Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-kw2vx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-10T20:49:30.356Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 13 - Emotional Labor in Parenting

from Part IV - Current Trends

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2024

Isabelle Roskam
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
James J. Gross
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Moïra Mikolajczak
Affiliation:
Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium

Summary

Today’s parents are subject to strict rules about how to raise their children. Some rules focus on parents’ emotions: Parents are bound by emotional display rules, according to which “ideal parents” should be “emotionally perfect” – showing positive emotions and refraining from negative ones while interacting with their children. This chapter introduces Lin and colleagues’ pioneering efforts in borrowing the emotional labor framework to shed light on the consequences for parents of their subjection to these emotional display rules. The emotional labor framework, initially proposed by sociologists, holds that employees must consciously align their feelings with emotional display rules when interacting with customers. This framework provides insight into the mechanisms underlying the relation between emotional labor performed by employees and their well-being. This chapter presents the application of the emotional labor framework to the parenting context and outlines future research directions that this approach opens. We believe that extending the emotional labor framework to the context of parenting opens a promising research avenue in the parenting field.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

In her groundbreaking book, The Managed Heart: The Commercialization of Human Feelings, Arlie Russell Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983) described the phenomenon of an organization delineating a set of emotion display rules and pressuring employees to hide their identity in order to comply with them. As Goffman (Reference Goffman1959) had discussed earlier, people play their social roles just like in a drama; and employees, Hochschild noted, follow emotional display rules, showing “appropriate” emotions and refraining from expressing “inappropriate” emotions to control the impression they make on others, maintain relationships, and achieve personal goals (Thoits, Reference Thoits, Manstead, Frijda and Fischer2004; von Scheve, Reference von Scheve2012; Wharton & Erickson, Reference Wharton and Erickson1993). Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) suggested that a similar phenomenon occurs in modern parenting, given the recent culture change in parenting (see Hays, Reference Hays1996): society defines a set of rules for emotion management in parenting and pressures parents to hide their identity in order to conform to these rules and expectations. In this chapter, our goal is to review the literature that supports Lin and colleagues’ study on emotional labor in parenting.

13.1 Modern Parenting

13.1.1 From Common Sense to Science

Parenting has undergone a major shift in recent decades. This can be traced back to the late twentieth century, when enormous economic and political changes occurred that were directly or indirectly caused by the two world wars (Overy, Reference Overy2009): the triumph of capitalism (Habib, Reference Habib1995), the generalization of the democratic spirit (Kettenacker & Riotte, Reference Kettenacker and Riotte2011), United Nations General Assembly, 1948), and the emergence of globalization (Barkawi, Reference Barkawi2006). These factors have intersected, eliminating authority and prompting people to doubt their own traditions (Giddens, Reference Giddens1999; Hobsbawm, Reference Hobsbawm1995). In parallel, science has acquired a higher status and it is to science (instead of traditions) that many people now turn to know how they should run their lives (Giddens, Reference Giddens, Beck, Giddens and Lash1994), including parenting. Science can check whether the evidence supports traditional behavior, for example, the “common sense” practices that parents used to feel obliged to follow in parenting. Based on its findings, science can suggest that a specific traditional practice be abolished or preserved. Alternatively, science can advocate innovative parenting strategies. In any case, recent decades have witnessed science playing a more critical role in instructing parents on child-rearing (Furedi, Reference Furedi2002; Pursell, Reference Pursell2007).

Science relating to the parenting domain has flourished for decades. Since the mid-nineteenth century, scientists have come to view children not only as unique and important, but also as fragile beings who require extra protective effort (Hoghughi, Reference Hoghughi, Hoghughi and Long2004). This ideology became more widespread after the Second World War. Many scholars, such as John Bowlby and Donald Winnicott, began to emphasize the importance of high-quality child-rearing and a sound mother–child relationship, contending that they create a supportive and warm family and thus ensure the harmonious development of children (Hendrick, Reference Hendrick2016). Such an emphasis – along with empirical research on the effects of parenting on children – has attributed to parenting a key, if not a decisive role in children’s development (Bornstein, Reference Bornstein, Bornstein and Leventhal2015). This ideology has been intensified to the point where some even believe that parents’ actions and choices with regard to their children irreversibly influence children’s development (for a critical discussion, see Furedi, Reference Furedi2002). Belief in the importance of parenting in child development has thus reached unprecedented levels and continues to grow (Lee, Reference Lee, Lee, Bristow, Faircloth and Macvarish2014b).

13.1.2 From Science to Prescribed Rules in Parenting

Given the importance of parenting, many people believe that parents should implement “correct parenting,” namely the practices approved by science and society (for related discussion; see Furedi, Reference Furedi2002; Lee, Reference Lee, Lee, Bristow, Faircloth and Macvarish2014a). Parenting has gradually become prescribed. The prescriptions concern what parents should do (e.g. provide their children with an emotionally secure environment, give them five helpings of fruit and vegetables a day); and what they should not do (e.g. use corporal or disproportionate punishment, put their very young children in front of screens). Along with these prescriptions there are expectations in terms of results: for instance, children should be physically healthy, emotionally secure, etc. If parents fail to meet some of these expectations, the authorities may punish them and remove their children from their custody. Some parents are terrified that they may be assessed as failing in their role (e.g. being judged as neglectful parents) and that they could receive a dire sanction: loss of custody of the child (Nomaguchi & Milkie, Reference Nomaguchi and Milkie2020, p. 200). The fulfilment of the parental role is now more or less controlled by implicit or explicit prescriptions.

13.1.3 Emotional Display Rules in Parenting

Among these prescriptions, particular attention is paid to parental emotions. This is not surprising if we consider the current scientific evidence concerning parental emotions. First, parenting occurs in a social context conducive to spontaneous emotional feelings. For example, research has shown that mothers report a greater variety of discrete emotions when they care for their children than when they do not (Kerr et al., Reference Kerr, Rasmussen, Buttitta, Smiley and Borelli2021). These emotions may be self-oriented, such as anger at themselves for not giving the child enough time and attention, or child oriented, such as anger at the child for not keeping their room tidy (Dix et al., Reference Dix, Gershoff, Meunier and Miller2004). Often, parenting activities evoke more negative emotions than other activities (Kahneman et al., Reference Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz and Stone2004), causing parents to experience more worry, stress, and anger than non-parents (Deaton & Stone, Reference Deaton and Stone2014). Parents’ negative emotions undermine parenting strategies, making them less supportive/positive and harsher (for a meta-analysis, see Rueger et al., Reference Rueger, Katz, Risser and Lovejoy2011), predicting poorer adjustment of children later on (e.g. more school aggression; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Schwartz, Dodge and McBride-Chang2003).

Second, how parents express and regulate emotions plays a crucial role in children’s social emotional development (Duncombe et al., Reference Duncombe, Havighurst, Holland and Frankling2012). On the one hand, how parents handle their own emotions acts as a model that provides children with important information about how to appropriately recognize, express, and regulate emotions in specific situations, which in turn socializes children’s emotion regulation (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Morris et al., Reference Morris, Criss, Silk and Houltberg2017). Research has shown that parents’ capacity for adaptive emotion regulation is associated with better emotion regulation and social adjustment in children (for a meta-analysis, see Zimmer-Gembeck et al., Reference Zimmer-Gembeck, Rudolph, Kerin and Bohadana-Brown2022). On the other hand, how parents respond to their children’s emotions is also crucial. Research has demonstrated that a warm and supportive parental response to children’s emotions facilitates children’s acquisition of emotional knowledge and social competence, whereas harsh and unsupportive responses are detrimental to children’s social-emotional development (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Fabes and Murphy1996, Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998, Reference Eisenberg, Fabes, Shepard, Guthrie, Murphy and Reiser1999; Hajal & Paley, Reference Hajal and Paley2020; see also Chapter 10).

This scientific evidence converges and shapes emotional display rules that revolve around parental emotions. Parents are now expected to feel and express the “right” emotions, with the “right” intensity and in the “right” situations. To this end, parents are increasingly encouraged to manage their emotion expression during their interactions with their children. They should refrain from expressing too many negative emotions, such as fear (which can make their relationship with their children insecure; e.g. Manassis et al., Reference Manassis, Bradley, Goldberg, Hood and Swinson1994) or anger; and they should also express more positive emotions such as warmth and affection, to sustain their children’s emotional safety (e.g. Bai et al., Reference Bai, Repetti and Sperling2016). Such prescriptions have spread and prevailed to the point where they now constitute a crucial aspect of parenting culture. A survey in 37 countries of more than 10,000 parents showed that Western parents believe that to be an ideal parent, they should show positive emotions and control their negative emotions (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Mikolajczak, Keller, Akgun, Arikan, Aunola, Barham, Besson, Blanchard, Boujut, Brianda, Brytek-Matera, César, Chen, Dorard, dos Santos Elias, Dunsmuir, Egorova, Escobar and Roskam2023).

Based on these observations, one may want to ask the following questions. What are the possible results of regulating parental emotion? Should parents simply follow these prescriptions and carry them out? Is compliance with them costly? Aiming to answer these questions, Lin et al. (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) came up with a pioneering adaptation to the parenting context of the emotional labor framework initially developed in the context of work (e.g. Grandey et al., Reference Grandey, Diefendorffand and Rupp2013). They borrowed this framework in order to describe the impact of display rules governing parents’ emotions and to summarize the consequences of complying with those rules. Before describing their adaptation, we first introduce the emotional labor framework in the work context below.

13.2 Emotional Labor Framework

13.2.1 The Origin of the Concept of Emotional Labor

The emotional labor concept dates from the 1980s, when Arlie Russell Hochschild, an American sociologist, published her foundational book, The Managed Heart: The Commercialization of Human Feelings (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983). Hochschild described in detail the job requirements of Delta Airlines flight attendants. What drew her attention was that to ensure that passengers felt comfortable, the flight attendants were expected to express appropriate emotions regardless of how they felt and how the passengers behaved. They were expected to possess traits such as emotional stability, interpersonal warmth, concern for others, and a collective orientation. These qualities allowed them to employ what Hochschild termed “emotional management,” that is, cognitive, behavioral, and expressive strategies that enable them to align their emotional experiences and expressions with the feelings and rules of expression required by the organization. Based on this observation, Hochschild concluded that certain jobs are not only defined by the physical or cognitive work performed but also involve emotional management. Hochschild termed such emotional management – regulating and expressing the “right” emotions to satisfy the emotional requirements of the job in exchange for a wage – as “emotional labor.”

13.2.2 The Definition of Emotional Labor

Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983, p. 7) described emotional labor as “the management of feelings to create a publicly observable facial and bodily manifestation.” Three main characteristics of jobs involving emotional labor were also enumerated: (1) they involve a high level of direct contact with customers (“voice to voice” or “face to face”) in terms of duration, frequency, or intensity of interaction (Morris & Feldman, Reference Morris and Feldman1996); (2) they require the use of specific emotional displays (either explicitly or implicitly specified) to elicit the desired affective responses from customers; and (3) the organization directly or indirectly controls employees’ emotional displays.

13.2.3 The Emotional Labor Framework in the Work Context

Hochschild’s (Reference Hochschild1983) pioneering work has inspired a great deal of research and theoretical work, resulting in several detailed emotional labor models (Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Grandey & Melloy, Reference Grandey and Melloy2017; Morris & Feldman, Reference Morris and Feldman1997; Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Tardino, Daus, Munz, Hartel, Zerbe and Ashkanasy2005; Totterdell & Holman, Reference Totterdell and Holman2003). Despite the differences in these theoretical models, researchers have concurred that emotional labor is a form of emotional management requiring effort from employees and should be positioned within a broader integrative framework. This framework includes (1) emotional demands of work (e.g. Morris & Feldman, Reference Morris and Feldman1996) as an antecedent to emotional dissonance experienced when perceived emotions do not match the demands of the job (e.g. Abraham, Reference Abraham1999; Zerbe, Reference Zerbe2000), causing (2) the employee to make an effort to engage in emotion regulation through deep and surface acting (Bono & Vey, Reference Bono, Vey, Härtel, Zerbe and Ashkanasy2005; Scott & Barnes, Reference Scott and Barnes2011),Footnote 1 and to produce the desired emotional displays, ultimately leading to (3) consequences for the employee (see Figure 13.1).

Figure 13.1 Emotional labor framework in the job context

13.2.3.1 Emotional Demands of Work: Emotional Display Rules

The cornerstone of all emotional labor models is the view that organizations set emotional display rules that specify which emotions are appropriate and how and when they should be expressed during interactions with customers (Ashforth & Humphrey, Reference Ashforth and Humphrey1993; Cropanzano et al., Reference Cropanzano, Weiss, Elias, Perrewe and Ganster2003; Diefendorff et al., Reference Diefendorff, Richard and Croyle2006; Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Rafaeli & Sutton, Reference Rafaeli and Sutton1990). Emotional display rules are imposed on employees as early as the recruitment process and are later reinforced by activities such as training, performance appraisal, and supervision (Kruml & Geddes, Reference Kruml and Geddes2000; Sutton & Rafaeli, Reference Sutton and Rafaeli1988). These rules have both a positive aspect, requiring employees to show certain emotions (e.g. “Put a smile on your face!” or “Show interest and excitement!”) and a negative aspect, prohibiting employees from showing certain emotions (e.g. “Don’t raise your voice!” or “Don’t show boredom!”).

Such emotional display rules – requiring employees to conceal negative emotions and express positive emotions – is especially common in customer service work (Wharton & Erickson, Reference Wharton and Erickson1993). The organization sets out these rules based on the assumption that employee behavior influences customer satisfaction and interest in the products or services offered (Grandey et al., Reference Grandey, Fisk, Mattila, Jansen and Sideman2005), and studies have in fact shown this to be true. When employees directly follow the rules and perform the emotional behavior expected by their customers such as greeting, thanking, speaking in a rhythmic voice, smiling, and maintaining eye contact (Grandey, Reference Grandey2003; Tsai, Reference Tsai2001), customers are in a better mood (Luong, Reference Luong2005), buy more, rate the service better, and are more loyal to the organization (Korczynski, Reference Korczynski2005; Pugh, Reference Pugh2001; Tsai, Reference Tsai2001).

13.2.3.2 Emotional Labor and Its Implication for Employee Well-Being

However, what happens when the emotions employees experience are not the same as those they are required to display? Researchers argue that incongruence between the emotions experienced and the expressions expected by the organization induces a negatively affect-laden state of emotional dissonance in employees (Holman et al., Reference Holman, Martinez-Iñigo, Totterdell, Ashkanasy and Cooper2008; Zapf & Holz, Reference Zapf and Holz2006). Jansz and Timmers (Reference Jansz and Timmers2002) point out that the concept of emotional dissonance underlines both the negative nature of this state and its motivational implications. When they experience a feeling of dissonance, employees are motivated to take action to reduce this feeling, which has implications for regulating the emotional process. Undertaking a regulatory effort is vital because emotional dissonance, if prolonged, threatens employees’ mental and physical well-being (Dijk & Brown, Reference Dijk and Brown2006; Hartel et al., Reference Hartel, Hsu, Boyle, Ashkanasy, Zerbe and Hartel2002; Schaubroeck & Jones, Reference Schaubroeck and Jones2000). However, the impact of this regulatory effort may depend on how it is made.

Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983) followed Goffman’s (Reference Goffman1959) metaphor of different ways of acting in a drama, describing two ways in which employees work to regulate their emotions, that is, two emotional labor strategies: surface acting and deep acting. Surface acting refers to modifying emotional expressions without changing internal feelings. It involves suppressing the display of felt emotions and faking the emotion required by the organization. Deep acting, on the other hand, refers to modifying actual feelings so that they are consistent with the employer’s emotional display rules. It entails an effort to change the felt emotion in order to elicit the appropriate emotional display. As discussed later, the distinction between surface and depth acting is critical in demonstrating and explaining the varying impact of emotional labor on employees’ well-being (see also Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Grandey & Gabriel, Reference Grandey and Gabriel2015; Grandey & Melloy, Reference Grandey and Melloy2017; Scott & Barnes, Reference Scott and Barnes2011).

Following Hochschild’s (Reference Hochschild1983) description of emotional labor strategies, studies have accumulated evidence that the two forms differ in their effects on employees’ well-being. Most studies have found that surface acting reliably and consistently predicts unfavorable consequences such as worse job performance, less job satisfaction, more psychological stress, more psychosomatic complaints, and more burnout symptoms (Bono & Vey, Reference Bono, Vey, Härtel, Zerbe and Ashkanasy2005; Grandey & Gabriel, Reference Grandey and Gabriel2015; Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011; Huppertz et al., Reference Huppertz, Ute, Velozo and Schreurs2020; Kammeyer-Mueller et al., Reference Kammeyer-Mueller, Rubenstein, Long, Odio, Buckman, Zhang and Halvorsen-Ganepola2013); however, the findings regarding the effect of deep acting are not as consistent across studies (Grandey, Reference Grandey2000; Grandey & Sayre, Reference Grandey and Sayre2019). Although some studies have shown a damaging effect of deep acting (although it is still thought to be less harmful than surface acting; Mikolajczak et al., Reference Mikolajczak, Menil and Luminet2007), most studies have found that deep acting is neither beneficial nor detrimental (e.g. Brotheridge & Lee, Reference Brotheridge and Lee2002; Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011).

The concept of “regulatory effort “ further explains the different consequences of surface and deep acting (Huppertz et al., Reference Huppertz, Ute, Velozo and Schreurs2020). Although surface and deep acting are both effortful, the amount of regulatory effort they require differs. In particular, if individuals rely more on surface acting to meet the organization’s rules, their effort and cognitive resources are more strained due to the need to monitor emotional expression constantly. The constant depletion of resources eventually causes tension and strain; if it becomes chronic, it can result in burnout (Brotheridge & Grandey, Reference Brotheridge and Grandey2002). Deep acting also requires cognitive resources to manage emotions, but these efforts are only needed at the beginning; this explains the weaker predictive relationship of deep acting with poor adaptation (see Grandey & Sayre, Reference Grandey and Sayre2019).

13.3 Extending the Emotional Labor Framework to Parenting

13.3.1 Rationale

Does emotional labor also occur in parenting? Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983) had already foreseen this possibility in her seminal book. She suggested that emotional labor may occur in the professional context and in private life, such as in the family. Hochschild pointed out that of all relationships in the family, emotional labor may be most pronounced in the parent–child relationship, given the strong bond between parents and their children (therefore, more contacts). However, for decades the idea remained at the stage of Hochschild’s early reflections and some pioneering work (see Wharton & Erickson, Reference Wharton and Erickson1993, Reference Wharton and Erickson1995 for how emotional labor may be performed within the family). Research on emotional labor has so far mainly focused on the professional sphere, and the assumption seems fairly widespread that emotional labor is only performed in return for salary, bonuses, or rewards (Grandey & Melloy, Reference Grandey and Melloy2017; Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011; Kammeyer-Mueller et al., Reference Kammeyer-Mueller, Rubenstein, Long, Odio, Buckman, Zhang and Halvorsen-Ganepola2013; von Scheve, Reference von Scheve2012). However, with recent developments in parenting, this picture is changing (see Section 13.1, Modern Parenting); emotional labor in parenting may have been a long-standing phenomenon that has been ignored because it is a taboo subject. In a pioneering study, however, Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) extended the emotional labor framework to the parenting context (see Figure 13.2).

Figure 13.2 Emotional labor framework in the parenting context

Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) point out that features of contemporary parenting present characteristics of emotional labor. First, the parent–child relationship is one of the closest social relationships (it includes “voice-to-voice,” “face-to-face,” and even “body-to-body” interaction; Bornstein, Reference Bornstein, Bornstein and Leventhal2015). Second, as also briefly summarized in Section 13.1, Modern Parenting, modern parents are now increasingly expected to regulate their emotions during interaction with children. This phenomenon is particularly evident in Western countries, where mothers and fathers are supposed to show positive emotions (e.g. being loving) and maintain patience to be considered ideal parents (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Mikolajczak, Keller, Akgun, Arikan, Aunola, Barham, Besson, Blanchard, Boujut, Brianda, Brytek-Matera, César, Chen, Dorard, dos Santos Elias, Dunsmuir, Egorova, Escobar and Roskam2023). Third, such emotional display rules have been explicitly expressed in government policies such as the Council of Europe’s policy on positive parenting (Rodrigo, Reference Rodrigo2010) or the positive parenting tips recommended by the National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities (2020), etc. The implementation of these rules is therefore institutionally monitored and controlled. As we have seen, these characteristics are consistent with Hochschild’s (Reference Hochschild1983) observation on the characteristics/determinants of emotional labor in the work context, confirming that the concept is eligible to be applied in parenting.

Given such eligibility, Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) proposed their framework model of “emotional labor in parenting.” As they describe in this framework, society, like an employer, sets explicit rules on emotional expression. And just as employees need to follow emotional display rules to meet organizational expectations and to be recognized as good employees, parents need to follow emotional display rules to meet societal and institutional expectations and to be recognized as parents who are at least good enough (and preferably ideal). As in the work context, these efforts may lead to an immediate beneficial outcome, that is, children may feel positive just as customers do when they observe positive emotional expressions from employees; yet these efforts may eventually jeopardize parental well-being, for example by leading to parental burnout. Together with other studies, the research by Lin and colleagues provides support for their proposal, as described next.

13.3.1.1 Emotional Demands of Parenting: Emotional Display Rules

To begin with, as we saw in Section 13.1, Modern Parenting, social institutions such as governments (e.g. Rodrigo, Reference Rodrigo2010) and media (see Douglas & Michaels, Reference Douglas and Michaels2004) have presented parents with a specific set of emotional display rules. As we might expect, Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) have shown that some parents internalize these rules to the extent that, as shown in a later study, parents think they need to show positive emotions and control their negative emotions in order to be perceived as an ideal parent (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Mikolajczak, Keller, Akgun, Arikan, Aunola, Barham, Besson, Blanchard, Boujut, Brianda, Brytek-Matera, César, Chen, Dorard, dos Santos Elias, Dunsmuir, Egorova, Escobar and Roskam2023). It is true that parents may make efforts to align their expressed emotions with the rules, and that this may indeed result in better parenting strategies (Minnotte et al., Reference Minnotte, Pedersen, Mannon and Kiger2010). Such strategies may eventually benefit children in terms of improving their emotional state (Olszanowski et al., Reference Olszanowski, Wróbel and Hess2020) and subsequent social functioning (e.g. Chen et al., Reference Chen, Haines, Charlton and VanderWeele2019). However, this is only one side of the coin. As discussed next, there is disturbing evidence that parents’ regulatory efforts can also be detrimental to their children’s well-being.

13.3.1.2 Emotional Labor and Its Implication for Parents’ Well-Being

Parental regulatory efforts may be costly for parents, just as emotional labor is costly for employees. Le and Impett’s (Reference Le and Impett2016) pioneering daily diary study found evidence that parents’ efforts to control emotional expression can have a negative impact on their emotional well-being and the parent–child relationship (see also Karnilowicz et al., Reference Karnilowicz, Waters and Mendes2019; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Karnilowicz, West and Mendes2020). Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) went a step further by demonstrating that such efforts (to satisfy emotional display rules) may be so demanding that they put parents at risk of parental burnout, although this depends on how parents make their efforts. Parents are at greater risk of burnout when they prefer surface acting (or expressive suppression; see Lin & Szczygieł, Reference Lin and Szczygieł2022) but are at lower risk when they prefer deep acting (or cognitive reappraisal; Lin et al., Reference Lin and Szczygieł2022). Either way, research has shown that when parental burnout occurs, some parents may become neglectful or violent toward their children (Brianda et al., Reference Brianda, Roskam, Gross, Franssen, Kapala, Gérard and Mikolajczak2020; Mikolajczak et al., Reference Mikolajczak, Brianda, Avalosse and Roskam2018, Reference Mikolajczak, Gross and Roskam2019), and that this maltreatment may eventually compromise children’s development (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2016). It is, therefore, plausible that well-intentioned parental regulatory efforts may have a paradoxical, undesirable effect and work against the child.

13.4 Future Research Directions

As discussed in this chapter, Lin and colleagues (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021), together with other pioneering researchers, provided convincing evidence to support their proposal to adapt the emotional labor framework identified in the work context (see Figure 13.1) to the parenting context (see Figure 13.2). Parents perceive the existence of emotional display rules and put effort into aligning their emotions with those rules through different emotional labor strategies, which ultimately have different consequences (such as different vulnerability to parental burnout). The strengths of this framework are that it provides a backbone connecting society and individuals. It specifically delineates the interplay between society’s rules for emotional expression and parents’ regulatory efforts, emotional responses, and well-being. For this framework to be useful, it should not only contribute to theoretical understanding but also provide direction for future studies. As we will see next, this backbone framework has the potential to open many future research directions in the study of parenting, just as it has contributed to organizational literature.

13.4.1 Within the Emotional Labor Framework

Most obviously, parenting researchers can examine the components of this backbone framework in detail. First, Hochschild (Reference Hochschild1983), in her conceptualization of emotional labor, emphasized the importance of culture in shaping emotional display rules. Following this reasoning, we can expect that rules on parental emotional display, such as which emotions can be expressed and which should be hidden, may differ across cultures (see Matsumoto & Hwang, Reference Matsumoto and Hwang2012). Cultural differences in emotional display rules may further influence how parents follow the rules and thus their effect on their well-being; however, future research is needed to verify and investigate this. Second, in this chapter we have followed Hochschild’s (Reference Hochschild1983) original approach, focusing on only two emotional labor strategies. In real-world parenting, parents use a broader range of emotion regulation strategies to comply with the emotional display rules (see Part II of this book), and these affect their well-being differently. In fact, researchers have already suggested that deep and surface acting are not the only ways to tune emotions to emotional display rules (see Diefendorff et al., Reference Diefendorff, Richard and Yang2008; Mikolajczak et al., Reference Mikolajczak, Tran, Brotheridge, Gross, Charmine, Neal and Wilfred2009). Pursuing the investigation of the impact of parents’ emotion regulation in their parental role and its impact on their well-being through a wider range of emotion regulation strategies will prove fruitful.

13.4.2 Beyond the Emotional Labor Framework

Researchers may also find it promising to include exogenous moderators in this backbone framework. First, there may be factors influencing parents’ propensity to choose specific emotional labor strategies, resulting in different consequences for parental well-being. In organizational literature, service employees’ personality traits have been shown to be a key variable affecting the type of emotional labor they perform (Austin et al., Reference Austin, Saklofske and Egan2005). For example, research findings indicate that individuals with high negative affectivity are more likely to use surface than deep acting (Bono & Vey, Reference Bono, Vey, Härtel, Zerbe and Ashkanasy2005; Hülsheger & Schewe, Reference Hülsheger and Schewe2011; Kammeyer-Mueller et al., Reference Kammeyer-Mueller, Rubenstein, Long, Odio, Buckman, Zhang and Halvorsen-Ganepola2013; Mesmer-Magnus et al., Reference Mesmer-Magnus, DeChurch and Wax2012). This may be because, for people who have a dispositional tendency to experience negative emotions and are therefore inclined to process information in a way that directs them towards negative affective states (Larsen & Ketelaar, Reference Larsen and Ketelaar1991), using deep acting to evoke positive emotions can be a real challenge. Thus, feigning positive emotions becomes the only way to meet their role expectations. On this basis, we can expect that negative affectivity may moderate parents’ adoption of emotional labor strategies. Lin and Szczygieł (Reference Lin and Szczygieł2022) provided preliminary evidence by showing that parents who place more emphasis on their mistakes in parenting have a higher propensity for expressive suppression.

In addition to employees’ personality traits, research on emotional labor also points to the importance of the circumstances – customer behavior such as mistreatment – in predicting the use of emotional labor strategies. These studies have found that employees who experience rudeness and mistreatment from customers find it easier and more convenient to use surface acting than deep acting and are therefore more likely to do so (Adams & Webster, Reference Adams and Webster2013; Sliter et al., Reference Sliter, Jex, Wolford and McInnerney2010; Szczygieł & Bazińska, Reference Szczygieł and Bazińska2021). In view of such evidence, we predict that in the parenting context, children’s challenging behaviors (like those of customers in the organizational context) may act as a crucial factor predisposing parents to use surface acting more often than deep acting and thus put them at risk of ill-being. In a recently published study, Zhang et al. (Reference Zhang, Gatzke-Kopp, Cole and Ram2023) demonstrated that mothers whose children exhibited more challenging behaviors experienced more negative emotions, which was associated with using multiple emotion regulation strategies. Although children’s challenging behavior was not directly significantly related to mothers’ emotion regulation strategies, the associations found between it and suppression and cognitive reappraisal support our reasoning here. Specifically, children’s challenging behavior was positively, albeit insignificantly, related to mothers’ expressive suppression and negatively, albeit insignificantly, related to cognitive reappraisal.

Finally, moderating factors may mitigate or exacerbate emotional labor’s consequences for parents’ well-being. When employees have no choice but to use emotional labor, their personality traits can mitigate the potential harm it causes. As a personality trait, emotional intelligence seems to be a promising moderator of this kind. Emotional intelligence (also known as emotional competence) refers to individuals’ ability to identify, express, understand, regulate and use their own and others’ emotions (Mayer & Salovey, Reference Mayer, Salovey, Salovey and Sluyter1997). Mikolajczak et al. (Reference Mikolajczak, Menil and Luminet2007) showed that when employees have higher emotional intelligence, their regulatory efforts to perform emotional labor are reduced, which in turn predicts a lower risk of job burnout. Based on this line of research, it can be expected that parents’ high emotional intelligence can reduce the regulatory efforts inherent to emotional labor and, consequently, its negative impact on their well-being. Lin and Szczygieł (Reference Lin and Szczygieł2022) provided initial evidence for this. They showed that parents’ emotional intelligence moderates the effects of expressive suppression on parental burnout such that the effects are reduced (although they remain significant) when parents have higher emotional intelligence.

To sum up, after our introduction to the emotional labor framework in the work context, in this section we have summarized the reasons for extending the emotional labor framework to the parenting context, described current evidence in favor of this proposal, and finally suggested a few fascinating areas that future studies may find it fruitful to explore further. Taken together, this demonstrates what a promising approach this is.

13.5 Conclusion

The goal of this chapter was to introduce Lin and colleagues’ (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) attempt to adapt emotional labor, originally intended for work, to the parenting context. As they themselves also noted, equating parenting with labor activity is controversial, as parenting does not meet the fundamental characteristic of “labor”: being financially rewarded. With this in mind, Lin and colleagues (p. 2703) pointed out that their import of emotional labor to the parenting context “does not amount to reducing parenting to a job”; rather, it should be seen as a metaphor to describe the situation faced by today’s parents, who are expected to adhere to rules regarding emotions when raising their children.

As we have seen in this chapter, Lin and colleagues’ (Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021) emotional labor framework offers a compelling theoretical lens to explain the mechanism by which desirable goals – to show more positive and demonstrate fewer negative emotions while interacting with children – can be so demanding in terms of emotional labor that they lead to poorer parent–child relationships or even severe costs such as burnout. The emotional labor framework emerged from the need to understand the service industry’s new labor form (Hochschild, Reference Hochschild1983). It has subsequently generated hundreds of studies about the cost of emotion management in service jobs. Although a handful of studies exploring emotional labor in a family context have already emerged (e.g. Wharton & Erickson, Reference Wharton and Erickson1993), there has been little investigation of parents’ regulatory efforts (the main exceptions being Le & Impett, Reference Le and Impett2016 and Lin et al., Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021). Yet these regulatory efforts probably resemble the emotional labor concept very closely in that modern parenting culture places external pressure on parents by prescribing emotional display rules (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Hansotte, Szczygieł, Meeussen, Roskam and Mikolajczak2021). In this sense, this framework contributes to a better understanding of the emotional experience of modern parents.

Footnotes

1 Deep acting refers to changing felt emotions to achieve the desired emotional display. Surface acting refers to changing emotional displays without changing internal feelings. Both concepts are discussed in detail later in this chapter.

References

Abraham, R. (1999). Emotional dissonance in organizations: Conceptualizing the roles of self‐esteem and job‐induced tension. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 20(1), 1825. https://doi.org/10.1108/01437739910251152Google Scholar
Adams, G. A., & Webster, J. R. (2013). Emotional regulation as a mediator between interpersonal mistreatment and distress. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 22(6), 697710. https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2012.698057Google Scholar
Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotional labor in service roles: The influence of identity. Academy of Management Review, 18(1), 88115. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1993.3997508Google Scholar
Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., & Egan, V. (2005). Personality, well-being and health correlates of trait emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(3), 547558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.05.009Google Scholar
Bai, S., Repetti, R. L., & Sperling, J. B. (2016). Children’s expressions of positive emotion are sustained by smiling, touching, and playing with parents and siblings: A naturalistic observational study of family life. Developmental Psychology, 52(1), 88101. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039854CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkawi, T. (2006). Globalization and war. Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Bono, J. E., & Vey, M. A. (2005). Toward understanding emotional management at work: A quantitative review of emotional labor research. In Härtel, C. E., Zerbe, W. J., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (Eds.), Emotions in organizational behavior (pp. 213233). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2015). Children’s parents. In Bornstein, M. H. & Leventhal, T. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Vol. 4. Ecological settings and processes (7th ed., pp. 55132). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Brianda, M. E., Roskam, I., Gross, J. J., Franssen, A., Kapala, F., Gérard, F., & Mikolajczak, M. (2020). Treating parental burnout: Impact of two treatment modalities on burnout symptoms, emotions, hair cortisol, and parental neglect and violence. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 330–332. https://doi.org/10.1159/000506354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brotheridge, C. M., & Grandey, A. A. (2002). Emotional labor and burnout: Comparing two perspectives of “people work.” Journal of Vocational Behavior, 60(1), 1739. https://doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.2001.1815Google Scholar
Brotheridge, C. M., & Lee, R. T. (2002). Testing a conservation of resources model of the dynamics of emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7(1), 5767. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.7.1.57Google Scholar
Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(4), 598606. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.17.4.598Google Scholar
Chen, Y., Haines, J., Charlton, B. M., & VanderWeele, T. J. (2019). Positive parenting improves multiple aspects of health and well-being in young adulthood. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(7), 684691. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0602-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (2016). Socioemotional, personality, and biological development: Illustrations from a multilevel developmental psychopathology perspective on child maltreatment. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 187211. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033259Google Scholar
Cropanzano, R., Weiss, H. M., & Elias, S. M. (2003). The impact of display rules and emotional labor on psychological well-being at work. In Perrewe, P. L. & Ganster, D. C. (Eds.), Emotional and physiological processes and positive intervention strategies (Vol. 3, pp. 4589). Emerald Group Publishing Limited. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1479-3555(03)03002-6Google Scholar
Deaton, A., & Stone, A. A. (2014). Evaluative and hedonic wellbeing among those with and without children at home. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(4), 13281333. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1311600111Google Scholar
Diefendorff, J. M., Richard, E. M., & Croyle, M. H. (2006). Are emotional display rules formal job requirements? Examination of employee and supervisor perceptions. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 79(2), 273298. https://doi.org/10.1348/096317905X68484Google Scholar
Diefendorff, J. M., Richard, E. M., & Yang, J. (2008). Linking emotion regulation strategies to affective events and negative emotions at work. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 73(3), 498508. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2008.09.006Google Scholar
Dijk, P. A. V., & Brown, A. K. (2006). Emotional labour and negative job outcomes: An evaluation of the mediating role of emotional dissonance. Journal of Management & Organization, 12(2), 101115. https://doi.org/10.5172/jmo.2006.12.2.101Google Scholar
Dix, T., Gershoff, E. T., Meunier, L. N., & Miller, P. C. (2004). The affective structure of supportive parenting: Depressive symptoms, immediate emotions, and child-oriented motivation. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 12121227. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1212Google Scholar
Douglas, S. J., & Michaels, M. W. (2004). The mommy myth: The idealization of motherhood and how it has undermined all women. Free Press.Google Scholar
Duncombe, M. E., Havighurst, S. S., Holland, K. A., & Frankling, E. J. (2012). The contribution of parenting practices and parent emotion factors in children at risk for disruptive behavior disorders. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 43(5), 715733. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-012-0290-5Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Murphy, B. C. (1996). Parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s social competence and comforting behavior. Child Development, 67(5), 22272247.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & Reiser, M. (1999). Parental reactions to children’s negative emotions: Longitudinal relations to quality of children’s social functioning. Child Development, 70(2), 513534.Google Scholar
Furedi, F. (2002). Paranoid parenting: Why ignoring the experts may be best for your child. Chicago Review Press.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1994). Living in a post-traditional society. In Beck, U., Giddens, A., & Lash, S., Reflexive modernization: Politics, tradition and aesthetics in the modern social order (pp. 56109). Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1999). Risk and responsibility. Modern Law Review, 62(1), 110.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Doubleday.Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotional regulation in the workplace: A new way to conceptualize emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5(1), 95110. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.95Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A. (2003). When “the show must go on”: Surface acting and deep acting as determinants of emotional exhaustion and peer-rated service delivery. Academy of Management Journal, 46(1), 8696. https://doi.org/10.5465/30040678Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., & Gabriel, A. S. (2015). Emotional labor at a crossroads: Where do we go from here? Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 2(1), 323349. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111400Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., & Melloy, R. C. (2017). The state of the heart: Emotional labor as emotion regulation reviewed and revised. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 22(3), 407422. https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000067Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., & Sayre, G. M. (2019). Emotional labor: Regulating emotions for a wage. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(2), 131137. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721418812771Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., Diefendorffand, J. M., & Rupp, D. E. (Eds.). (2013). Emotional labor in the 21st century: Diverse perspectives on the psychology of emotion regulation at work. Routledge.Google Scholar
Grandey, A. A., Fisk, G. M., Mattila, A. S., Jansen, K. J., & Sideman, L. A. (2005). Is “service with a smile” enough? Authenticity of positive displays during service encounters. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 96(1), 3855. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.08.002Google Scholar
Habib, I. (1995). Capitalism in history. Social Scientist, 23(7–9), 1531. https://doi.org/10.2307/3517858Google Scholar
Hajal, N. J., & Paley, B. (2020). Parental emotion and emotion regulation: A critical target of study for research and intervention to promote child emotion socialization. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 403417. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000864Google Scholar
Hartel, C. E. J., Hsu, A. C. F., & Boyle, M. V. (2002). A conceptual examination of the causal sequences of emotional labor, emotional dissonance, and emotional exhaustion: The argument for the role of contextual and provider characteristics. In Ashkanasy, N. M., Zerbe, W. J., & Hartel, C. E. J. (Eds.), Managing emotions in the workplace (pp. 251275). M. E. Sharpe.Google Scholar
Hays, S. (1996). The cultural contradictions of motherhood. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Hendrick, H. (2016). Narcissistic parenting in an insecure world: A history of parenting culture 1920s to present. Policy Press. https://doi.org/10.1332/policypress/9781447322559.001.0001Google Scholar
Hobsbawm, E. (1995). The age of extremes: A history of the world, 1914–1991. Abacus.Google Scholar
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Hoghughi, M. (2004). Parenting – an introduction. In Hoghughi, M. & Long, N. (Eds.), Handbook of parenting: Theory and research for practice (pp. 118). Sage.Google Scholar
Holman, D., Martinez-Iñigo, D., & Totterdell, P. (2008). Emotional labour and employee well-being: An integrative review. In Ashkanasy, N. M. & Cooper, C. L. (Eds.), Research companion to emotion in organizations (pp. 315340). Edward Elgar Publishing. https://www.elgaronline.com/view/9781845426378.00029.xmlGoogle Scholar
Hülsheger, U. R., & Schewe, A. F. (2011). On the costs and benefits of emotional labor: A meta-analysis of three decades of research. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 16(3), 361389. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022876Google Scholar
Huppertz, A. V., Ute, R. H., Velozo, J. D. C., & Schreurs, B. H. (2020). Why do emotional labor strategies differentially predict exhaustion?: Comparing psychological effort, authenticity, and relational mechanisms. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 25(3), 214226. https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000179Google Scholar
Jansz, J., & Timmers, M. (2002). Emotional dissonance: When the experience of an emotion jeopardizes an individual’s identity. Theory & Psychology, 12(1), 7995. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354302121005Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306(5702), 17761780. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103572Google Scholar
Kammeyer-Mueller, J. D., Rubenstein, A. L., Long, D. M., Odio, M. A., Buckman, B. R., Zhang, Y., & Halvorsen-Ganepola, M. D. K. (2013). A Meta-analytic structural model of dispositional affectivity and emotional labor. Personnel Psychology, 66(1), 4790. https://doi.org/10.1111/peps.12009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karnilowicz, H. R., Waters, S. F., & Mendes, W. B. (2019). Not in front of the kids: Effects of parental suppression on socialization behaviors during cooperative parent–child interactions. Emotion, 19(7), 11831191. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000527Google Scholar
Kerr, M. L., Rasmussen, H. F., Buttitta, K. V., Smiley, P. A., & Borelli, J. L. (2021). Exploring the complexity of mothers’ real-time emotions while caregiving. Emotion, 21(3), 545556. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000719CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kettenacker, L., & Riotte, T. (2011). The legacies of two world wars: European societies in the twentieth century. Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
Korczynski, M. (2005). Skills in service work: An overview. Human Resource Management Journal, 15(2), 314.Google Scholar
Kruml, S. M., & Geddes, D. (2000). Exploring the dimensions of emotional labor: The heart of Hochschild’s work. Management Communication Quarterly, 14(1), 849. https://doi.org/10.1177/0893318900141002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1), 132140. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.1.132Google Scholar
Le, B. M., & Impett, E. A. (2016). The costs of suppressing negative emotions and amplifying positive emotions during parental caregiving. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(3), 323336. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216629122Google Scholar
Lee, E. (2014a). Experts and parenting culture. In Lee, E., Bristow, J., Faircloth, C., & Macvarish, J. (Eds.), Parenting culture studies (pp. 5175). Palgrave Macmillan UK. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137304612_3Google Scholar
Lee, E. (2014b). Introduction. In Lee, E., Bristow, J., Faircloth, C., & Macvarish, J. (Eds.), Parenting culture studies (pp. 122). Palgrave Macmillan UK. https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137304612_1Google Scholar
Lin, G.-X., & Szczygieł, D. (2022). Perfectionistic parents are burnt out by hiding emotions from their children, but this effect is attenuated by emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 184, 111187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111187Google Scholar
Lin, G.-X., Goldenberg, A., Arikan, G., Brytek-Matera, A., Czepczor-Bernat, K., Manrique-Millones, D., Mikolajczak, M., Overbye, H., Roskam, I., Szczygieł, D., Ustundag-Budak, A. M., & Gross, J. J. (2022). Reappraisal, social support, and parental burnout. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(4), 10891102. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12380Google Scholar
Lin, G.-X., Hansotte, L., Szczygieł, D., Meeussen, L., Roskam, I., & Mikolajczak, M. (2021). Parenting with a smile: Display rules, regulatory effort, and parental burnout. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 38(9), 27012721. https://doi.org/10.1177/02654075211019124Google Scholar
Lin, G.-X., Mikolajczak, M., Keller, H., Akgun, E., Arikan, G., Aunola, K., Barham, E., Besson, E., Blanchard, M. A., Boujut, E., Brianda, M. E., Brytek-Matera, A., César, F., Chen, B.-B., Dorard, G., dos Santos Elias, L. C., Dunsmuir, S., Egorova, N., Escobar, M. J., … Roskam, I. (2023). Parenting culture(s): Ideal-parent beliefs across 37 countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 54(1), 424. https://doi.org/10.1177/00220221221123043Google Scholar
Luong, A. (2005). Affective service display and customer mood. Journal of Service Research, 8(2), 117130. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094670505279341Google Scholar
Manassis, K., Bradley, S., Goldberg, S., Hood, J., & Swinson, R. P. (1994). Attachment in mothers with anxiety disorders and their children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(8), 11061113. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199410000-00006Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. S. (2012). Culture and emotion: The integration of biological and cultural contributions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(1), 91118. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111420147Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In Salovey, P. & Sluyter, D. J. (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (pp. 331). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., DeChurch, L. A., & Wax, A. (2012). Moving emotional labor beyond surface and deep acting: A discordance–congruence perspective. Organizational Psychology Review, 2(1), 653. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041386611417746Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Brianda, M. E., Avalosse, H., & Roskam, I. (2018). Consequences of parental burnout: Its specific effect on child neglect and violence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 80, 134145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.03.025Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Gross, J. J., & Roskam, I. (2019). Parental burnout: What is it and why does it matter? Clinical Psychological Science, 7(6), 13191329.Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Menil, C., & Luminet, O. (2007). Explaining the protective effect of trait emotional intelligence regarding occupational stress: Exploration of emotional labour processes. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(5), 11071117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.01.003Google Scholar
Mikolajczak, M., Tran, V., Brotheridge, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Chapter 11: Using an emotion regulation framework to predict the outcomes of emotional labor. In Charmine, E. J. H, Neal, M. A, & Wilfred, J. Z (Eds.), Emotions in groups, organizations and cultures (Vol. 5, pp. 245273). Emerald Group Publishing Limited. https://doi.org/10.1108/S1746-9791(2009)0000005013Google Scholar
Minnotte, K. L., Pedersen, D. E., Mannon, S. E., & Kiger, G. (2010). Tending to the emotions of children: Predicting parental performance of emotion work with children. Marriage & Family Review, 46(3), 224241. https://doi.org/10.1080/01494929.2010.490199Google Scholar
Morris, A. S., Criss, M. M., Silk, J. S., & Houltberg, B. J. (2017). The impact of parenting on emotion regulation during childhood and adolescence. Child Development Perspectives, 11(4), 233238. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12238Google Scholar
Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. (1996). The dimensions, antecedents, and consequences of emotional labor. Academy of Management Review, 21(4), 9861010. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1996.9704071861Google Scholar
Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. (1997). Managing emotions in the workplace. Journal of Managerial Issues, 9(3), 257274.Google Scholar
National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities. (2021). Positive parenting tips. National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/childdevelopment/positiveparenting/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Nomaguchi, K., & Milkie, M. A. (2020). Parenthood and well-being: A decade in review. Journal of Marriage and Family, 82(1), 198223. https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12646Google Scholar
Olszanowski, M., Wróbel, M., & Hess, U. (2020). Mimicking and sharing emotions: A re-examination of the link between facial mimicry and emotional contagion. Cognition and Emotion, 34(2), 367376. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1611543Google Scholar
Overy, R. (2009). The morbid age: Britain and the crisis of civilisation, 1919–1939. Penguin UK.Google Scholar
Pugh, S. D. (2001). Service with a smile: Emotional contagion in the service encounter. Academy of Management Journal, 44(5), 10181027. https://doi.org/10.5465/3069445Google Scholar
Pursell, C. (2007). Technology in postwar America: A history. Columbia University Press. https://doi.org/10.7312/purs12304Google Scholar
Rafaeli, A., & Sutton, R. I. (1990). Busy stores and demanding customers: How do they affect the display of positive emotion? Academy of Management Journal, 33(3), 623637. https://doi.org/10.5465/256584Google Scholar
Rodrigo, M. J. (2010). Promoting positive parenting in Europe: New challenges for the European society for developmental psychology. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(3), 281294. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405621003780200Google Scholar
Rubin, R. S., Tardino, V. M. S., Daus, C. S., & Munz, D. C. (2005). A reconceptualization of the emotional labor construct: On the development of an integrated theory of perceived emotional dissonance and emotional labor. In Hartel, C. E. J., Zerbe, W. J., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (Eds.), Emotions in organizational behavior (pp. 189211). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rueger, S. Y., Katz, R. L., Risser, H. J., & Lovejoy, M. C. (2011). Relations between parental affect and parenting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Parenting, 11(1), 133. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2011.539503Google Scholar
Schaubroeck, J., & Jones, J. R. (2000). Antecedents of workplace emotional labor dimensions and moderators of their effects on physical symptoms. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21(2), 163183. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2<163::AID-JOB37>3.0.CO;2-LGoogle Scholar
Scott, B. A., & Barnes, C. M. (2011). A multilevel field investigation of emotional labor, affect, work withdrawal, and gender. Academy of Management Journal, 54(1), 116136. https://doi.org/10.5465/amj.2011.59215086Google Scholar
Sliter, M., Jex, S., Wolford, K., & McInnerney, J. (2010). How rude! Emotional labor as a mediator between customer incivility and employee outcomes. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15(4), 468481. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020723Google Scholar
Sutton, R. I., & Rafaeli, A. (1988). Untangling the relationship between displayed emotions and organizational sales: The case of convenience stores. Academy of Management Journal, 31(3), 461487. https://doi.org/10.5465/256456Google Scholar
Szczygieł, D., & Bazińska, R. (2021). Emotional intelligence mitigates the effects of customer incivility on surface acting and exhaustion in service occupations: A moderated mediation model. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.506085Google Scholar
Thoits, P. A. (2004). Emotion norms, emotion work, and social order. In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N., & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam Symposium (pp. 359378). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Totterdell, P., & Holman, D. (2003). Emotion regulation in customer service roles: Testing a model of emotional labor. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8(1), 5573. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.8.1.55Google Scholar
Tsai, W.-C. (2001). Determinants and consequences of employee displayed positive emotions. Journal of Management, 27(4), 497512. https://doi.org/10.1177/014920630102700406Google Scholar
United Nations General Assembly. (1948). Universal declaration of human rights. http://www.ohchr.org/EN/UDHR/Documents/UDHR_Translations/eng.pdfGoogle Scholar
von Scheve, C. (2012). Emotion regulation and emotion work: Two sides of the same coin? Frontiers in Psychology, 3. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00496Google Scholar
Waters, S. F., Karnilowicz, H. R., West, T. V., & Mendes, W. B. (2020). Keep it to yourself? Parent emotion suppression influences physiological linkage and interaction behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 34(7), 784793. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000664CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wharton, A. S., & Erickson, R. J. (1993). Managing emotions on the job and at home: Understanding the consequences of multiple emotional roles. The Academy of Management Review, 18(3), 457486. https://doi.org/10.2307/258905Google Scholar
Wharton, A. S., & Erickson, R. J. (1995). The consequences of caring: Exploring the links between women’s job and family emotion work. The Sociological Quarterly, 36(2), 273296.Google Scholar
Zapf, D., & Holz, M. (2006). On the positive and negative effects of emotion work in organizations. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 15(1), 128. https://doi.org/10.1080/13594320500412199Google Scholar
Zerbe, W. J. (2000). Emotional dissonance and employee well-being. In Emotions in the workplace: Research, theory, and practice (pp. 189214). Quorum Books/Greenwood Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Zhang, X., Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Cole, P. M., & Ram, N. (2023). The role of strategy-use and parasympathetic functioning in maternal emotion regulation. Journal of Family Psychology, 37(1), 110120. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0001017Google Scholar
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Rudolph, J., Kerin, J., & Bohadana-Brown, G. (2022). Parent emotional regulation: A meta-analytic review of its association with parenting and child adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 46(1), 6382. https://doi.org/10.1177/01650254211051086Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 13.1 Emotional labor framework in the job context

Figure 1

Figure 13.2 Emotional labor framework in the parenting context

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×