired oculomotor processes by head adjustments. As we simultaneously recorded eye and head movements, we observed that head movements during stimulus exploration were not different between the groups, and were not related to impaired performance.
Analysis of eye movements during the discrimination phase showed that correct responses were linked to selective scanning of the correct pattern. For patients, however, significant time was also spent scanning two incorrect patterns: the start patterns, expressing the first part of the stimulus, and the rhythm patterns, approximating its global structure Reference Cocchi, Schenk, Volken, Bovet, Parnas and Vianin(23). This interest in incorrect patterns with a ‘global’ structure of the stimulus supports the hypothesis that patients have a general, but imprecise, global internal representation of the stimulus because of the high contribution of early parallel visual encoding processes (e.g. Reference Altmann, Bulthoff and Kourtzi(35)). The assumption that patients relied on a less accurate global representation of the target stimulus is also coherent with the larger number of explorations of the invalid start patterns in comparison to controls.
During the discrimination phase of incorrect trials, control subjects paid little attention to the correct patterns, indicating poor or incomplete stimulus representation. When combined with the large variability in gaze during stimulus encoding and the low number of incorrect choices (13%), this result suggests that controls’ incorrect choices are probably due to a transitory lack of attention or to a miscoding of the target stimulus. In contrast, patients remained attracted by the correct pattern even though they selected another. These findings are also consistent with the assumption that patients’ erroneous choices are mainly due to a failure to complete the early global stimuli representation because of limited VSWM resources.
Recent studies show that ageing is accompanied by a significant reduction in VSWM, affecting attentional resources and perceptual processes (Reference Gazzaley, Clapp, Kelley, McEvoy, Knight and D'Esposito29,Reference Gazzaley, Cooney, Rissman and D'Esposito36, Reference Gazzaley and D'Esposito37). Therefore, a specific ocular motor behaviour during static stimuli encoding can also be present in healthy aged subjects. Further studies are needed to investigate the specificity of the compensatory encoding mechanisms in schizophrenia, especially in ultra high risk and first episode samples, in order to potentially identify new diagnostic and outcome markers. Understanding particular compensatory processes in subjects with impaired VSWM functions (such as patients with schizophrenia and healthy aged individuals) might also suggest specific intervention in order to improve VSWM abilities .
This study had a number of limitations. Firstly, we could not analyse the relationship between symptoms, visual scanning and VSWM due to limited information about ongoing symptoms at the time of testing. Recent findings suggest that specific symptoms can influence these variables Reference Minassian, Granholm, Verney and Perry(13,Reference Nishiura, Morita, Kurakake, Igimi and Maeda38)—however, the fact that the patients included in this study were in a stable clinical condition probably attenuates the confounding effect of symptoms on our results. Furthermore, all patients were taking antipsychotic medication. Considering the small sample size and the large heterogeneity of medication, we did not consider this variable as a covariate in statistical analysis. In addition, the influence of antipsychotic medication on oculomotor processes Reference Hutton, Huddy and Barnes(18,Reference Gaebel, Ulrich and Frick39), visual processing Reference Butler, Harkavy-Friedman, Amador and Gorman(40) and cognitive processes Reference Daban, Amado and Bourdel(41) is unclear. Finally, we were not able to match the level of education between controls and patients. However, unpublished behavioural results on healthy subjects (n = 36) suggest that this variable is not related to task performance.
In summary, our results confirm the important deficit of patients with schizophrenia in VSWM and suggest that patients try to compensate for reduced VSWM ability by using specific encoding strategies Reference Cocchi, Schenk, Volken, Bovet, Parnas and Vianin(23). Impaired VSWM ability and specific encoding strategies in schizophrenia do not seem a mere consequence of basic oculomotor dysfunctions .
Acknowledgements
We thank the patients and control persons who volunteered in this study. The authors especially thank Prof. Pierre Bovet and Dr François Grasset for allowing us to test patients under their care and Dr Pascal Vianin and Jerôme Favrod for their efficient collaboration. The Swiss National Foundation for Scientific Research supported this study (Prof. F. Schenk) 3100A0—105765 and (Dr L. Cocchi) PBLAB—119622. Dr Wood is supported by a Clinical Career Development Award from the NHMRC (Australia). The Robert Bosch Research Institute of Lonay (Switzerland) supported this study by providing technological facilities for eye movement recording and analysis.